Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gvvz8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T07:45:15.022Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Learner-driven innovation in the stone tool technology of early Homo sapiens

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 July 2020

Jayne Wilkins*
Affiliation:
Australian Research Centre for Human Evolution, Environmental Futures Research Institute, Griffith University, 170 Kessels Road, Nathan, QLD4111, Australia; and Human Evolution Research Institute, University of Cape Town, South Africa
*
*Corresponding author. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Current perspectives of stone tool technology tend to emphasize homogeneity in tool forms and core reduction strategies across time and space. This homogeneity is understood to represent shared cultural traditions that are passed down through the generations. This represents a top-down perspective on how and why stone tools are manufactured that largely restricts technological agency to experts, adults and teachers. However, just as bottom-up processes driven by children and youth influence technological innovation today, they are likely to have played a role in the past. This paper considers evidence from the archaeological record of early Homo sapiens’ lithic technology in Africa that may attest to our long history of bottom-up social learning processes and learner-driven innovation. This evidence includes the role of emulative social learning in generating assemblages with diverse reduction strategies, a high degree of technological fragmentation across southern Africa during some time periods, and technological convergence through the Pleistocene. Counter to some perspectives on the uniqueness of our species, our ability to learn independently, to ‘break the rules’ and to play, as opposed to conforming to top-down influences, may also account for our technological success.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Media summary: Lithic technological variability in Pleistocene Africa attests to our long history of non-conformity and bottom-up social learning.

Introduction

Humans are unusual among extant primates for our heavy reliance on social learning (Tomasello et al., Reference Tomasello, Kruger and Ratner1993). By at least 3.3 Ma, our hominin ancestors were making and using stone tools to aid in the acquisition of resources (Harmand et al., Reference Harmand, Lewis, Feibel, Lepre, Prat, Lenoble and Roche2015). Stone tool production creates a large amount of durable debris, and this debris is often the only surviving record of past human behaviours, thus, stone tool technology is the main avenue available to archaeologists for empirically investigating the evolution of human social learning (Stout & Hecht, Reference Stout and Hecht2017; Stout, Reference Stout2018; Shea, Reference Shea2006; Ranhorn et al., Reference Ranhorn, Pargeter and Premo2020). Some of the earliest stone tools hint that our capacities for social learning via imitation have deep roots in the Plio-Pleistocene (Stout et al., Reference Stout, Rogers, Jaeggi and Semaw2019), with more enhanced capacities evidenced in the complex technologies associated with the emergence of our species, Homo sapiens (Wadley, Reference Wadley2010; Brown et al., Reference Brown, Marean, Herries, Jacobs, Tribolo, Braun and Bernatchez2009, Reference Brown, Marean, Jacobs, Schoville, Oestmo, Fisher and Matthews2012; Mourre et al., Reference Mourre, Villa and Henshilwood2010; Wilkins et al., Reference Wilkins, Schoville, Brown and Chazan2012).

