Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t8hqh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-29T08:18:51.010Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cognition in pregnancy and motherhood: prospective cohortstudy

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Helen Christensen*
Affiliation:
Centre for Mental Health Research, The Australian National University, Canberra
Liana S. Leach
Affiliation:
Centre for Mental Health Research, The Australian National University, Canberra
Andrew Mackinnon
Affiliation:
ORYGEN Research Centre, The University of Melbourne, Australia
*
Helen Christensen, Centre for Mental Health Research, TheAustralian National University, Canberra ACT 0200, Australia. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Research has reported that pregnant women and mothers become forgetful. However, in these studies, women are not recruited prior to pregnancy, samples are not representative and studies are underpowered.

Aims

The current study sought to determine whether pregnancy and motherhood are associated with brief or long-term cognitive deterioration using a representative sample and measuring cognition during and before the onset of pregnancy and motherhood.

Method

Women aged 20–24 years were recruited prospectively and assessed in 1999, 2003 and 2007. Seventy-six women were pregnant at follow-up assessments, 188 became mothers between study waves and 542 remained nulliparous.

Results

No significant differences in cognitive change were found as a function of pregnancy or motherhood, although late pregnancy was associated with deterioration on one of four tests of memory and cognition.

Conclusions

The hypothesis that pregnancy and motherhood are associated with persistent cognitive deterioration was not supported. Previous negative findings may be a result of biased sampling.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2010 

Many pregnancy guidebooks, the popular press and websites counsel pregnant women on the possibility of short-term memory problems during pregnancy, describing the condition as ‘baby brain’ or ‘placenta brain’. Reference Crystal13 These views are supported by scientific research evidence and systematic reviews. Reference Henry and Rendell4Reference Casey20 For example, a recent systematic review found that pregnant women perform more poorly than non-pregnant women on memory and other cognitive tests. Reference Henry and Rendell4 Similar deficits are reported in motherhood in cross-sectional Reference Swain, O'Hara, Starr and Gorman19 and longitudinal studies Reference de Groot, Vuurman, Hornstra and Jolles13,Reference Harris, Deary, Harris, Lees and Wilson18 (but see Crawley et al). Reference Crawley, Dennison and Carter17 However, the animal-model literature paints a radically different picture of the effects of pregnancy and motherhood on cognitive capabilities. Kinsley et al Reference Kinsley, Madonia, Gifford, Tureski, Griffin and Lowry21 reported better spatial learning and memory during pregnancy in rats, as have others. Reference Bodensteiner, Cain, Ray and Hamula22 These improvements persist. In motherhood, parous rats compared with nulliparous rats navigated mazes more efficiently, Reference Kinsley, Bardi, Karelina, Rima, Christon and Friedenberg23 experienced less anxiety and fear, as indexed by levels of stress hormones in the blood and by behaviour in open fields. Reference Love, Torrey, McNamara, Morgan, Banks and Hester24,Reference Kinsley and Lambert25 Although the tests used to measure cognitive functioning in rodents and humans differ markedly, it is surprising that outcomes of the human and rat research appear to be at such odds, given that ‘most mammals share similar maternal behaviours, which are probably controlled by the same brain regions in both humans and rats’. Reference Kinsley and Lambert25 This suggested to us that the effect of pregnancy or motherhood on cognitive abilities may not have been adequately tested. No study of human mothers has collected or examined data on cognitive performance prior to pregnancy, the samples recruited to the studies were effectively convenience samples, with many women recruited as volunteers from prenatal classes and matched to non-pregnant ‘friends’ and, although studies examined for the possibility of brief impairments immediately pre- and post-birth, few studies examined long-term effects, months or years after pregnancy or motherhood. The current study sought to determine whether pregnancy and motherhood are associated with cognitive deterioration using a study that measured baseline cognitive performance before pregnancy and motherhood, and used a large representative sample. Hypotheses were framed in terms of deficits. To test our first hypothesis (hypothesis one) that pregnancy leads to impaired cognitive functioning, we compared women pregnant at wave two with women not pregnant at or before wave two on change in cognitive function from wave one to wave two. Analogously, change from wave two to wave three was compared between those pregnant at wave three and those not pregnant at or before wave three. To assess short-term effects we examined the effects of length of pregnancy on cognitive performance as a function of stage of pregnancy (1–4 months v. 5 months or more). To investigate whether the birth of a child (i.e. motherhood) impaired cognitive capacity (hypothesis two), we compared women who became pregnant between waves one and two and who were, therefore, new mothers at wave two with women not pregnant at or before wave two. Similarly, we compared women who became pregnant between waves two and three and who were new mothers at wave three with women not pregnant at or before wave three. Four areas of cognition were assessed. These were cognitive speed, working memory, immediate and delayed recall.

