Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-g8jcs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-30T20:27:53.619Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Are we all implicit puritans? New evidence that work and sex are intuitively moralized in both traditional and non-traditional cultures

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 October 2023

Warren Tierney
Affiliation:
Organisational Behaviour Area/Marketing Area, INSEAD, Singapore, Singapore [email protected] [email protected] [email protected]
Wilson Cyrus-Lai
Affiliation:
Organisational Behaviour Area/Marketing Area, INSEAD, Singapore, Singapore [email protected] [email protected] [email protected]
Eric Luis Uhlmann
Affiliation:
Organisational Behaviour Area/Marketing Area, INSEAD, Singapore, Singapore [email protected] [email protected] [email protected]

Abstract

Contradicting our earlier claims of American moral exceptionalism, recent self-replication evidence from our laboratory indicates that implicit puritanism characterizes the judgments of people across cultures. Implicit cultural evolution may lag behind explicit change, such that differences between traditional and non-traditional cultures are greater at a deliberative than an intuitive level. Not too deep down, perhaps we are all implicit puritans.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Is puritanism steeped in the cultural and religious histories of specific groups of people, or a general characteristic of human moral cognition? Some years ago, drawing on research on automatic and unconscious mental processes (Bargh, Reference Bargh and Wyer1997; Greenwald & Banaji, Reference Greenwald and Banaji1995), as well as cross-disciplinary scholarship on American exceptionalism (Baker, Reference Baker2005; de Tocqueville, Reference de Tocqueville1840/1990; Landes, Reference Landes1998; Lipset, Reference Lipset1996), we proposed a theory of implicit puritanism in American moral cognition (Poehlman, Reference Poehlman2007; Uhlmann, Reference Uhlmann2012; Uhlmann, Poehlman, & Bargh, Reference Uhlmann, Poehlman, Bargh, Sorrentino and Yamaguchi2008, Reference Uhlmann, Poehlman, Bargh, Jost, Kay and Thorisdottir2009; Uhlmann, Poehlman, Tannenbaum, & Bargh, Reference Uhlmann, Poehlman, Tannenbaum and Bargh2011). We posited that because of a unique history of religious migration and settlement, contemporary Americans harbor automatic and intuitive responses that reflect traditional Protestant–Puritan mores. As a result, Americans, more so than members of comparison cultures, intuitively valorize working in the absence of material need – for example, perceiving a lottery winner who continues to work in a low-paying job as having outstanding moral character. In experimental laboratory settings, Americans further exhibit responses to sexual promiscuity on implicit and indirect measures more negative than their explicit, carefully considered judgments.

The moral disciplining account proposed by Fitouchi et al. directly challenges such culture-specific accounts, arguing that puritanism stems from universal moral concerns such as identifying quality cooperation partners and avoiding defectors. As they acknowledge, western, educated, industrialized, rich, and democratic (WEIRD; Henrich, Heine, & Norenzayan, Reference Henrich, Heine and Norenzayan2010) societies are often markedly less puritanical than non-WEIRD societies. However, this occurred slowly over time as cooperation concerns faded considering the increasing economic prosperity and individual-level human capital in WEIRD nations. As social cooperation became less objectively necessary for individual survival and goal pursuit, such cultures turned away from puritanical moral values. The moral disciplining account thus predicts that today's Americans ought to be less puritanical than members of less privileged societies where individuals must constantly depend on friends, neighbors, and community members for assistance.

The initial experimental investigations of implicit puritanism were conducted prior to the wave of methodological reforms in the field of psychology starting in 2011 (Nelson, Simmons, & Simonsohn, Reference Nelson, Simmons and Simonsohn2018; Simmons, Nelson, & Simonsohn, Reference Simmons, Nelson and Simonsohn2011). The studies in question relied on small samples, and the analyses were not pre-registered in advance, thus increasing statistical noise and researcher degrees of freedom to potentially dangerous levels. As a result, either the effects themselves (intuitive moralization of needless work, implicit puritanical tendencies with regard to sex) or cultural differences in such phenomena could represent false positives.