One advantage of social learning is that it facilitates cumulative culture change (i.e. the ‘rachet effect’), which is the human capacity to build on the cultural behaviours of others, allowing increases in complexity and/or efficiency over time (Tomasello, Reference Tomasello2016; Tennie et al., Reference Tennie, Call and Tomasello2009, Reference Tennie, Braun, Premo, McPherron, Haidle, Conard and Bolus2016; Boyd & Richerson, Reference Boyd and Richerson2005; Richerson & Boyd, Reference Richerson and Boyd2005). In consideration of human social learning, much emphasis has been placed on our capacity for high-fidelity information transfer from one generation to the next and the role that active teaching plays in ensuring accurate down-the-line transmission of knowledge (Boyd & Richerson, Reference Boyd and Richerson2005; Richerson & Boyd, Reference Richerson and Boyd2005; Tennie et al., Reference Tennie, Call and Tomasello2009; Gärdenfors & Högberg, Reference Gärdenfors and Högberg2017; Shipton, Reference Shipton2019; Hiscock, Reference Hiscock2014; Sterelny, Reference Sterelny2012; Henrich, Reference Henrich2017; Shennan, Reference Shennan2002; Tehrani & Riede, Reference Tehrani and Riede2008). In response, lithic experimental studies have largely focused on skill transmission. For example, it has been shown that teaching through gesture and/or verbal language improves skill transmission, improving flake quality and knapping efficiency in novice knappers (Morgan et al., Reference Morgan, Uomini, Rendell, Chouinard-Thuly, Street, Lewis and de la Torre2015; Putt et al., Reference Putt, Woods and Franciscus2014, Reference Putt, Wijeakumar, Franciscus and Spencer2017; Cataldo et al., Reference Cataldo, Migliano and Vinicius2018). Interestingly, verbal instruction appears to actually hinder skill development in handaxe reduction; teaching through gesture alone is sufficient for the production of high-quality bifaces (Putt et al., Reference Putt, Woods and Franciscus2014). The first skills that novices need to learn include how to position the core and exploit appropriate core angles (Geribàs et al., Reference Geribàs, Mosquera and Vergès2010), and to control the force of each blow (Bril et al., Reference Bril, Rein, Nonaka, Wenban-Smith and Dietrich2010). With respect to tool form, the opportunity to imitate the manufacturing process appears to reduce ‘copy errors’ and results in more homogeneity in final tool shape (Schillinger et al., Reference Schillinger, Mesoudi and Lycett2015, Reference Schillinger, Mesoudi and Lycett2016). Pargeter et al. (Reference Pargeter, Khreisheh and Stout2019) have developed a skill metric for handaxe production based on multiple measurable variables that include symmetry, flake scar density, and thickness. Knapping activates regions in the brain associated with spatial awareness, strategic action planning, working memory, and language (Stout et al., Reference Stout, Toth, Schick and Chaminade2008, Reference Stout, Hecht, Khreisheh, Bradley and Chaminade2015; Stout & Hecht, Reference Stout and Hecht2017), suggesting that our capacities for knapping may have co-evolved with the other elements of complex cognition and sociality that characterize our species (Stout, Reference Stout2018). Ethnographic studies focused on lithic technology have emphasized the role of long-term apprenticeship in the production of specific tool forms, while further demonstrating how lithic production can be complexly entwined in the social lives, customs, and epistemologies of the artisans (Weedman, Reference Weedman2006; Stout Reference Stout2002; Arthur, Reference Arthur2018).

These previous studies have focused almost exclusively on the top-down vertical transmission of knowledge from expert to novice knapper. There has been less consideration of other directions of information flow or how new technologies are introduced and spread, despite innovation being a key component of cumulative culture change (Charbonneau, Reference Charbonneau2015; Riede et al., Reference Riede, Johannsen, Högberg, Nowell and Lombard2018). In lithic studies, research context may partially explain this bias. Academic lithicists often learn how to produce stone tools in an academic setting (Shea, Reference Shea2015; Clarkson, Reference Clarkson2017; Eren et al., Reference Eren, Kollecker, Clarkson and Bradley2010; Patterson, Reference Patterson1981), where active teaching is standard practice and high-fidelity information transfer is rewarded with formalized evaluation systems. This expert-novice dynamic also characterizes the historical trajectory of lithic studies, in general, and indeed many academic disciplines. It was the highly respected expert knappers François Bordes, Don Crabtree and Jacques Tixier, in the 1950s and 1960s, who set the foundation for many of the technological and experimental approaches that characterize lithic studies today (Kooyman, Reference Kooyman2000; Whittaker, Reference Whittaker1994; Andrefsky, Reference Andrefsky2005; Johnson, Reference Johnson1978). Academic lineages linked to these founders is a point of pride for many lithicists. Thus, in the context of a formalized learning institute and WEIRD (Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich and Democratic) society (Henrich et al., Reference Henrich, Heine and Norenzayan2010), students of lithic technology and knapping learn early on that the most valuable knowledge is transmitted vertically down the line from teacher to learner.