Method

Participants

The Personality and Total Health (PATH) Through Life Project is a prospective longitudinal narrow-age cohort community survey concerned with health and well-being. Three cohorts were recruited in 1999 (20–24 years, 40–44 years and 60–64 years). Reference Jorm, Korten, Rodgers, Jacomb and Christensen26 The sample for this study consisted of women from the youngest cohort. In this cohort, 2404 individuals aged 20–24 years were recruited from the electoral roll with a recruitment rate of 58.6%. Of these, 1241 were women who completed the baseline measurement in 1999. Subsequently, 1126 and 1058 women completed the first and second follow-up in 2003 and 2007. This represented a follow-up rate of 91% and 85% respectively. Of the 115 participants who were not interviewed beyond the first wave, 83 refused or were unable to be interviewed for medical reasons, 30 could not be located and 2 died between the waves. Only women who had completed at least both waves one and two were considered in the present analyses. Ethics approval was obtained from the Australian National University's Human Research Ethics Committee.

Over the 8 years of the study, 76 were pregnant at follow-up (2003 or 2007) and 188 women became mothers (but were not pregnant at the time of the interview). Only primigravidae and first-time mothers were studied because multiple pregnancies and mothering of more than one child would introduce additional factors. Individuals becoming pregnant or becoming mothers between 1999 and 2003 were considered separately to women becoming pregnant or mothers between 2003 and 2007. In the analyses the two waves were compared separately because, over the 8-year period of the study, well-documented changes in IQ, as demonstrated in standard IQ scales such as Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Reference Wechsler27 would be expected as a result of maturation, practice effects and additional education. Further, participants who did not become pregnant or progress to motherhood at any time point may have had characteristics that were different from those not yet pregnant. The PATH survey has a broad focus and provided no cue that cognitive performance with respect to either pregnancy or motherhood was a focus of attention.

Survey procedure

Individuals selected at random from the electoral roll were sent a letter informing them of the survey and that an interviewer would contact them soon to see if they wanted to participate. If a person agreed to participate, the interviewer arranged to meet them at some convenient location, usually the participant's home or the Centre for Mental Health Research at the Australian National University. Most of the interview was self-completed on a palmtop or laptop computer. However, testing by the interviewer was required for the physical tests and for some of the cognitive tests.

Cognitive tests

Four domains of cognitive functioning were assessed using tests that are sensitive to change: cognitive speed, working memory, immediate recall and delayed recall.

Cognitive speed

Mental speed was measured with the Symbol–Digit Modalities Test (SDMT), which asks the participant to substitute as many digits for symbols as possible in 90 s. Reference Smith28

Working memory

Working memory was assessed with the Digits Backwards subtest of the Wechsler Memory Scale, Reference Wechsler29 which presents participants with series of digits at the rate of one per second and asks them to repeat the digits backwards.

Immediate and delayed recall

These were assessed with the first trial of the California Verbal Learning Test, Reference Delis, Kramer, Kaplan and Ober30 which involves recalling a list of 16 nouns. The interval between immediate and delayed recall was occupied by a test of grip strength.

Cognitive change scores were calculated for each test by subtracting wave one scores from wave two scores.

Predictor or control variables

Education

Educational attainment was measured using six questions concerning the full spectrum of past and current primary (elementary), secondary and tertiary educational attainment. Responses to these questions were coded into a single measure corresponding to the number of years of education. For the purposes of the analyses, education was then categorised into four groups: 0–12 years, 13 years (i.e. high school), 14–15 years and 16 years or more.

Depression

Depression was measured by the Goldberg Depression Scale. Reference Goldberg, Bridges, Duncan-Jones and Grayson31 The Goldberg Depression Scale consists of nine items, which are rated with a ‘yes/no’ response. Total scale scores are calculated by summing the number of ‘yes’ responses.

Anxiety

Anxiety was measured by the Goldberg Anxiety Scale. Reference Goldberg, Bridges, Duncan-Jones and Grayson31 The Goldberg Anxiety Scale consists of nine items, which are rated with a ‘yes/no’ response. Total scale scores are calculated by summing the number of ‘yes’ responses.

