Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dsjbd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-30T17:04:18.974Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Individual-level solutions may support system-level change â if they are internalized as part of one's social identity

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 August 2023

Lina Koppel
Affiliation:
Department of Management and Engineering, Division of Economics, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden [email protected] Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY, USA [email protected] [email protected]; www.alijaveed.com [email protected]; https://stevenrathje.com/ [email protected]; https://www.mvlasceanu.com/
Claire E. Robertson
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY, USA [email protected] [email protected]; www.alijaveed.com [email protected]; https://stevenrathje.com/ [email protected]; https://www.mvlasceanu.com/
Kimberly C. Doell
Affiliation:
Department of Cognition, Emotion, and Methods in Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria [email protected]; http://www.kimdoell.com
Ali M. Javeed
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY, USA [email protected] [email protected]; www.alijaveed.com [email protected]; https://stevenrathje.com/ [email protected]; https://www.mvlasceanu.com/
Jesper Rasmussen
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark [email protected]; https://jesper-rasmussen.github.io/
Steve Rathje
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY, USA [email protected] [email protected]; www.alijaveed.com [email protected]; https://stevenrathje.com/ [email protected]; https://www.mvlasceanu.com/
Madalina Vlasceanu
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY, USA [email protected] [email protected]; www.alijaveed.com [email protected]; https://stevenrathje.com/ [email protected]; https://www.mvlasceanu.com/
Jay J. Van Bavel
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY, USA [email protected] [email protected]; www.alijaveed.com [email protected]; https://stevenrathje.com/ [email protected]; https://www.mvlasceanu.com/ Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, NY, USA [email protected]; www.jayvanbavel.com Department of Strategy and Management, Norwegian School of Economics, Bergen, Norway

Abstract

System-level change is crucial for solving society's most pressing problems. However, individual-level interventions may be useful for creating behavioral change before system-level change is in place and for increasing necessary public support for system-level solutions. Participating in individual-level solutions may increase support for system-level solutions – especially if the individual-level solutions are internalized as part of one's social identity.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

In order to address society's most pressing problems, change is needed at the system level. Chater & Loewenstein (C&L) argue that behavioral scientists should focus on research that informs such system-level solutions (e.g., carbon taxes to reduce greenhouse-gas emissions) rather than promoting individual-level solutions (e.g., carbon footprint calculators), as the latter are likely insufficient, less impactful, and ultimately undermine system-level approaches. Specifically, they claim that individual-level solutions generally produce negative spillover effects that reduce public support for system-level solutions. This is an important paper and the authors do an excellent job of highlighting the potential risks of individual-focused intervention. Unfortunately, many of their concerns about negative spillover are speculative and not yet backed by scientific evidence. Although we agree that system-level change is crucial for solving many of society's problems and that behavioral science can (and should) be used to inform such change, we are not convinced that individual-level solutions necessarily undercut system-level solutions.

In this commentary, we argue that negative spillover is not inevitable or even common. Moreover, social identity may be key to generating positive rather than negative spillover effects between individual-level interventions and system-level solutions. As such, individual-level change is often beneficial for achieving system-level change rather than undercutting it.

Prior research on behavioral spillover effects paints a complex picture. Although several studies have provided evidence for negative spillover (see target article), many other studies have also provided evidence for positive spillover effects (see Truelove, Carrico, Weber, Raimi, & Vandenbergh, Reference Truelove, Carrico, Weber, Raimi and Vandenbergh2014). For example, increasing individual-level proenvironmental behavior such as recycling or conscious consumption is associated with increased political activism and support for system-level solutions such as wind power (Thøgersen & Noblet, Reference Thøgersen and Noblet2012; Willis & Schor, Reference Willis and Schor2012). This may suggest that individual-level interventions help build public support that is necessary for system-level policy change – through positive spillover.

Studies on spillover effects have used a variety of methodologies and measures, producing contradictory results (e.g., Carrico, Reference Carrico2021). For example, a meta-analysis of 77 effects from studies of behavioral interventions to promote proenvironmental behavior found an overall positive spillover effect on behavioral intentions, a small negative effect on actual behavior, and no effect on policy support (Maki et al., Reference Maki, Carrico, Raimi, Truelove, Araujo and Yeung2019). Importantly, the direction and magnitude of spillover effects also varied across interventions, suggesting that there may be ways to increase positive spillover by using specific types of interventions or targeting specific types of behaviors or processes.