This perception of how knapping is learned, which might reflect Western, patriarchal values, has influenced not only the experimental studies discussed above, but also how we interpret the archaeological record. First, lithic analysts tend to emphasize homogeneity, looking for similarities across space and time to define industries (Clark et al., Reference Clark, Cole, Isaac and Kleindienst1966; Bishop & Clark, Reference Bishop and Clark1967), techno-complexes (Lombard et al., Reference Lombard, Wadley, Deacon, Wurz, Parsons, Mohapi and Mitchell2012) and ‘named stone tool industries’, or NASTIES (Shea, Reference Shea2014), and assuming that more similarity represents more interaction, more teaching, more cultural transmission, more top-down transmission of knowledge. Because of this, the concept of standardization became value-laden, and researchers have critiqued the way in which standardization has become problematically linked to cognition, language, and cultural sophistication (Marks et al., Reference Marks, Hietala and Williams2001; Chazan, Reference Chazan1995; Nowell, Reference Nowell2000). Second, lithic analysts often present one dominant chaîne opératoire for an assemblage or type of technology (e.g. Soriano et al., Reference Soriano, Villa and Wadley2007, Reference Soriano, Villa, Delagnes, Degano, Pollarolo, Lucejko and Wadley2015; Wurz, Reference Wurz2002). Variability is often presented as the unintended consequence of raw material peculiarities, or novices, or other factors that effectively represent ‘noise’ or errors (e.g. Schillinger et al., Reference Schillinger, Mesoudi and Lycett2016).

More than top-down transmission of knowledge

Human social learning is more complex than lithic studies tend to acknowledge. We rely on much more than just down-the-line knowledge transmission via active teaching. Most skill-learning requires both implicit and explicit learning; implicit learning does not rely on conscious efforts and is a foundation process for acquiring complex, tacit knowledge (Seger, Reference Seger1994; Reber, Reference Reber1989). Human skill acquisition involves the interaction of two learning pathways: top-down learning (from explicit to implicit knowledge) and bottom-up learning (from implicit to explicit knowledge) (Sun & Zhang, Reference Sun and Zhang2004). Of course, cultural transmission can occur both inter- and intra-generationally, vertically, obliquely or horizontally (Boyd & Richerson, Reference Boyd and Richerson2005; Richerson & Boyd, Reference Richerson and Boyd2005; Shennan, Reference Shennan2002). Learner skepticism is an important aspect of social learning in humans; learners actively evaluate teachers and will avoid copying those they identify as poor models (Kline, Reference Kline2015; Boyd & Richerson, Reference Boyd and Richerson2005). Technological innovation can be used to manifest new identities in opposition and in reference to the status quo (e.g. see discussion of skeuomorphs by Frieman, Reference Frieman2013). Innovation relies just as much on modification generating processes and individuals breaking from the status quo as it does transmission (Charbonneau, Reference Charbonneau2015). Indeed, the human capacity for rapid cultural change, as opposed to cultural conservation, can offer advantages in variable environments (Potts, Reference Potts1998). Thus, in some situations, one might consider the most successful learners to be those that do not conform to top-down influences but are active innovative agents that may ‘flip-the-script’ and end up teaching the teachers.