In a recent self-replication initiative, we revisited the key experimental evidence regarding implicit puritanism leveraging large-scale multi-national data collections as well as pre-registration of analyses and theoretical predictions (Tierney et al., Reference Tierney, Hardy, Ebersole, Leavitt, Viganola, Clemente and Uhlmann2020, Reference Tierney, Hardy, Ebersole, Viganola, Clemente, Gordon and Uhlmann2021). Introducing the “creative destruction” approach to replication, we competed the original implicit puritanism account claiming American moral exceptionalism with a half dozen alternative theories of culture and morality. The winning theory was the general moralization of work, which posits that implicit puritanism characterizes the judgments of people across cultures and is not uniquely American at all. Although very surprising to us at the time, this outcome is consistent with Fitouchi et al.'s moral disciplining account, in which puritanical judgments are caused by general social concerns such as detecting reliable versus unreliable cooperation partners. Further attesting to such generalizability is a recent set of conceptual replications of the needless work effect designed by 13 independent research teams (Landy et al., Reference Landy, Jia, Ding, Viganola, Tierney, Dreber and Uhlmann2020; see also Celniker et al., Reference Celniker, Gregory, Koo, Piff, Ditto and Shariff2023).

At the same time, another outcome from the self-replication initiative suggests a major theoretical modification of the moral disciplining framework. In one of the initial demonstrations of implicit puritanism, American participants were asked for either their rational and deliberative judgment or their intuitive gut reaction to a description of a target person (a previously established mindset manipulation; Epstein, Lipson, Holstein, & Huh, Reference Epstein, Lipson, Holstein and Huh1992). In the experimental scenario, a lottery winner either retired or continued to work peeling potatoes in a restaurant kitchen despite now being a multi-millionaire. When asked for their intuitive judgment, American participants were significantly more likely to perceive needless work as reflecting good moral character than when functioning in a deliberative mindset (Poehlman, Reference Poehlman2007; Uhlmann, Poehlman, & Bargh, Reference Uhlmann, Poehlman, Bargh, Jost, Kay and Thorisdottir2009). Tierney et al.'s (Reference Tierney, Hardy, Ebersole, Leavitt, Viganola, Clemente and Uhlmann2021) attempted replications recruited more than 50 times as many participants as the original investigation and spanned four nations and continents (India, Australia, the United States, and the United Kingdom). Disproving the notion that implicit puritanism is a uniquely American phenomenon, participants from the United States, the United Kingdom, and Australia moralized work more intuitively than deliberatively. Although the manipulation of the intuitive-deliberative response had no effect on the moral character judgments of Indian participants, an exploratory internal analysis yielded a fascinating pattern of results. Specifically, differences between the traditional (India) and non-traditional (United States, United Kingdom, Australia) cultures were greater at a deliberative than an intuitive level. In other words, Indian participants exhibited no effect of the mindset manipulation because both their intuitive and reasoned responses to needless work were puritanical.

This points to a potential dual-process account of cultural change and stability in puritan morality. As Fitouchi et al. highlight, WEIRD societies have become less traditional regarding work, sex, and related issues over the years, which they attribute to the steadily diminishing need for social cooperation in such nations. Our self-replication findings (Tierney et al., Reference Tierney, Hardy, Ebersole, Viganola, Clemente, Gordon and Uhlmann2021) suggest that similar to the persistence of many social stereotypes (Charlesworth & Banaji, Reference Charlesworth and Banaji2022; Charlesworth, Yang, Mann, Kurdi, & Banaji, Reference Charlesworth, Yang, Mann, Kurdi and Banaji2021), implicit cultural evolution may lag behind explicit change. As a result, even members of non-traditional cultures who deliberatively endorse a narrow harm-based morality (Graham et al., Reference Graham, Haidt, Koleva, Motyl, Iyer, Wojcik and Ditto2013) may exhibit implicit puritanism when in an intuitive mindset, cognitively depleted, or in their responses on implicit and indirect measures. These are currently only speculations based on a comparison of just four nations, and confirmatory tests sampling more non-traditional and especially traditional cultures are needed prior to drawing strong conclusions. Although it remains to be seen if the “implicit lag” hypothesis receives broad empirical support, Fitouchi et al. may be even more right than they thought: Puritanism could be universally human, albeit implicitly for some cultures and individuals.