Other

Antidepressant medication or anxiolytic medication was assessed by asking whether participants were ‘taking any depression medication’ (yes/no), or ‘taking anxiety medication’ (yes/no). Participants were also asked their marital status (married/de facto married, not married), and whether they had sleeping difficulties, using one item ‘sleeping poorly’ (yes/no).

Cohorts

We sought to maximise available data at each wave and to use appropriate groups for comparison. Change in those pregnant at waves two and three were compared with change in contemporaneously non-pregnant women in separate contrasts. Seven relevant patterns of pregnancy and motherhood were identified and subgroups formed on the basis of these (Fig. 1). Groups one and two consisted of women who were either pregnant at the first wave of the study or who had been so beforehand. Because no information was available about the cognitive status of these women before they became pregnant or mothers these women could not be included in the study. Groups three to six comprise women who became pregnant at different stages of the study. Group seven are women who had never been pregnant up until the last occasion of measurement. The groups were variously used to test hypotheses as outlined in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1 Cohorts defined by pregnancy status at each wave and interwave interval.

N, Non-mother; P, Pregnant; M, Mother; DO, participant who dropped out.

Group 1: Mothers at entry; group 2: pregnant wave 1; group 3: new mothers wave 2; group 4: pregnant wave 2; group 5: new mothers wave 3; group 6: new mothers wave 3; group 7: never pregnant. Additional groups not shown in the figure: 115 women who dropped out post-wave 1 and 149 women who experienced multiple pregnancies between waves of data collection.

H1 refers to hypothesis one that pregnancy is associated with impaired functioning. Two contrasts compared women pregnant at the time of assessment (P) with those women not (yet) pregnant (P̄). The first contrast (W2–W1) involved women pregnant at wave 2, the second (W3–W2), women pregnant at wave 3. H2 refers to hypothesis two that motherhood leads to impaired cognitive functioning. Two contrasts compared women who had become mothers between a previous and the next assessment (M) with women who were not (yet) mothers (M̄). The first contrast (W2–W1) involves women who became mothers at wave 2, the second (W3–W2), women who became mothers at wave 3.

Statistical analyses

One-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were used to test baseline differences between each of the relevant subgroups for each of the cognitive variables. Mixed model repeated measures ANOVA Reference Brown and Prescott32 with group (as defined in Fig. 1) and wave as factors were fitted to each variable. Within-participant variation was modelled using an unstructured covariance matrix. The outcomes of conventional tests of each main effect and the interaction were not relevant to this study. Testing each hypothesis involved specifying a two degree of freedom contrast composed of the comparison appropriate subgroups for wave one to wave two change and an analogous comparison for wave two to wave three change. Results from each comparison were also available, enabling exploration of effects present only for wave two or for wave three. The numbers of participants with missing data for each comparison are given in the footnote to the corresponding tables. Sensitivity analyses indicated that, given the size of relevant subgroups, individual contrasts would have 80% power to detect between-group differences in the range 0.3 to 0.5 standard deviations.

To examine the effects of pregnancy and recency of motherhood on cognitive functioning we categorised pregnant women into early and late pregnancy. To examine the effects of recent transition to motherhood, we divided women into those with babies under 12 months at the time of testing and those with older infants. Choice of categorisations was moderated by sample size considerations. Significance was set at P = 0.05.

Results

One-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs) did not reveal baseline differences between the relevant sample groups on the tests of cognitive function (Tables 1 and 2). To examine the effects of potential confounding variables, the relationship of each cognitive outcome with age, education, depression, anxiety, taking medication for anxiety or depression, marital status and ‘sleeping poorly’ was investigated (time-varying for each occasion of measurement). Only education was identified as a significant covariate for all of the cognitive factors (all P<0.001). However, further investigations comparing the adjusted and unadjusted models and estimated means for the outcome variables clearly demonstrated that the effect of education was negligible. Therefore, the analyses reported below were not adjusted for education (or any of the other potential covariates).