From a traditional perspective of human social learning, learners and novices are children and youth. In accounts of anthropologists and historians, teaching (specifically, student-centred, developmentally appropriate instruction by dedicated adults) plays a minor role in child development outside WEIRD societies (Lancy, Reference Lancy2010). In fact, student-centred teaching between adults and children is so rare that Lancy (Reference Lancy2010) suggests that ‘teaching has been largely superfluous in the process of cultural transmission through human history’ (p. 97), except for the very recent past. Rather, children learn by being immersed in the activities of their society, and with various members of that society assisting through opportunity provisioning, evaluation and increasing accessibility (an ethological definition of teaching, Lew-Levy et al. Reference Lew-Levy, Kissler, Boyette, Crittenden, Mabulla and Hewlett2020). For example, hunter–gatherer children learn several kinds of skills, including subsistence-related skills, from other children, not just the adults in their social network (Lew-Levy et al., Reference Lew-Levy, Reckin, Lavi, Cristóbal-Azkarate and Ellis-Davies2017, Reference Lew-Levy, Kissler, Boyette, Crittenden, Mabulla and Hewlett2020). As part of the developmental process, children copy some adult behaviours, often through play and sometimes with miniatures – small imitations of material culture, examples of which have been observed ethnographically and archaeologically (Langley & Litster, Reference Langley and Litster2018). Through play, children and youth explore and test the boundaries of materials, systems, technologies, and language, and in this way, play not only serves a role in learning, but can also can act as a primer for innovation (Riede et al., Reference Riede, Johannsen, Högberg, Nowell and Lombard2018; Nowell, Reference Nowell2016; Pellegrini et al., Reference Pellegrini, Dupuis and Smith2007; Langley et al., Reference Langley, Benítez-Burraco and Kempe2020). In other words, children and youth do not simply absorb and recreate the behaviours of the adults around them. They learn independently, they learn from other children, and thus, they can also be active, innovative agents of technological and societal change.

The purpose of this paper is to explore learner-driven innovation in lithic technology. Innovation requires a new idea that is subsequently transmitted. The general perception in lithic studies is that the source of the new idea is the expert, adult or teacher, but the research summarized above shows that the source can also be the novice, child or learner. Learner-driven innovation is facilitated by what I describe as bottom-up social learning processes – actions that result in knowledge transfer up the chain, from, for example, novices to experts (Figure 1). This contrasts with top-down social learning processes that result in knowledge transfer down the chain, from experts to novices, which is the emphasis of most previous considerations of social learning and lithic technology. Here, I consider how the archaeological record can inform us of past human social learning environments, which may help direct future experimental research. I suggest three ways learner-driven innovation may be expressed archaeologically and consider examples of this for the African Middle Stone Age (MSA, ~500–40 ka), which is critical for understanding the behavioural evolution of early Homo sapiens. The three archaeological correlates of learner-driven innovation that I suggest here are: emulation – applying self-determined strategies to accomplish a shared end goal; fragmentation – high diversity across geographic space; and convergence – independently invented similarity (Figure 1). These correlates are not isolated illustrations of individual innovation events, but rather the results of processes that over the long term led to the archaeological patterns of the African MSA that attest to our human capacity for learner-driven innovation.

Figure 1. Summary of bottom-up vs. top-down social learning processes and lithic technological correlates proposed here.

Emulation

Although sometimes used colloquially as synonyms, emulation and imitation differ with respect to process. Imitation occurs when learners copy the exact procedures and strategies of others for accomplishing a shared end goal (process copying), while emulation occurs when learners employ different strategies for accomplishing that shared end goal (product copying, Schillinger et al., Reference Schillinger, Mesoudi and Lycett2015; Caldwell & Millen, Reference Caldwell and Millen2009; Tennie et al., Reference Tennie, Call and Tomasello2009; Bentley, Reference Bentley, O’Brien, Buchanan and Eren2018; Whiten et al., Reference Whiten, McGuigan, Marshall-Pescini and Hopper2009). Both imitative and emulative forms of learning are part of the ‘portfolio of different social learning processes’ employed by humans, chimpanzees (Whiten et al., Reference Whiten, McGuigan, Marshall-Pescini and Hopper2009), and by extension, probably all species within the hominin lineage. Under some conditions, human children, but not chimpanzees, overimitate – that is, they copy actions that are casually irrelevant with high fidelity (for a review see Whiten et al., Reference Whiten, McGuigan, Marshall-Pescini and Hopper2009). In part owing to this difference between chimpanzee and human children, considerations of Palaeolithic and Stone Age technologies have almost exclusively focused on imitation, and the human capacity for imitation has been used to explain cumulative culture change (Boyd & Richerson Reference Boyd and Richerson2005). However, whether human children overimitate depends on several factors, which include whether the model is constrained to act that way or not (Gergely et al., Reference Gergely, Bekkering and Király2002), and whether the actions can be interpreted as accidents (Carpenter et al., Reference Carpenter, Akhtar and Tomasello1998). Emulation can also lead to cumulative culture change in the absence of imitation (Caldwell & Millen, Reference Caldwell and Millen2009; Wasielewski, Reference Wasielewski2014). Yet the role of emulation in the past as reflected in lithic technology, and its role in understanding cumulative culture change in the Pleistocene, is rarely examined.