Social identity has been proposed as a key moderator of spillover effects in proenvironmental behavior (Truelove et al., Reference Truelove, Carrico, Weber, Raimi and Vandenbergh2014). Specifically, when a decision to act proenvironmentally is based on a social role or identity (e.g., the identity of an environmentalist) or when initial proenvironmental behavior is attributed internally (e.g., to one's identity as an environmentalist), positive spillover (vs. negative or no spillover) is more likely to occur (Truelove et al., Reference Truelove, Carrico, Weber, Raimi and Vandenbergh2014). People who reflected on proenvironmental behaviors in connection to their values or identity (relative to no reflection or identity irrelevant reflection) increased their support for a carbon tax (Sparkman, Attari, & Weber, Reference Sparkman, Attari and Weber2021). Furthermore, people who were reminded of their previous performance of a range of proenvironmental behaviors were more likely to make “green” product decisions because of an increase in environmental identity (Van der Werff, Steg, & Keizer, Reference Van der Werff, Steg and Keizer2013). Thus, when one's social identity as someone who cares about the environment is triggered or made salient, positive spillover is more likely to occur.

A similar phenomenon has been observed during the coronavirus disease-2019 (COVID-19) pandemic. A global study of 67 nations (with nearly 50,000 participants) during the pandemic found that people who supported individual-level behavior change, including reducing social gatherings, were far more likely to support system-level policies, like reducing social crowds (d > 0.8; Van Bavel et al., Reference Van Bavel, Cichocka, Capraro, Sjåstad, Nezlek and Pavlović2022). In addition, national identification predicted engagement in and support for both individual- and system-level solutions, which suggests that people who cared more about protecting their social group/country were most likely to act to reduce the spread of COVID-19. Hence, this global dataset supports the idea that there may be an indirect path for individual-level interventions to increase support for system-level interventions when people are identified with a group or issue.

Finally, support for individual-level interventions may not necessarily crowd out support for system-level changes. C&L describe a crowding-out effect; when easy-to-achieve nudges (an individual-level intervention) were presented alongside system-level policies, people supported the easier individual-level option more (Hagmann, Ho, & Loewenstein, Reference Hagmann, Ho and Loewenstein2019). However, when the small impact of nudges and the low cost of the policies were highlighted, the crowding-out effect was eliminated without diminishing support for the nudge. Although C&L conclude that individual-frame solutions crowd out system-frame solutions, the research they cite shows that simply highlighting the realistic potential efficacy of behavioral nudges can reduce negative spillover. Thus, it seems the effects of spillover can be easily mitigated with accurate and effective communication.

We enthusiastically share the view that individual-level interventions should not replace efforts for system-level policy. However, our review of the literature indicates there are situations where individual-level interventions can have positive spillover effects that benefit (or at least do not harm) system-level change. System-level change takes time and – at least in democratic societies – requires public support. Individual-level solutions could help mitigate social problems before system-level change is in place, generate support among leaders and key stakeholders, and help generate the necessary public support for system-level reform (via positive spillover effects), especially if the individual-level solutions are internalized as part of one's identity. Considering social identity as key to generating positive spillover effects may help make sense of existing literature and provide testable predictions for future investigations.

Competing interest

None.

Footnotes

*

Shared first authorship.

References

Carrico, A. R. (2021). Climate change, behavior, and the possibility of spillover effects: Recent advances and future directions. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 42, 7682. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.03.025CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hagmann, D., Ho, E. H., & Loewenstein, G. (2019). Nudging out support for a carbon tax. Nature Climate Change, 9(6), 484489. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41558-019-0474-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maki, A., Carrico, A. R., Raimi, K. T., Truelove, H. B., Araujo, B., & Yeung, K. L. (2019). Meta-analysis of pro-environmental behaviour spillover. Nature Sustainability, 2(4), 307315. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41893-019-0263-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sparkman, G., Attari, S. Z., & Weber, E. U. (2021). Moderating spillover: Focusing on personal sustainable behavior rarely hinders and can boost climate policy support. Energy Research & Social Science, 78, 102150. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.erss.2021.102150CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thøgersen, J., & Noblet, C. (2012). Does green consumerism increase the acceptance of wind power? Energy Policy, 51, 854862. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.enpol.2012.09.044.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Truelove, H. B., Carrico, A. R., Weber, E. U., Raimi, K. T., & Vandenbergh, M. P. (2014). Positive and negative spillover of pro-environmental behavior: An integrative review and theoretical framework. Global Environmental Change, 29, 127138. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2014.09.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Bavel, J. J., Cichocka, A., Capraro, V., Sjåstad, H., Nezlek, J. B., Pavlović, T., … Boggio, P. S. (2022). National identity predicts public health support during a global pandemic. Nature Communications, 13, 517. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-021-27668-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van der Werff, E., Steg, L., & Keizer, K. (2013). I am what I am, by looking past the present: The influence of biospheric values and past behavior on environmental self-identity. Environment and Behavior, 46, 132. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916512475209Google Scholar
Willis, M. M., & Schor, J. B. (2012). Does changing a light bulb lead to changing the world? Political action and the conscious consumer. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 644(1), 160190. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002716212454831CrossRefGoogle Scholar