Table 1 Means and standard deviations for pregnant and non-pregnant (comparison) subgroups (hypothesis one)a

Mean (s.d.)
Subgroup Pregnant wave 2 (n = 30) Pregnant wave 3 (n = 46) New mothers wave 3 (n = 112) Never pregnant (n = 542)
Status wave 2 P
Status wave 3 - P -
Wave 1
    Symbol—Digit Modalities Test 65.27 (9.62) 65.64 (11.09) 66.16 (9.94) 65.49 (10.07)
    Digits Backwards 5.07 (2.24) 5.30 (2.45) 5.29 (2.08) 5.36 (2.29)
    Immediate recall 8.30 (2.25) 8.24 (1.95) 8.77 (2.36) 8.54 (2.21)
    Delayed recall 7.90 (2.41) 7.78 (2.61) 8.06 (2.55) 7.77 (2.34)
Wave 2
    Symbol—Digit Modalities Test 65.93 (8.89) 68.36 (9.70) 68.41 (8.80) 66.83 (9.68)
    Digits Backwards 5.93 (2.02) 5.88 (2.31) 5.81 (2.13) 5.77 (2.28)
    Immediate recall 9.13 (2.43) 9.02 (2.24) 9.32 (2.24) 8.96 (2.40)
    Delayed recall 8.53 (2.80) 8.14 (2.65) 8.86 (2.60) 8.41 (2.59)
Wave 3
    Symbol—Digit Modalities Test - 66.18 (9.81) - 67.25 (9.84)
    Digits Backwards - 5.35 (2.35) - 5.90 (2.26)
    Immediate recall - 9.20 (2.50) - 9.23 (2.28)
    Delayed recall - 8.48 (2.37 - 8.69 (2.46)

Table 2 Means and standard deviations for new mother and non-mother (comparison) subgroups (hypothesis two)

Mean (s.d.)
Subgroup New mothers wave 2 (n = 76) New mothers wave 3 (n = 112) Pregnant wave 3 (n = 46) Never pregnant (n = 542)
Status wave 2 M
Status wave 3 - M -
Wave 1
    Symbol—Digit Modalities Test 64.60 (9.14) 66.16 (9.94) 65.64 (11.09) 65.49 (10.07)
    Digits Backwards 4.78 (2.36) 5.29 (2.08) 5.30 (2.45) 5.36 (2.29)
    Immediate recall 8.28 (2.17) 8.77 (2.36) 8.24 (1.95) 8.54 (2.21)
    Delayed recall 7.67 (2.31) 8.06 (2.55) 7.78 (2.61) 7.77 (2.34)
Wave 2
    Symbol—Digit Modalities Test 65.67 (8.98) 68.41 (8.80) 68.36 (9.70) 66.83 (9.68)
    Digits Backwards 5.14 (2.07) 5.81 (2.13) 5.88 (2.31) 5.77 (2.28)
    Immediate recall 8.34 (2.13) 9.32 (2.24) 9.02 (2.24) 8.96 (2.40)
    Delayed recall 7.84 (2.48) 8.86 (2.60) 8.14 (2.65) 8.41 (2.59)
Wave 3
    Symbol—Digit Modalities Test - 66.89 (8.64) - 67.25 (9.84)
    Digits Backwards - 6.13 (2.36) - 5.90 (2.26)
    Immediate recall - 9.61 (2.45) - 9.23 (2.28)
    Delayed recall - 9.07 (2.76) - 8.69 (2.46)

The pre-specified contrasts within mixed model repeated measures ANOVAs found no significant differences in cognitive change from waves one to two between those who were pregnant and those who were not on tests of cognitive speed (SDMT) and memory recall (immediate recall, delayed recall) (Fig. 2 and Table 3). There was one significant effect in favour of non-pregnant women on working memory (Digits Backwards subtest) between waves two and three. Those who became pregnant at wave three experienced a greater decline in their Digits Backwards score than those who remained non-pregnant (mean difference in change scores −0.640, P = 0.037). However, the aggregate effect over the two waves was not significant and if correction had been made for multiple testing, the effect would not be significant.

Fig. 2 Scores on cognitive tests as a function of pregnancy status.

Table 3 Mean differences in change scores for cognitive tests by pregnancy status for each wave

Wave 1-2 Wave 2-3 Overall
Testa Contrast valueb (95% CI) P Contrast valueb (95% CI) P P
Symbol—Digit Modalities Test -1.41 (-4.22 to 1.41) 0.327 -1.73 (-3.70 to 2.33) 0.084 0.160
Digits Backwards 0.35 (-0.41 to 1.12) 0.365 -0.64 (-1.25 to -0.04) 0.037 0.058
Immediate recall 0.24 (-0.68 to 1.15) 0.610 -0.07 (-0.83 to 0.68) 0.848 0.850
Delayed recall 0.05 (-0.87 to 0.99) 0.914 0.13 (-0.66 to 0.92) 0.746 0.947

There were no significant differences in cognitive change between waves one and two for those who had become mothers between waves and those who had not. This was also the case for cognitive change between waves two and three (Fig. 3 and Table 4). The mean cognitive scores for each sample group across time can be seen in Fig. 3. Performance improved in all groups.