There are several varieties of emulation (Subiaul et al., Reference Subiaul, Patterson and Barr2016; Whiten et al., Reference Whiten, McGuigan, Marshall-Pescini and Hopper2009). One main distinction directly relevant to this discussion is whether the actions and techniques are observable or not. In reverse engineering, or end-state emulation in the absence of a demonstration, only the finished product is observed, and there is no opportunity to observe how the actions are carried out. In goal emulation, the actions of another carrying out or attempting to carry out the goal are observed.

Both types of emulation are considered a reflection of learner-driven innovation here, because the learner develops their own strategies for accomplishing a goal, rather than relying on top-down transmission of those strategies. Emulation still involves copying a model – ideas about what to produce are shared – but exactly how those goals are accomplished can vary significantly between the novice and the expert. Through emulation, novices generate new technological procedures that others within the social network may subsequently uptake via imitation. Emulation as a social learning strategy may be advantageous in certain situations (Wilkins, Reference Wilkins, O'Brien, Buchanan and Eren2018). First, emulation relies on little to no teaching effort from experts, which frees up time for experts to pursue other goals. Second, because the novice is employing trial-and-error learning on the procedures used to accomplish a goal, it may lead more quickly to effective solutions on new or variable raw materials that the expert has not regularly encountered. Lastly, it generates diversity on which selective processes can act, leading to the transmission of innovations and inventions.

Based on experimental studies, lithic assemblages resulting from reverse engineering have end products that exhibit higher degrees of shape variability (Schillinger et al., Reference Schillinger, Mesoudi and Lycett2015; Ranhorn, Reference Ranhorn2017) and core reduction strategies (Ranhorn, Reference Ranhorn2017). Lithic reduction experiments focused specifically on goal emulation have not yet been reported. However, in an experiment where novices learned how to make handaxes in verbal and non-verbal settings, those in the non-verbal setting tended to emulate the demonstrator, rather than imitate (Putt et al., Reference Putt, Woods and Franciscus2014). While there were few differences between the resulting handaxes from the two conditions, the non-verbal condition where novices knappers tended to emulate resulted in debitage reflecting higher levels of flaking efficiency (i.e. large, thin flakes), but also more debitage. Participants in the verbal condition tended to imitate the production of ambitious platforms that they did not yet have the skill to execute, and this resulted in debitage reflecting lower levels of flaking efficiency (i.e. large, thick flakes), but also less debitage (Putt et al. Reference Putt, Woods and Franciscus2014).

Reverse engineering requires no direct interaction between individuals, while goal emulation, in contrast, occurs in social settings. Based on what we know about early human sociality (Gamble, Reference Gamble1999), there would have been many opportunities for goal emulation through the Pleistocene, and indeed for the whole portfolio of social learning processes available to early humans. For example, over the course of their lifetime, individuals witness others making large, regular points for their spears, and they may witness experienced knappers using bifacial, hierarchical cores to do so. Through a combination of observation and trial-and-error learning across many unstructured learning sessions, they develop their own strategy for making large, regular points, which is similar to, but also different from the model knappers around them. In this sort of context, it is likely that some active teaching and imitation also occur at some stages of the learning process, which is why a continuum is appropriate (Bentley, Reference Bentley, O’Brien, Buchanan and Eren2018). On the emulation side of this continuum, one expects more diversity in core reduction strategies, and on the imitation and active teaching side of this continuum, one expects less diversity in core reduction strategies. Based solely on the metrics of discarded flakes, one cannot accurately deduce core reduction strategy (Eren et al., Reference Eren, Buchanan, O'Brien, O’Brien, Buchanan and Eren2018); however, experimental research has shown that through a combination of core and flake attributes, it is possible to deduce MSA core reduction strategies (Scerri et al., Reference Scerri, Gravina, Blinkhorn and Delagnes2016).

