We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings.
To save content items to your account,
please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies.
If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account.
Find out more about saving content to .
To save content items to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected]
is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings
on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part
of your Kindle email address below.
Find out more about saving to your Kindle.
Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations.
‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi.
‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.
Sensory science began with the Greek philosophers (back to 600 BC). One early view: sensory experience is a faithful reflection of the physical world. As history swept through the Roman Empire, the fall of Rome and the transfer of science to Arabic scholars, sensory science as we know it had to await Andreas Vesalius and human dissections in the sixteenth century. Luigi Galvani’s animal electricity (eighteenth century) ushered in the scientific explosion of the nineteenth century that brought us Johannes Müller (doctrine of specific nerve energies) and the birth of psychology in the laboratory of Wilhelm Wundt (1879). Advances rapidly showed how information from each of our senses is processed through the nervous system; contrary to the Greeks, our perceptions are not always a faithful reflection of the physical world. Just as with physical science, sensory science requires measurement. Gustav Fechner argued that the unit of sensation is the jnd (the just-noticeable difference a stimulus must be increased for a person to detect a difference). This view held for a century until S. S. Stevens noted all jnds did not appear to be equal. He revolutionized the study of sensory and perceptual experience, and that revolution continues today.
Recommend this
Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this to your organisation's collection.