Fig. 3 Scores on cognitive tests as a function of motherhood status.

Table 4 Mean differences in change scores for cognitive tests by motherhood status for each wave

Wave 1-2 Wave 2-3 Overall
Testa Contrast valueb (95% CI) P Contrast valueb (95% CI) P P
Symbol—Digit Modalities Test -1.02 (-2.92 to 0.87) 0.289 -1.21 (-2.50 to 0.09) 0.068 0.121
Digits Backwards -0.14 (-0.66 to 0.37) 0.583 0.24 (-0.16 to 0.64) 0.235 0.400
Immediate recall -0.53 (-1.14 to 0.08) 0.088 0.04 (-0.46 to 0.54) 0.878 0.223
Delayed recall -0.41 (-1.04 to 0.22) 0.198 -0.04 (-0.56 to 0.48) 0.889 0.437

Secondary analyses examining stage of pregnancy and time since birth were also undertaken. Women pregnant at wave two and at wave three were divided into those only recently pregnant (5 or more months remaining) and those in later stages (last 4 months of pregnancy). Women in each of these groups were compared separately with the non-pregnant groups on each measure. The only effect found was on the SDMT for women in the later stages of pregnancy. Performance on this test fell from the previous wave by 2.60 points and 2.79 points for women pregnant at waves two and three respectively, whereas non-pregnant women recorded an improvement (2.23 points at wave two) or stability (0.13 point drop at wave three). These contrasts were significant (t 687.7 = 2.000, P = 0.046 and t 646.5 = 2.138, P = 0.033). There were no effects for working memory or immediate or delayed recall. Secondary analysis of the motherhood data compared recent (infant less than 12 months) with established motherhood (older). There were no significant differences, and numbers precluded additional analysis of women immediately post-birth.

Discussion

Findings from the study

In short, no negative cognitive impacts of pregnancy and motherhood were observed in the full sample in this representative prospective study, except for the Digits Backwards subtest at wave three. Pregnant women at wave three, but not wave two, remembered approximately one digit backwards fewer than prior to pregnancy. However, this isolated finding would not remain significant under adjustment for multiple testing and was not significant when both waves were combined. These results provided no support for the study's hypotheses with respect to long-term cognitive change as a function of pregnancy or motherhood. In secondary analyses, we did find that women in later pregnancy in both waves were poorer on the SDMT but not on any tests of memory functioning.

Reasons for the discrepancy with extant literature

Although we found an isolated effect of late pregnancy on speed of cognitive performance in women in late pregnancy, findings from this prospective study are not consistent with the bulk of the literature, which reports various forms of cognitive deficit in both pregnancy and motherhood. One interpretation of the inconsistency is that the findings from previous studies are biased. This could be because of the recruitment of volunteer mothers who may differ in significant ways from ‘average’ pregnant women, being more concerned or anxious about the effect of pregnancy on their cognitive status or more depressed or sleep deprived. Alternatively, control volunteers may have differed from the pregnant groups in ways other than their non-gravid status. They may have a greater investment in cognitive performance than the pregnant women. Previously reported effects have generally been modest and could well have resulted from relatively subtle biases to which samples of convenience and non-randomly assigned groups are at risk.

A second explanation is that more cognitive deficits do exist, but that the tests used failed to detect them. This is unlikely as the measures employed were the same or similar to those previously used with reported effects for working memory Reference Janes, Casey, Huntsdale and Angus7,Reference Rendell and Henry8,Reference Condon, Derham and Kneebone12 and speed. Reference Jarrahi-Zadeh, Kane, Van de Castlf, Lachenbruch and Ewing14,Reference Silber, Almkvist, Larsson and Uvnas-Moberg16 It remains possible that cognitive tests that reflect fluctuations in attention might reveal more subtle differences. Recently, Rendell and Henry have argued that pregnant women exhibited increased difficulties in implementing delayed intentions in daily life, and that these deficits emerged only outside of laboratory environments. Reference Rendell and Henry8 Moreover, tests in the present study, like the majority of previous research, are not designed to detect whether any potential deficits were because of motivational or dispositional differences between pregnant and non-pregnant women. Some commentators have suggested that pregnant women may be less disposed to undertake cognitive testing, may regard it as less important than control women, and thus perform more poorly because of poor motivation rather than diminished capacity. Reference Christensen, Poyser, Pollit and Cubis11 A limitation of this epidemiological study is that the tests in the current battery did not assess these specific, narrow abilities.

Limitations of our study

In interpreting the results the time between testing occasions needs to be considered. For the pregnant women, the pre-pregnancy assessments were up to 4 years before the testing during pregnancy. Women were at various stages of pregnancy at the time of testing. For these reasons, it is important to distinguish between short-term and longer-term effects. With the exception of one effect at wave two, our primary analyses indicated that there were no longer-term average effects of pregnancy on cognitive performance if all stages of pregnancy were combined. Our secondary analyses, which separated groups into those early and late in pregnancy, did find an effect for women late in pregnancy on a speed task within both waves, but this deficit was not found on three tests of memory. This finding underscores the importance of examining effects as a function of stage of pregnancy. Achieving this in unselected studies will be difficult because of the small numbers of women at each stage of pregnancy in even the largest studies. Regardless of this, the specific finding of a speed effect in late pregnancy requires replication.

For tests involving new mothers, the interval between not being a mother and becoming one was variable. For instance, some women had been mothers for periods of days whereas others had been mothers for years. There is no clear indication from the research literature as to when deficits might be likely to arise or to peak. Some studies have found memory deficits 32 weeks postpartum. Reference de Groot, Vuurman, Hornstra and Jolles13 Other studies have reported that the cognitive deficits are no longer apparent after short intervals of days. Reference Condon, Derham and Kneebone12 We found no correlation between the age of the child (i.e. period of motherhood) and cognitive deficits in supplementary analyses.

This study is the largest of its kind. Nevertheless it is limited in its ability to detect small effects because of the size of the pregnant and new mother groups. Steps were taken to maximise power and we did detect significant change in cognition (largely improvements) over the period. We were able to assess and exclude or control the effects of depression, anxiety, medication, sleep problems, age and education on the test performance. In the absence of the possibility of mimicking animal studies and randomly assigning women to pregnancy or non-pregnancy conditions, the design of the present trial is as robust as possible in the context of human experimentation.

Finally, one of the weaknesses of our study was our inability to link cognitive change with biological changes associated with pregnancy. We were unable to look at the effects of contraceptive use in mothers and non-mothers. These analyses require targeted rather than epidemiological investigation. Our study, however, had the advantage of being a large population-based sample with pre-test measures, features that are not present in studies with retrospective recruitment.

Implications

We did not find that outcomes for the gravid rat held for the human female. In particular we did not find significant cognitive improvements in functioning in pregnant women or in mothers relative to controls as have been found in earlier animal work. In this context it is important to acknowledge that human and animal studies do differ substantially, in that domains of cognition are different (for example water mazes in comparison to verbal recall), and that improvements in spatial functioning in animals are not universally found, with some studies finding deficits in rats during the third trimester. Reference Brown and Prescott32 Nonetheless, there were no indications of improvement in our study. Moreover, although cognitive tests will differ for humans and rats, tests are similar in that they reflect hippocampal functioning (see for example Lye et al) Reference Lye, Piguet, Grayson, Creasey, Ridley and Bennett34 so some degree of consistency might have been expected. Whether the differences between humans and animals are a result of differing biological substrates, the social environment or their interplay remains to be explored. Perhaps, more importantly from the perspective of mothers to be and those caring for them, we were also not able to establish substantial or consistent cognitive deficits. Except in a brief period in later pregnancy, these findings challenge the common myth that women develop ‘placenta brain’ or ‘baby brain’. We found no deficits on memory tests in particular. Since both women and their partners believe that women experience cognitive deficits in pregnancy (see Christensen et al), Reference Christensen, Poyser, Pollit and Cubis11 women and their partners need to be encouraged to be less automatic in their willingness to attribute common memory lapses to the salient causal factor of a growing or new baby. Obstetricians, general family doctors and midwives may need to use the findings from this study to promote the view that ‘placenta brain’ is not inevitable, and that perceptions of impairment may reflect emotional or other unknown factors. Not so long ago pregnancy was ‘confinement’ and motherhood meant the end of career aspirations. Our results challenge the view that mothers are anything other than the intellectual peers of their contemporaries.

Funding

The PATH Through Life Project is funded by NHMRC Grant number 418039. H.C. is funded by NHMRC Fellowship number 525411 and L.L. by NHMRC PhD Scholarship number 3451048.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank K. J. Anstey, A. F. Jorm, P. J. Butterworth P. A. Jacomb and K. Maxwell for contributions to the design of the PATH Through Life Project. Amelia Gulliver assisted in developing the reference list.

Footnotes

The PATH Through Life Project is funded by NHMRC Grant number 418039. H.C. is funded by NHMRC Fellowship number 525411 and L.L. by NHMRC PhD Scholarship number 3451048.

Declaration of interest

None.

References

1 Crystal, G. What is Pregnancy Brain? Wise Geek (http://www.wisegeek.com/what-is-pregnancy-brain.htm).Google Scholar
2 BabyCenter Medical Advisory Board. Forgetfulness During Pregnancy. BabyCenter (http://www.babycenter.com/0_forgetfulness-during-pregnancy_236.bc).Google Scholar
3 Denis Campbell. Pregnancy ‘does cause memory loss’. The Guardian 2008; 3 February (http://www.guardian.co.uk/science/2008/feb/03/medicalresearch.pregnancy).Google Scholar
4 Henry, JD, Rendell, PJ. A review of the impact of pregnancy on memory function. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2007; 29: 793803.Google Scholar
5 Brindle, PM, Brown, MW, Brown, J, Griffith, HB, Turner, GM. Objective and subjective memory impairment in pregnancy. Psychol Med 1991; 21: 647–53.Google Scholar
6 de Groot, R, Hornstra, R, Roozendaal, N, Jolles, J (2003) Memory performance, but not information processing speed, may be reduced during early pregnancy. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2003; 25: 482–8.Google Scholar
7 Janes, C, Casey, P, Huntsdale, C, Angus, G. Memory in pregnancy. I: Subjective experiences and objective assessment of implicit, explicit and working memory in primigravid and primiparous women. J Psychosom Obstet Gynaecol 1999; 20: 80–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8 Rendell, PG, Henry, JD (2008) Prospective-memory functioning is affected during pregnancy and postpartum. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2008; 30: 913–9.Google Scholar
9 Sharp, K, Brindle, PM, Brown, MW, Turner, GM. Memory loss during pregnancy. Br J Obstet Gynaecol 1993; 100: 209–15.Google Scholar
10 Shetty, DN, Pathak, SS. Correlation between plasma neurotransmitters and memory loss in pregnancy. J Reprod Med 2002; 47: 494–6.Google Scholar
11 Christensen, H, Poyser, C, Pollit, P, Cubis, J. Pregnancy may confer a selective cognitive advantage. J Reprod Infant Psychol 1999; 17: 725.Google Scholar
12 Condon, J, Derham, D, Kneebone, A. Cognitive functioning during pregnancy: a controlled investigation using psychometric testing. Int J Prenat Perinat Stud 1999; 199: 212.Google Scholar
13 de Groot, R, Vuurman, E, Hornstra, G, Jolles, J. Differences in cognitive performance during pregnancy and early motherhood. Psychol Med 2006; 36: 1023–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
14 Jarrahi-Zadeh, A, Kane, FJ, Van de Castlf, RL, Lachenbruch, PA, Ewing, JA. Emotional and cognitive changes in pregnancy and early puerperium. Br J Psychiatry 1969; 115: 797805.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15 Keenan, PA, Yaldoo, DT, Stress, ME, Fuerst, DR, Ginsburg, KA. Explicit memory in pregnant women. Am J Obstet Gynecol 1998; 179: 731–7.Google Scholar
16 Silber, M, Almkvist, O, Larsson, B, Uvnas-Moberg, K. Temporary peripartal impairment in memory and attention and its possible relation to oxytocin concentration. Life Sci 1990; 47: 5765.Google Scholar
17 Crawley, RA, Dennison, K, Carter, C. Cognition in pregnancy and the first year post-partum. Psychol Psychother 2003; 76: 6984.Google Scholar
18 Harris, N, Deary, I, Harris, M, Lees, M, Wilson, J. Peripartal cognitive impairment: secondary to depression? Br J Health Psychol 1996; 1: 127–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
19 Swain, AM, O'Hara, MW, Starr, KR, Gorman, LL. A prospective study of sleep, mood, and cognitive function in postpartum and nonpostpartum women. Obstet Gynecol 1997; 90: 381–6.Google Scholar
20 Casey, P. A longitudinal study of cognitive performance during pregnancy and new motherhood. Arch Womens Ment Health 2000; 3: 6576.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
21 Kinsley, CH, Madonia, L, Gifford, GW, Tureski, K, Griffin, GR, Lowry, C, et al. Motherhood improves learning and memory. Nature 1999; 402: 137–8.Google Scholar
22 Bodensteiner, KJ, Cain, P, Ray, AS, Hamula, LA. Effects of pregnancy on spatial cognition in female Hooded Long-Evans rats. Horm Behav 2006; 49: 303–14.Google Scholar
23 Kinsley, CH, Bardi, M, Karelina, K, Rima, B, Christon, L, Friedenberg, J, et al. Motherhood induces and maintains behavioral and neural plasticity across the lifespan in the rat. Arch Sex Behav 2008; 37: 4356.Google Scholar
24 Love, G, Torrey, N, McNamara, I, Morgan, M, Banks, M, Hester, NW, et al. Maternal experience produces long-lasting behavioral modifications in the rat. Behav Neurosci 2005; 119: 1084–96.Google Scholar
25 Kinsley, CH, Lambert, KG. The maternal brain. Sci Am 2006; 294: 72–9.Google Scholar
26 Jorm, AF, Korten, AE, Rodgers, B, Jacomb, PA, Christensen, H. Sexual orientation and mental health: results from a community survey of young and middle-aged adults. Br J Psychiatry 2002; 180: 423–7.Google Scholar
27 Wechsler, D. Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – Revised (WAIS–R). Psychological Corporation, 1981.Google Scholar
28 Smith, A. Symbol–Digit Modalities Test (SDMT) Manual. Western Psychological Services, 1982.Google Scholar
29 Wechsler, D. A standardized memory scale for clinical use. J Psychol 1945; 19: 8795.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
30 Delis, DC, Kramer, JH, Kaplan, E, Ober, BA. California Verbal Learning Test. Psychological Corporation/Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, 1987.Google Scholar
31 Goldberg, D, Bridges, K, Duncan-Jones, P, Grayson, D. Detecting anxiety and depression in general medical settings. Br Med J 1988; 297: 897–9.Google Scholar
32 Brown, H, Prescott, R. Applied Mixed Models in Medicine. Wiley, 2006.Google Scholar
33 Galea, LAM, Ormerod, BK, Sampath, XK, Wikie, DM, Phelps, MT. Spatial working memory and hippocampal size across pregnancy in rats. Horm Behav 2000; 37: 8695.Google Scholar
34 Lye, TC, Piguet, O, Grayson, DA, Creasey, H, Ridley, LJ, Bennett, HP, et al. Hippocampal size and memory functioning in the ninth and tenth decades of life: the Sydney Older Person's Study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2004; 75: 548–54.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Cohorts defined by pregnancy status at each wave and interwave interval.N, Non-mother; P, Pregnant; M, Mother; DO, participant who dropped out.Group 1: Mothers at entry; group 2: pregnant wave 1; group 3: new mothers wave 2; group 4: pregnant wave 2; group 5: new mothers wave 3; group 6: new mothers wave 3; group 7: never pregnant. Additional groups not shown in the figure: 115 women who dropped out post-wave 1 and 149 women who experienced multiple pregnancies between waves of data collection.H1 refers to hypothesis one that pregnancy is associated with impaired functioning. Two contrasts compared women pregnant at the time of assessment (P) with those women not (yet) pregnant (P̄). The first contrast (W2–W1) involved women pregnant at wave 2, the second (W3–W2), women pregnant at wave 3. H2 refers to hypothesis two that motherhood leads to impaired cognitive functioning. Two contrasts compared women who had become mothers between a previous and the next assessment (M) with women who were not (yet) mothers (M̄). The first contrast (W2–W1) involves women who became mothers at wave 2, the second (W3–W2), women who became mothers at wave 3.

Figure 1

Table 1 Means and standard deviations for pregnant and non-pregnant (comparison) subgroups (hypothesis one)a

Figure 2

Table 2 Means and standard deviations for new mother and non-mother (comparison) subgroups (hypothesis two)

Figure 3

Fig. 2 Scores on cognitive tests as a function of pregnancy status.

Figure 4

Table 3 Mean differences in change scores for cognitive tests by pregnancy status for each wave

Figure 5

Fig. 3 Scores on cognitive tests as a function of motherhood status.

Figure 6

Table 4 Mean differences in change scores for cognitive tests by motherhood status for each wave

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.