There is empirical evidence from the MSA record of Africa for the application of emulative learning strategies. In the Middle Pleistocene assemblage from Kathu Pan 1, three lines of evidence were used to argue that there was more emphasis on emulation than imitation to produce points discarded there (Wilkins, Reference Wilkins, O'Brien, Buchanan and Eren2018). First, diverse reduction strategies, including preferential and recurrent Levallois strategies, as well as non-Levallois strategies, were used to produce blades and flakes. Second, both blades and flakes were used to produce unifacially retouched points. Third, diverse point types (unretouched blades and flakes, unifacially retouched blades and flakes) show evidence of having been used as weapon tips. From a top-down perspective, one would expect similarity in the reduction strategies and selective processes employed for manufacturing stone tools, but rather, intra-assemblage diversity suggests that a variety of processes were used to accomplish a single goal – a stone point suitable for use as a weapon tip. While evidence for emulation is rarely explicitly reported, several descriptions of MSA assemblages highlight core reduction strategy diversity (Table 1). This includes the MSA levels at Porc-Epic Cave, Ethiopia (Pleurdeau, Reference Pleurdeau2006), Koimilot, Kenya (Tryon, Reference Tryon2006; Tryon & McBrearty, Reference Tryon and McBrearty2006; Tryon et al., Reference Tryon, McBrearty and Texier2005) and Kudu Koppie, South Africa (Wilkins et al., Reference Wilkins, Pollarolo and Kuman2010). This intra-assemblage diversity hints that top-down, high-fidelity cultural transmission was not always a priority for MSA knappers.

Table 1. Summary of MSA assemblages that document coeval diverse core reduction strategies

Fragmentation

Various proxies are used by archaeologists to make interpretations about inter-group connectedness in the MSA. MSA stone points and proposed armature tips (i.e. backed pieces as arrowheads) show variability on continent-wide geographic and temporal scales, and it has been suggested that this variability reflects cultural patterns (Clark, Reference Clark1992; McBrearty & Brooks, Reference McBrearty and Brooks2000; Foley & Lahr, Reference Foley and Lahr1997, Reference Foley and Lahr2003; Willoughby, Reference Willoughby2007; Wadley, Reference Wadley2001). This suggestion is largely based on the potential role that armatures (and other technologies) can play in communicating messages about ethnic or group affiliation (Wiessner, Reference Wiessner1983). Armatures and other technologies can also serve as important symbolic objects in regional exchange networks, and thus may reflect cultural inter-connectedness (Wilkins, Reference Wilkins2010). The transport distances of stone raw material across the landscape can also shed light on the potential reach of exchange networks for goods and information (Ambrose & Lorenz, Reference Ambrose, Lorenz and Mellars1990). Because knapping is learned in a social environment, reduction strategies and other aspects of lithic technology (beyond point form) can also serve as proxies for inter-group connectedness (Gamble, Reference Gamble1999).

In their meta-analysis of MSA technological variability, Mackay et al. (Reference Mackay, Stewart and Chase2014) considered the spatial and temporal patterns of various lithic technological systems (provisioning, raw material selection, flaking systems and implement types) to identify past periods of inter-regional coalescence (interaction) and fragmentation. They suggest that during MIS 4, which is a glacial period dated to ~57–71 ka, there was increased interconnectedness, or ‘coalescence’ of human groups across southern Africa (Table 2). In contrast, during MIS 5 and 3 (interglacials dated to ~71–130 ka and ~29–57 ka, respectively), human populations were more fragmented. In other words, during some periods in the MSA, early human populations did not share lithic technological information across large areas or for long periods of time. Instead, local approaches thrived.