Financial support

This research was supported by an R&D grant from INSEAD.

Competing interest

None.

References

Baker, W. (2005). America's crisis of values. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Bargh, J. A. (1997). The automaticity of everyday life. In Wyer, R. S. Jr. (Ed.), Advances in social cognition, Vol. 10. The automaticity of everyday life: Advances in social cognition (Vol. 10, pp. 161). Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Celniker, J. B., Gregory, A., Koo, H. J., Piff, P. K., Ditto, P. H., & Shariff, A. F. (2023). The moralization of effort. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 152(1), 6079. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001259CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charlesworth, T. E., & Banaji, M. R. (2022). Patterns of implicit and explicit stereotypes III: Long-term change in gender stereotypes. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 13(1), 1426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charlesworth, T. E., Yang, V., Mann, T. C., Kurdi, B., & Banaji, M. R. (2021). Gender stereotypes in natural language: Word embeddings show robust consistency across child and adult language corpora of more than 65 million words. Psychological Science, 32(2), 218240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Tocqueville, A. (1840/1990). Democracy in America. Vintage Books.Google Scholar
Epstein, S., Lipson, A., Holstein, C., & Huh, E. (1992). Irrational reactions to negative outcomes: Evidence for two conceptual systems. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 328339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graham, J., Haidt, J., Koleva, S., Motyl, M., Iyer, R., Wojcik, S. P., & Ditto, P. H. (2013). Chapter 2: Moral foundations theory: The pragmatic validity of moral pluralism. In P. Devine & A. Plant (Eds.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 47, pp. 55130). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102, 427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(61–83), 111135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landes, D. S. (1998). The wealth and poverty of nations: Why some are so rich and some so poor. Norton.Google Scholar
Landy, J. F., Jia, M., Ding, I. L., Viganola, D., Tierney, W., Dreber, A., … Uhlmann, E. L. (2020). Crowdsourcing hypothesis tests: Making transparent how design choices shape research results. Psychological Bulletin, 146, 451479.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lipset, S. M. (1996). American exceptionalism: A double edged sword. Norton.Google Scholar
Nelson, L., Simmons, J., & Simonsohn, U. (2018). Psychology's renaissance. Annual Review of Psychology, 69, 511534.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poehlman, T. A. (2007). Ideological inheritance: Implicit puritanism in American moral cognition. Doctoral dissertation, Yale University.Google Scholar
Simmons, J. P., Nelson, L. D., & Simonsohn, U. (2011). False-positive psychology: Undisclosed flexibility in data collection and analysis allows presenting anything as significant. Psychological Science, 22, 13591366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tierney, W., Hardy, J. H. III, Ebersole, C. R., Leavitt, K., Viganola, D., Clemente, E., … Uhlmann, E. (2020). Creative destruction in science. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 161, 291309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tierney, W., Hardy, J. H. III, Ebersole, C. R., Viganola, D., Clemente, E. G., Gordon, M., … Uhlmann, E. L. (2021). A creative destruction approach to replication: Implicit work and sex morality across cultures. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 93, 104060.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uhlmann, E. L. (2012). American psychological isolationism. Review of General Psychology, 16, 381390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uhlmann, E. L., Poehlman, T. A., & Bargh, J. A. (2008). Implicit theism. In Sorrentino, R. & Yamaguchi, S. (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition across cultures (pp. 7194). Elsevier/Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uhlmann, E. L., Poehlman, T. A., & Bargh, J. A. (2009). American moral exceptionalism. In Jost, J. T., Kay, A. C., & Thorisdottir, H. (Eds.), Social and psychological bases of ideology and system justification (pp. 2752). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uhlmann, E. L., Poehlman, T. A., Tannenbaum, D., & Bargh, J. A. (2011). Implicit puritanism in American moral cognition. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 312320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar