Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-gb8f7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T20:30:13.623Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Sleep quality and the cortisol and alpha-amylase awakening responses in adolescents with depressive disorders

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 August 2024

Rebekka Krempel
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, University of Regensburg, Germany
Irina Jarvers
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, University of Regensburg, Germany
Angelika Ecker
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, University of Regensburg, Germany
Daniel Schleicher
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, University of Regensburg, Germany
Romuald Brunner
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, University of Regensburg, Germany
Stephanie Kandsperger*
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, University of Regensburg, Germany
*
Correspondence: Stephanie Kandsperger. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Depressive disorders in adolescents affect all aspects of life and impose a very large burden of disease. Sleep is frequently affected by depression and is crucial for facing challenges during development. One of the postulated reasons for depression-induced sleep disruption is dysregulation of the physiological stress system.

Aims

To investigate the links of adolescent depressive disorders with subjective sleep quality, objective sleep quality, and the course of cortisol and alpha-amylase after awakening.

Method

We compared subjective sleep quality (via daily questionnaires) and objective sleep quality (via actigraphy measurement) of 35 adolescents with depressive disorders and 29 healthy controls over 7 consecutive days. In addition, saliva samples were collected on 3 days to examine cortisol and alpha-amylase patterns after awakening.

Results

No significant differences in cortisol or alpha-amylase awakening responses were observed between participants with depressive disorders and healthy controls. We found severe reductions in subjective sleep quality in the depression group (Z = −5.19, P < 0.001, d = 1.80) and a prolonged actigraphy-measured sleep onset latency (Z = −2.42, P = 0.015, d = 0.64) compared with controls. Reductions in subjective sleep quality were partially correlated with objective sleep measures (sleep onset latency: r = −0.270, P = 0.004, sleep efficiency: r = 0.215, P = 0.017).

Conclusions

Sleep onset latency seems to aggravate depressive symptoms and to have an important role in perception of sleep quality. Adolescents with depressive disorders should be supported regarding the establishment of good sleep hygiene and avoiding activities that may impede falling asleep.

Type
Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Royal College of Psychiatrists

Adolescent depression and sleep quality

Depressive disorders are common during adolescence, with a point prevalence of approximately 8%Reference Shorey, Ng and Wong1 and a considerable increase in onset risk through adolescence.Reference Merikangas, Nakamura and Kessler2 Reduced sleep quality is a widely experienced symptom accompanying major depressive disorder (MDD). Previous research has indicated that sleep problems accompanying depression may be even more frequent in adolescents than in adults.Reference Rice, Riglin, Lomax, Souter, Potter and Smith3

Sleep quality can be subdivided into objective and subjective aspects. Objective aspects comprise sleep onset latency, sleep duration, sleep efficiency, sleep stages and sleep patterns and can be measured by polysomnography or actigraphy. Subjective characteristics include estimated sleep onset latency, estimated frequency of nocturnal awakening, quality of dreams, presence of unintended early awakening and restfulness of sleep. These aspects are usually assessed via questionnaires. Consequences of impaired sleep quality include higher severity of currentReference Liu, Buysse, Gentzler, Kiss, Mayer and Kapornai4 and later depressive symptoms,Reference Kuhlman, Chiang, Bower, Irwin, Seeman and McCreath5 increased anxietyReference Baum, Desai, Field, Miller, Rausch and Beebe6 and decreased positive emotions.Reference Talbot, McGlinchey, Kaplan, Dahl and Harvey7 A previous study in a nonclinical adolescent sample found a positive correlation between later sleep onset and depressive symptom severity and a negative correlation between total sleep duration and depressive symptom severity.Reference Thorburn-Winsor, Neufeld, Rowthorn, van Sluijs, Brage and Jones8 Moreover, sleep problems increase the risk of dying by suicide in adolescence.Reference Goldstein, Bridge and Brent9

Relationship between sleep and biomarkers of stress systems

Sleep has a modulating effect on the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis.Reference Nicolaides, Vgontzas, Kritikou, Chrousos, Feingold, Anawalt, Blackman, Boyce, Chrousos and Corpas10 The HPA axis is also known in particular for its role in the stress response, which is primarily mediated by the stress hormone cortisol.Reference O'Connor, Thayer and Vedhara11 Dysregulation of the HPA axis and subsequent alterations in cortisol secretion comprise a common hypothesis in neuroendocrinological research on depressive disorders. Cortisol secretion follows a characteristic diurnal cycle with an early morning peak, a prompt increase after awakening (cortisol awakening response (CAR)) and a decline throughout the day.Reference Fries, Dettenborn and Kirschbaum12 Regarding adolescent depression, previous research is heterogeneous, with some studies observing an increase, some finding a decrease and others identifying no difference in levels of morning cortisol in adolescent participants with depressive disorders.Reference Zajkowska, Gullett, Walsh, Zonca, Pedersen and Souza13

Another biomarker besides cortisol that plays an important part in the stress response is salivary alpha-amylase.Reference Nater and Rohleder14 Many studies also indicate the relevance of salivary alpha-amylase in the context of sleep.Reference Thieux, Guyon, Seugnet and Franco15 The diurnal secretion pattern of alpha-amylase in adolescents is not well understood; some studies have reported a characteristic decrease within the first 30 min after awakening (amylase awakening response (AAR)Reference Doane and Van Lenten16), whereas other research did not observe a decrease.Reference Adam, Till Hoyt and Granger17 During the day, amylase levels increase.Reference Doane and Van Lenten16,Reference Adam, Till Hoyt and Granger17 However, secretion patterns and levels seem to be variable and depend on the stage of puberty.Reference Adam, Till Hoyt and Granger17

In a sample of adults with depression, Bauduin et al found elevated levels of alpha-amylase after awakening compared with healthy controls and compared with individuals with other psychiatric disorders; they proposed alpha-amylase as a ‘new candidate biomarker for MDD specifically’.Reference Bauduin, van Noorden, van der Werff, de Leeuw, van Hemert and van der Wee18 However, research concerning alpha-amylase secretion, especially in adolescents with depressive disorders, has been very limited.Reference Jezova, Trebaticka, Buzgoova, Durackova and Hlavacova19 In a sample of adolescents with depression, Jezova et al found no differences in activity of morning alpha-amylase, lower midday alpha-amylase activity, and no increase in enzyme activity throughout the day compared with healthy controls.Reference Jezova, Trebaticka, Buzgoova, Durackova and Hlavacova19

Hypotheses

We predicted that adolescents with depressive disorders would have lower objective and subjective sleep quality than healthy controls. We also expected significant differences in CAR and AAR in the depression group compared with healthy controls.

Method

Study design

This non-interventional, single-centre study was conducted at the Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy at the University of Regensburg, Germany. The study protocol has been previously published.Reference Krempel, Schleicher, Jarvers, Ecker, Brunner and Kandsperger20

Sample

Several a priori power analyses were conducted with the program G*PowerReference Faul, Erdfelder, Lang and Buchner21 depending on the statistical method. Determined sample sizes were n = 28 for repeated measures, n = 44 for correlations and n = 60 for direction-dependence analysis (see ref. Reference Krempel, Schleicher, Jarvers, Ecker, Brunner and Kandsperger20 for details).

We recruited participants for the depression group from the out-patient and day clinic to minimise sleep alterations by measuring sleep in a familiar environment. Patients with diagnosed single-episode and recurrent MDD (F32.0, F32.1, F32.2, F33.0, F33.1, F33.2) or adjustment disorder with prolonged depressive reaction (F43.21) according to the ICD-10 diagnostic criteria22 without psychiatric medication were eligible for participation. Intake of antidepressants could be a factor influencing the inconsistency of the results; hence, we only included participants who were not taking antidepressants.Reference Elder, Wetherell, Barclay and Ellis23 Except for anxiety disorders, no comorbid psychiatric disorders were included. Furthermore, the exclusion criteria were psychotic or acute suicidal conditions, cannabis consumption within 3 months before participation, pregnancy, neurological or endocrinological illnesses with a known influence on brain or sleep, and intake of glucocorticoid medication.

The control group was recruited via advertisement in mailing lists, social media channels, websites of the study hospital, and via a local TV channel. Adolescent participants without current or past psychiatric diseases or a history of psychotherapeutic or psychiatric intervention were age-matched to those in the depression group. The same exclusion criteria were applied as in the patient group. We included 35 adolescents in the depression group and 29 adolescents in the control group after excluding one adolescent reporting a prior depressive episode.

Ethics and consent

Written informed consent from participants and their legal guardians was required for participation and was obtained before any assessment was conducted. Withdrawal by adolescents or their legal guardians was possible at any time.

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. All procedures involving human patients were approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Regensburg (reference number 20-1711-101).

As an expense allowance, all participants received a €50 gift voucher.

Assessments

Measurements on the first study day

Psychiatric diagnoses were conducted by qualified clinicians under the supervision of a certified child and adolescent psychiatrist using the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview for Children and Adolescents 6.0Reference Sheehan, Lecrubier, Sheehan, Amorim, Janavs and Weiller24 according to the DSM-IV and ICD-10 criteria. We assessed depressive symptoms in the past 2 weeks with the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II)Reference Hautzinger, Keller and Kühner25 but excluded item 21 (loss of interest in sexuality) to adapt this questionnaire to the younger participants. Somatic anxiety symptoms in the past 2 weeks were evaluated with the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI).Reference Margraf and Ehlers26

Sleep quality in the past 2 weeks was evaluated with the Sleep Questionnaire B (SF-B/R).Reference Görtelmeyer27 Sleeping traits (chronotype, regularity of bedtime, duration of sleep needed to feel rested, and frequency of sleeping through school or work) were assessed with a self-designed sleeping trait questionnaire. To assess stress vulnerability and the use of stress coping strategies, we administered the Stress and Coping Questionnaire for Children and Adolescents (SSKJ 3-8 R).Reference Lohaus, Eschenbeck, Kohlmann and Klein-Heßling28 Pubertal status was evaluated with the self-report Pubertal Development Scale (PDS).Reference Watzlawik29

For a detailed description of the questionnaires used, please see Supplementary Table 1 available at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjo.2024.730.

Sleep questionnaires during measurement week

For 7 consecutive days (measurement week), participants reported their satisfaction with the past night's sleep, current mood and feeling of recreation, difficulties with sleeping in, nocturnal awakening, stress level in the past day, presence of nightmares, quantity of caffeinated beverages consumed, quantity of alcoholic beverages consumed, and sleeping and wake-up times on a self-designed sleep questionnaire completed in the morning after awakening.

Accelerometry

During the measurement week (Monday to Sunday), participants wore an Act Trust 2 wrist actimeter (Condor Instruments) on their non-dominant hand overnight for 7 consecutive nights. The device recorded data in 1-min intervals during the night. Participants were instructed to mark their intended sleeping time, any occurrences during the night (for example, going to the bathroom) and their wake-up time by pushing the event button on the device.

Recordings were checked, and the main sleeping periods were identified with ActStudio software (Condor Instruments, São Paulo, Brazil, PC version 1.0.10, for Windows). Bedtime, wake-up time, time in bed, total sleep time, sleep onset latency, sleep efficiency, wakefulness after sleep onset and awakenings were derived for each night and automatically averaged for subsequent analysis.

Saliva sampling

On three consecutive mornings during the measurement week (Tuesday to Thursday), participants provided saliva samples directly after awakening (baseline), 30 min after awakening (CAR peak) and 45 min after awakening (CAR decline). According to Zajkowska et al,Reference Zajkowska, Gullett, Walsh, Zonca, Pedersen and Souza30 the assessment of CAR has been quite heterogeneous throughout studies examining CAR in adolescents with depressive disorder; therefore, we orientated our CAR assessment to the expert consensus guidelines for CAR.Reference Stalder, Kirschbaum, Kudielka, Adam, Pruessner and Wüst31 To simplify handling for the participants, salivettes were labelled with sampling times and marked with different colours for each sampling day. No eating, drinking or tooth brushing was allowed 10 min before sampling, and accurate sampling was demonstrated and practised on the first study day. In addition, participants were provided with written instructions for saliva sampling. Salivettes (Salivette Cortisol, Sarstedt, Nümbrecht, Germany, item number 51.1534.500) were used and stored in a container equipped with the Medication Event Monitoring System (MEMS®, Aardex, Liege, Belgium) to check sampling times. We informed the participants about the monitoring before sampling to improve the accuracy of sampling times. After sampling, we asked participants to store the samples in the freezer. On the second study day, samples were stored at −20 °C, and cortisol and alpha-amylase levels were determined by the Department of Biopsychology, Technical University of Dresden, Germany. See Supplementary Table 2 for an overview of sampling times, and refer also to the supplementary information for details of the alpha amylase assay analysis.

Measurement consistency

For all participants, the measurement week began on a Sunday evening and included the same number of weekdays and weekend days. To control for the influence of the menstrual cycle on the CAR,Reference LeRoux, Wright, Perrot and Rusak32 female participants not taking contraception participated during their luteal phase.

Statistical analyses

In the first step, group differences in control variables were examined via t-tests, equivalence tests and Mann‒Whitney U-tests. To examine differences in cortisol and alpha-amylase measurements over time, values were square-root transformed to achieve a normal distributionReference Kobayashi and Miyazaki33 and averaged over the 3 days. Repeated-measures analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was performed with cortisol and alpha-amylase levels as dependent variables and group, PDS scores, BDI scores and BAI scores as independent variables. As a further measure, the cortisol increase and alpha-amylase decrease were computed via area under the curve analyses relative to ground/increase (AUCg/AUCi)Reference Pruessner, Kirschbaum, Meinlschmid and Hellhammer34 and delta scores between the baseline value and the peak value.Reference Kudielka, Buske-Kirschbaum, Hellhammer and Kirschbaum35,Reference Kudielka, Schommer, Hellhammer and Kirschbaum36 Regarding subjective and objective sleep quality, group differences were computed depending on variable distribution. Kendall's τ correlations were used to examine the relationship between objective and subjective measures. A false discovery rate correction was applied where appropriate.Reference Hochberg and Benjamini37 Effect sizes are reported with Cohen's d, with 0.2 corresponding to a small effect, 0.5 to a medium effect and 0.8 to a large effect. As a final step, direction dependence analysis (DDA) was used to examine the direction of the relationship between sleep quality and depressive symptomsReference Wiedermann and Li38 while controlling for pubertal status as a covariate. This was possible because the main variables were not normally distributed. All statistical analyses were conducted using IBM SPSS version 29.0 for Windows with a P-value threshold of 0.05 for significance. For additional details, see the research protocol.Reference Krempel, Schleicher, Jarvers, Ecker, Brunner and Kandsperger20

Results

The demographic and clinical characteristics of our study sample are shown in Table 1. There was statistical equivalence in age between the groups, but the depression group contained a higher proportion of female participants. In the depression group, pubertal development was more advanced compared with that of healthy controls, whereas the control group had a higher level of secondary school than the depression group. Most participants in the depression group had a first, moderate depressive episode (71.4%). In addition, 37.1% of the depression group was diagnosed with comorbid social phobia (F40.1). As expected, the groups differed strongly in mean BDI-II and BAI scores but not in use of hormonal contraceptives (Table 1).

Table 1 Sample characteristics

BDI, Beck Depression Inventory II (possible values from 0 to 60 owing to exclusion of item 21); BAI, Beck Anxiety Inventory (possible values from 0 to 63); F32.0, mild depressive episode; F32.1, moderate depressive episode; F32.2, severe depressive episode without psychotic symptoms; F33.0, recurrent depressive disorder, current episode mild; F33.1, recurrent depressive disorder, current episode moderate; F33.2, recurrent depressive disorder, current episode severe without psychotic symptoms.

a. Equivalence test.

b. Mann–Whitney U-test.

c. School types refer to secondary schools following elementary school in Germany. Gymnasium: highest level of secondary school, regular duration of 8–9 years, general qualification for university entrance; Fachoberschule/Berufsoberschule: tertiary school to achieve advanced technical college certificate, subject-related entrance qualification or general qualification for university entrance after visiting Realschule, duration of 2–3 years in addition to duration of Realschule; Realschule: intermediate level of secondary school, regular duration of 6 years; Mittelschule: 9 years of elementary school.

d. Chi-squared test.

Objective sleep quality

A significant group difference was found in sleep onset latency (Z = −2.423, P = 0.015). Other objective sleep quality variables (total sleep time, sleep efficiency, wakefulness after sleep onset, awakenings) did not differ significantly between the two groups (Table 2).

Table 2 Group differences in cortisol increase, alpha-amylase decrease, subjective sleep quality and objective sleep quality

CORT, cortisol; AMYL, alpha-amylase.

a. Subjective sleep quality: total sleep time, difficulty falling asleep, difficulty staying asleep, premature awakening (sleep indices of Sleep Questionnaire B); sleep quality: factor scale of Sleep Questionnaire B.

b. Objective sleep quality: actigraphy-measured variables, analyses of group differences with Mann–Whitney U-test.

Subjective sleep quality

Group comparisons regarding subjective and objective sleep quality are shown in Table 2. Significant group differences were found in the mean score on the daily sleep questionnaire (Z = −5.19, P < 0.001), sleep index for difficulties falling asleep (Z = −3.01, P = 0.003), sleep index for staying asleep (Z = −4.103, P < 0.001), sleep index for premature awakening (Z = −2.03, P = 0.042) and factor scale for sleep quality (Z = −5.045, p < 0.001). Scores on our custom-designed daily sleep questionnaire were correlated strongly with the factor scale sleep quality of the well-established sleep questionnaire (SF-B/R) (r = 0.51, P < 0.001).

Correlations of subjective and objective sleep quality with depressive symptoms

Correlations of subjective and objective sleep quality parameters with the severity of depressive symptoms are shown in Table 3. There was a significant correlation between sleep quality as reported on the daily sleep questionnaire and sleep onset latency (r = −0.27, P = 0.004) as well as sleep efficiency (r = 0.22, P = 0.017). The subjective estimation of total sleep time was significantly correlated with the actigraphy-measured objective total sleep time (r = 0.30, P = 0.001). Moreover, subjective estimation of sleep onset difficulties (SF-B/R sleep index: difficulty falling asleep) and actigraphy-measured sleep onset latency were significantly correlated (r = 0.22, P = 0.021). Regarding difficulties in sleeping through the night, there was no correlation between subjective estimation and actigraphy-measured variables (awakening, wakefulness after sleep onset).

Table 3 Correlations between subjective sleep quality, objective sleep quality and depressive symptoms (total sample)

a. Objective sleep quality: actigraphy-measured variables.

b. Subjective sleep quality: total sleep time, difficulty falling asleep, difficulty staying asleep, premature awakening (sleep indices of Sleep Questionnaire B); sleep quality: factor scale of Sleep Questionnaire B.

c.Depression symptom severity according to mean Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) II score.

*P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001.

Depressive symptoms were correlated with sleep quality as reported on the daily questionnaire (r = −0.52, P < 0.001) and the SF-B/R factor scale for sleep quality (r = −0.53, P < 0.001). Moreover, depressive symptoms were correlated with the sleep indices for difficulties falling asleep (r = 0.35, P < 0.001), staying asleep (r = 0.48, P < 0.001) and premature awakening (r = −0.26, P = 0.010). Regarding objective sleep quality parameters, depressive symptoms were correlated with actigraphy-measured total sleep time (r = −0.19, P = 0.033) and sleep onset latency (r = 0.22, P = 0.015).

Direction dependence analysis

DDA was used to evaluate two competing regression models of depression/anxiety symptoms (objective sleep onset latency → depression/anxiety symptoms (BDI/BAI scores) versus depression/anxiety symptoms (BDI/BAI scores) → objective sleep onset latency) while adjusting for the covariate of PDS. The first model was favoured by the skewness and kurtosis tests (z = 2.02, P = 0.043; z = 2.77, P = 006), homoscedasticity tests (χ2 = 22.10, P < 0.001) and nonlinear correlation tests using the square function (t(61) = 2.44, P = 0.018), suggesting that sleep onset latency causally influenced depression and anxiety symptoms. Regarding subjective sleep, DDA was not able to clearly decipher the causal direction.

Cortisol and alpha-amylase

The averaged courses of cortisol and alpha-amylase after awakening are presented in Fig. 1. There was no significant group difference regarding the time course of cortisol (F(2.59) = 0.39, P = 0.628). In addition, no significant group difference was found for the time course of alpha-amylase (F(2.59) = 1.41, P = 0.248). Regarding control variables, there was a statistically significant effect of the interaction between time and pubertal status on cortisol (F(2.59) = 4.99, P = 0.014) and on alpha-amylase (F(2.59) = 3.86, P = 0.029) (see ANCOVA results in Supplementary Table 3). The results did not change when the BDI was included as a dimensional measure, nor when n = 2 individuals and n = 5 values per day were excluded according to the MEMS® system.

Fig. 1 Cortisol and alpha-amylase awakening response of depression group and healthy control group. CORT, cortisol; AMYL alpha-amylase.

No significant group differences were found for the AUCg values for cortisol (Z = −0.49, P = 0.623) or alpha-amylase (Z = −0.47, P = 0.642) levels, AUCi values for cortisol (Z = −0.59, P = 0.557) or alpha-amylase (Z = −0.05, P = 0.962), delta increase in cortisol (Z = −0.28, P = 0.782) or delta decrease in alpha-amylase (Z = −0.64, P = 0.522).

Discussion

The present study assessed the sleep quality of adolescents with depressive disorders from different perspectives. The depression group showed significantly worse sleep quality and prolonged sleep onset latency compared with controls. However, the groups did not differ significantly in the magnitude of their CAR and AAR.

Objective sleep quality

Our findings revealed prolonged sleep onset latency in the depression group but no group differences in sleep efficiency, wakefulness after sleep onset or number of awakenings. These results seem largely consistent with previous findings of sleep research in adolescents with depression, according to a review by Rao.Reference Rao39 Nine of 13 studies found no difference in sleep efficiency for the depression group, whereas three studies observed a decrease in sleep efficiency in this group. Our results are in accordance with most of these studies. Regarding sleep onset latency, seven studies showed no difference and five studies indicated prolonged sleep onset latency in the depression group; the latter is in line with the results of the present study. Regarding the prolonged sleep onset latency, our results align with a review by Ivanenko et al, who concluded that prolonged sleep onset latency is ‘the most consistent change in subjective and objective (instrumental) measurements of sleep’Reference Ivanenko, McLaughlin Crabtree and Gozal40 in young people with depressive disorders. The negative correlation between sleep duration and depressive symptoms found in the present study aligns with the findings of Thorburn-Winsor et al.Reference Thorburn-Winsor, Neufeld, Rowthorn, van Sluijs, Brage and Jones8

Subjective sleep quality

The results of our study underline the frequency and variety of subjective sleep quality reductions in adolescents with depressive disorders. Adolescents with depressive disorders reported lower overall sleep quality but also problems in all stages of sleep. Our findings confirm the longer subjective sleep onset latency and longer subjective wakefulness after sleep onset previously observed in adolescents with depression.Reference Sivertsen, Harvey, Lundervold and Hysing41 However, Sivertsen et al also found a decrease in subjective total sleep time in adolescents with depression,Reference Sivertsen, Harvey, Lundervold and Hysing41 which was not confirmed in our study. In our study, reductions in sleep quality coincided in different measurements that assessed sleep quality in the previous 2 weeks as well as daily sleep quality.

Several participants in the depression group indicated that they were unable to complete the questionnaires in the morning because of bad mood and apathy. This raises the question of whether an assessment including an essential feature in the morning is suitable and feasible for use in a cohort of participants with depressive disorders, as these individuals are likely to experience pronounced morning lows. However, we are convinced that assessments such as ours help to visualise all aspects of the burden of disease. This is emphasised by the ability of the majority of participants to complete the morning assessment, although they were burdened or had experienced unsatisfying sleep.

Relationship between subjective and objective sleep quality

In our study, subjective sleep onset latency was prolonged in the depression group, but the objective wake time after sleep onset was not increased, and the number of awakenings was not significantly higher compared with the control group. Nonetheless, these aspects were rated significantly worse in the questionnaires by the depression group. This may indicate that during retrospective assessment the next morning, prolonged sleep onset latency may be generalised to some extent and may negatively affect the overall perception of sleep. Thus, sleep onset latency may be an interesting target for treating sleep problems in adolescents with depression, because reducing it could improve perceived sleep quality noticeably.

However, the reduction in subjective sleep quality in our study was correlated with decreased objective sleep efficiency. Similarly, subjective problems falling asleep were correlated with increased objective sleep onset latency, and estimated and objectively measured total sleep time were correlated. These findings partially contrast with those of Rao, who assumed a mismatch between subjective and objective sleep quality in her review,Reference Pruessner, Kirschbaum, Meinlschmid and Hellhammer34 and those of Thorburn-Winsor et al, who found a mismatch between estimated and actigraphy-measured total sleep time.Reference Thorburn-Winsor, Neufeld, Rowthorn, van Sluijs, Brage and Jones8

Cortisol and alpha-amylase

We found no significant difference in cortisol or alpha-amylase levels after awakening between the depression group and the control group. Our results raise the question of whether biological correlates could be less decisive than sleep characteristics and cognitive assessments when trying to approach the issue of impaired sleep quality in adolescent depressive disorders. Similarly, a recent meta-analysis reported divergent findings regarding morning cortisol levels in depressed adolescents, with no overall difference in morning cortisol.Reference Zajkowska, Gullett, Walsh, Zonca, Pedersen and Souza13 As a biomarker for MDD in adolescents, cortisol may be useful early on but less meaningful over time.Reference Zajkowska, Gullett, Walsh, Zonca, Pedersen and Souza13 However, other findings have indicated an increase in CAR in a sample of female adolescents with depressive disorders.Reference Schmidt, Laessle and Hellhammer42 Our study included both males and females; however, given the higher proportion of female participants in both of our study groups, larger deviations in the CAR of female participants should have also been shown in our cohort.

There have been few studies of alterations in alpha-amylase levels after awakening in adolescents with depressive disorders. Our finding of no differences in morning alpha-amylase levels of adolescents with depression is in line with those of Jezova et al, who studied a sample with a similar mean age.Reference Jezova, Trebaticka, Buzgoova, Durackova and Hlavacova19 By contrast, previous studies have found elevated levels of alpha-amylase at awakening, as well as elevated AUC values for morning alpha-amylase in adults with depressive disorders, and have even identified alpha-amylase as a ‘valuable candidate biomarker’ with high specificity for depressive disorders.Reference Bauduin, van Noorden, van der Werff, de Leeuw, van Hemert and van der Wee18 Considering the dependence on developmentReference Chung, Mukerji and Kozlowska43 and pubertal status,Reference Adam, Till Hoyt and Granger17 interrelations could be less clear during adolescence; thus, alpha-amylase levels may be a better biomarker of depression in adults.

Limitations

Some limitations of the study are important to note. Actigraphy measurements are less wide-ranging than polysomnography measurements and are unable to measure some important aspects (REM/non-REM). In addition, although we provided extensive verbal and written instructions to ensure the ecological validity of saliva sampling, some influencing factors in the home environment could not be controlled. Regarding methodology, the use of absorbent materials for saliva collection for alpha-amylase has been criticised by some authors.Reference Bosch, Veerman, de Geus and Proctor44 Although it would probably be better to collect passive drool samples here,Reference Bosch, Veerman, de Geus and Proctor44 results argue against a great influence. Possible differences should be the subject of further investigations. In addition, the generalisability of these findings may be limited owing to the exclusion of adolescents in in-patient settings, those taking antidepressant medication and those with other comorbidities besides anxiety disorders. Moreover, our sample size was limited, and the two groups differed in terms of sex distribution, pubertal status and educational attainment.

Implications and future directions

Our findings highlight the importance of incorporating sleep quality in the diagnosis and treatment process of depressive disorders as early as possible and then continuously throughout the course of disease. As prolonged sleep onset latency seems to aggravate depressive symptoms and may play an important part in perception of sleep quality, adolescents with depressive disorders should be supported regarding the establishment of good sleep hygiene and avoidance of activities that may impede falling asleep. Psychoeducation, consistent evening routines and relaxation techniques could potentially be used to support adolescents with sleep onset problems and reduce suffering due to impaired sleep if they are regularly incorporated and practised in therapeutic care.

Perceived sleep quality seems to be a good indicator of some aspects of objective sleep quality; thus, a self-report assessment of important aspects of sleep quality should be integrated into the early assessment of adolescents showing symptoms of depressive disorder. In addition, adolescents with persistent difficulties falling asleep should be monitored for mental health problems such as depressive symptoms or pathological anxiety.

Supplementary material

Supplementary material is available online at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjo.2024.730.

Data availability

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author, S.K., on reasonable request.

Acknowledgements

We thank Nicole Küfner and Elisa Heidingsfelder for their reliable support in data collection and Prof. Dr Clemens Kirschbaum (Department of Biopsychology, Technical University of Dresden) and his laboratory service team for their competent support and examination of our saliva samples. We also thank all the participating adolescents and their families.

Author contributions

R.K. participated in designing and coordinating the study and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. S.K. participated in study design and coordination and helped to write the manuscript. I.J. was responsible for the statistical analyses and the sample size calculation, helped to write the manuscript and critically revised the paper. A.E. helped to design tables and figures and critically revised the paper. R.B. and D.S. participated in study design and coordination and critically revised the manuscript. The final manuscript was revised and approved by all authors.

Funding

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

Declaration of interest

None.

References

Shorey, S, Ng, ED, Wong, CHJ. Global prevalence of depression and elevated depressive symptoms among adolescents: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Br J Clin Psychol 2022; 61(2): 287305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merikangas, KR, Nakamura, EF, Kessler, RC. Epidemiology of mental disorders in children and adolescents. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 2009; 11(1): 7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rice, F, Riglin, L, Lomax, T, Souter, E, Potter, R, Smith, DJ, et al. Adolescent and adult differences in major depression symptom profiles. J Affect Disord 2019; 243: 175–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, X, Buysse, DJ, Gentzler, AL, Kiss, E, Mayer, L, Kapornai, K, et al. Insomnia and hypersomnia associated with depressive phenomenology and comorbidity in childhood depression. Sleep 2007; 30(1): 8390.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhlman, KR, Chiang, JJ, Bower, JE, Irwin, MR, Seeman, TE, McCreath, HE, et al. Sleep problems in adolescence are prospectively linked to later depressive symptoms via the cortisol awakening response. Dev Psychopathol 2020; 32(3): 9971006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baum, KT, Desai, A, Field, J, Miller, LE, Rausch, J, Beebe, DW. Sleep restriction worsens mood and emotion regulation in adolescents. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2014; 55(2): 180–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Talbot, LS, McGlinchey, EL, Kaplan, KA, Dahl, RE, Harvey, AG. Sleep deprivation in adolescents and adults: changes in affect. Emotion 2010; 10(6): 831–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thorburn-Winsor, EA, Neufeld, SAS, Rowthorn, H, van Sluijs, EMF, Brage, S, Jones, PB, et al. Device-measured sleep onset and duration in the development of depressive symptoms in adolescence. J Affect Disord 2022; 310: 396403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldstein, TR, Bridge, JA, Brent, DA. Sleep disturbance preceding completed suicide in adolescents. J Consult Clin Psychol 2008; 76(1): 8491.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nicolaides, NC, Vgontzas, AN, Kritikou, I, Chrousos, G et al. HPA axis and sleep. In Endotext (eds Feingold, KR, Anawalt, B, Blackman, MR, Boyce, A, Chrousos, G, Corpas, E). MDText.com, Inc., 2000 (http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK279071/ [cited 18 Apr 2024]).Google Scholar
O'Connor, DB, Thayer, JF, Vedhara, K. Stress and health: a review of psychobiological processes. Annu Rev Psychol 2021; 72: 663–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fries, E, Dettenborn, L, Kirschbaum, C. The cortisol awakening response (CAR): facts and future directions. Int J Psychophysiol 2009; 72(1): 6773.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zajkowska, Z, Gullett, N, Walsh, A, Zonca, V, Pedersen, GA, Souza, L, et al. Cortisol and development of depression in adolescence and young adulthood – a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2022; 136: 105625.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nater, UM, Rohleder, N. Salivary alpha-amylase as a non-invasive biomarker for the sympathetic nervous system: current state of research. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2009; 34(4): 486–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thieux, M, Guyon, A, Seugnet, L, Franco, P. Salivary α-amylase as a marker of sleep disorders: a theoretical review. Sleep Med Rev 2024; 74: 101894.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doane, LD, Van Lenten, SA. Multiple time courses of salivary alpha-amylase and dimensions of affect in adolescence. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2014; 49: 4753.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adam, EK, Till Hoyt, L, Granger, DA. Diurnal alpha amylase patterns in adolescents: associations with puberty and momentary mood states. Biol Psychol 2011; 88(2): 170–3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bauduin, SEEC, van Noorden, MS, van der Werff, SJA, de Leeuw, M, van Hemert, AM, van der Wee, NJA, et al. Elevated salivary alpha-amylase levels at awakening in patients with depression. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2018; 97: 6977.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jezova, D, Trebaticka, J, Buzgoova, K, Durackova, Z, Hlavacova, N. Lower activity of salivary alpha-amylase in youths with depression. Stress 2020; 23(6): 688–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krempel, R, Schleicher, D, Jarvers, I, Ecker, A, Brunner, R, Kandsperger, S. Sleep quality and neurohormonal and psychophysiological accompanying factors in adolescents with depressive disorders: study protocol. BJPsych Open 2022; 8(2): e57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Faul, F, Erdfelder, E, Lang, AG, Buchner, A. G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods 2007; 39(2): 175–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
World Health Organization (WHO). The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines. WHO, 1992.Google Scholar
Elder, GJ, Wetherell, MA, Barclay, NL, Ellis, JG. The cortisol awakening response – applications and implications for sleep medicine. Sleep Med Rev 2014; 18(3): 215–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheehan, DV, Lecrubier, Y, Sheehan, KH, Amorim, P, Janavs, J, Weiller, E, et al. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry 1998; 59(20): 2233.Google ScholarPubMed
Hautzinger, M, Keller, F, Kühner, C. BDI-II: Beck Depressions-Inventar: Revision: Manual. Pearson Assessment, 2009.Google Scholar
Margraf, J, Ehlers, A. Beck-Angst-Inventar [BAI]. Deutschsprachige Adaptation des Beck Anxiety Inventory von AT Beck und RA Stern [BAI: Beck Anxiety Inventory. Manual. German Adaptation]. Harcourt, 2007.Google Scholar
Görtelmeyer, R. Schlaffragebogen A und B: SF-A/R und SF-B/R, 1st Auflage [Sleep Questionnaires A and B: SF-A/R and SF-B/R]. Hogrefe, 2011.Google Scholar
Lohaus, A, Eschenbeck, H, Kohlmann, CW, Klein-Heßling, J. Fragebogen zur erhebung von stress und stressbewältigung im kindes- und jugendalter (SSKJ 3–8) [Questionnaire for the measurement stress and coping in children and adolescents (SSKJ 3–8)]. Diagnostica 2006; 52(3): 131–42.Google Scholar
Watzlawik, M. Die erfassung des pubertätsstatus anhand der Pubertal Development Scale [Assessing pubertal status with the Pubertal Development Scale]. Diagnostica 2009; 55(1): 5565.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zajkowska, Z, Gullett, N, Walsh, A, Zonca, V, Pedersen, GA, Souza, L, et al. Cortisol and development of depression in adolescence and young adulthood – a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2022; 136: 105625.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stalder, T, Kirschbaum, C, Kudielka, BM, Adam, EK, Pruessner, JC, Wüst, S, et al. Assessment of the cortisol awakening response: expert consensus guidelines. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2016; 63: 414–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LeRoux, A, Wright, L, Perrot, T, Rusak, B. Impact of menstrual cycle phase on endocrine effects of partial sleep restriction in healthy women. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2014; 49: 3446.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kobayashi, H, Miyazaki, Y. Distribution characteristics of salivary cortisol measurements in a healthy young male population. J Physiol Anthropol 2015; 34(1): 30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pruessner, JC, Kirschbaum, C, Meinlschmid, G, Hellhammer, DH. Two formulas for computation of the area under the curve represent measures of total hormone concentration versus time-dependent change. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2003; 28(7): 916–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kudielka, BM, Buske-Kirschbaum, A, Hellhammer, DH, Kirschbaum, C. HPA axis responses to laboratory psychosocial stress in healthy elderly adults, younger adults, and children: impact of age and gender. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2004; 29(1): 8398.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kudielka, BM, Schommer, NC, Hellhammer, DH, Kirschbaum, C. Acute HPA axis responses, heart rate, and mood changes to psychosocial stress (TSST) in humans at different times of day. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2004; 29(8): 983–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hochberg, Y, Benjamini, Y. More powerful procedures for multiple significance testing. Stat Med 1990; 9(7): 811–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiedermann, W, Li, X. Direction dependence analysis: a framework to test the direction of effects in linear models with an implementation in SPSS. Behav Res Methods 2018; 50(4): 1581–601.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rao, U. Sleep disturbances in pediatric depression. Asian J Psychiatry 2011; 4(4): 234–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ivanenko, A, McLaughlin Crabtree, V, Gozal, D. Sleep and depression in children and adolescents. Sleep Med Rev 2005; 9(2): 115–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sivertsen, B, Harvey, AG, Lundervold, AJ, Hysing, M. Sleep problems and depression in adolescence: results from a large population-based study of Norwegian adolescents aged 16–18 years. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2014; 23(8): 681–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, U, Laessle, R, Hellhammer, D. Major depression in young girls is related to altered cortisol awakening response. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2013; 22(6): 379–84.Google Scholar
Chung, J, Mukerji, S, Kozlowska, K. Cortisol and α-amylase awakening response in children and adolescents with functional neurological (conversion) disorder. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 2023; 57(1): 115–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bosch, JA, Veerman, ECI, de Geus, EJ, Proctor, GB. α-Amylase as a reliable and convenient measure of sympathetic activity: don't start salivating just yet! Psychoneuroendocrinology 2011; 36(4): 449–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1 Sample characteristics

Figure 1

Table 2 Group differences in cortisol increase, alpha-amylase decrease, subjective sleep quality and objective sleep quality

Figure 2

Table 3 Correlations between subjective sleep quality, objective sleep quality and depressive symptoms (total sample)

Figure 3

Fig. 1 Cortisol and alpha-amylase awakening response of depression group and healthy control group. CORT, cortisol; AMYL alpha-amylase.

Supplementary material: File

Krempel et al. supplementary material 1

Krempel et al. supplementary material
Download Krempel et al. supplementary material 1(File)
File 32.3 KB
Supplementary material: File

Krempel et al. supplementary material 2

Krempel et al. supplementary material
Download Krempel et al. supplementary material 2(File)
File 19.9 KB
Supplementary material: File

Krempel et al. supplementary material 3

Krempel et al. supplementary material
Download Krempel et al. supplementary material 3(File)
File 22.2 KB
Supplementary material: File

Krempel et al. supplementary material 4

Krempel et al. supplementary material
Download Krempel et al. supplementary material 4(File)
File 15.7 KB
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.