Table 2. Summary of population connectedness during the Middle and Later Stone Age in South Africa based on lithic technological evidence, after Mackay et al. (Reference Mackay, Stewart and Chase2014)

The archaeological record of MIS 3, in particular, shows a mosaic, patchy pattern of technological variability consistent with fragmented populations. In South Africa, assemblages dated to this time variability exhibit evidence for bipolar percussion, bladelets and microliths, formal tool types with repeated forms such as hollow-based points, unifacial points, and scrapers and knives, as well as point and blade-based assemblages with rare retouched forms (Lombard et al., Reference Lombard, Wadley, Deacon, Wurz, Parsons, Mohapi and Mitchell2012; Wurz, Reference Wurz2013, Reference Wurz2019). Some regional patterns are hinted at, but are difficult to define, and overall this time period shows highly variable lithic technologies across South Africa. A similar pattern of irregular temporo-spatial distribution of lithic assemblage traits also characterizes the Late Pleistocene in East Africa (Ranhorn & Tryon, Reference Ranhorn and Tryon2018; Tryon & Faith, Reference Tryon and Faith2013).

Fragmentation can be considered a result of learner-driven innovation on a large scale. It reflects weaker homogenizing processes over time and across space, with less social emphasis on imitation, high-fidelity information transfer and the passing down of technological traditions. This is not to say that top-down processes were absent; they must have been present to maintain some intra-assemblage and intra-regional similarities, but the social rules governing top-down information transfer must have also been relaxed enough to permit the innovation and spread of new ideas about lithic reduction.

Convergence

Humans often converge on similar solutions to similar problems, without cultural transmission. Convergence in stone tool form and reduction strategies has been documented across the world for many time periods (e.g. Kuhn & Zwyns, Reference Kuhn and Zwyns2018; Clarkson et al., Reference Clarkson, Hiscock, Mackay and Shipton2018; Will et al., Reference Will, Mackay and Phillips2015; Sharon, Reference Sharon, Overmann and Coolidge2019; Eren et al., Reference Eren, Patten, O'Brien and Meltzer2013, Reference Eren, Buchanan, O'Brien, O’Brien, Buchanan and Eren2018; Wilkins, Reference Wilkins, O'Brien, Buchanan and Eren2018; Wilkins & Chazan, Reference Wilkins and Chazan2012; Lycett, Reference Lycett2009; Jennings & Smallwood, Reference Jennings, Smallwood, O’Brien, Buchanan and Eren2018; Adler et al., Reference Adler, Wilkinson, Blockley, Mark, Pinhasi, Schmidt-Magee and Gasparian2014), and convergent evolutionary processes in lithic technology are receiving increasingly more attention from methodological and theoretical perspectives (O'Brien et al., Reference O'Brien, Buchanan and Eren2018; Groucutt, Reference Groucutt2020).

Convergence can reflect learner-driven innovation; similar technological solutions result from independent, trial-and-error learning to solve similar problems, rather than through cultural transmission. It can represent a much larger scale than the concept of emulation described above. It can involve individuals, but also groups at various scales. Individuals can converge on a solution, for example, and so can groups or populations.

Archaeologically, convergence can be challenging to empirically demonstrate. Early twentieth-century Diffusionist approaches tended to overinterpret similarity across geographic space as an indication of spreading populations and ideas. Convergence is now more commonly accepted as a potential explanation for similarity in form between very distant geographic locales. Some researchers draw from evolutionary ecology, employing cladistic approaches to examine evolutionary relationships in form and technological characteristics between different assemblages of stone tools (Lycett, Reference Lycett2009; O'Brien et al., Reference O'Brien, Darwent and Lyman2001; Buchanan & Collard, Reference Buchanan and Collard2007; O'Brien & Lyman, Reference O'Brien and Lyman2003). This can suggest different ‘lineages’ within populations of stone tools that are superficially similar in form (i.e. projectile points). Technological similarity between assemblages is sometimes used to support arguments for cultural connection. For example, a connection has been made between Clovis and Solutrean assemblages in North America and Europe, respectively. The argument is based on the observation that their characteristic bifacial points were produced using similar reduction strategies (Stanford & Bradley, Reference Stanford and Bradley2012; Lohse et al., Reference Lohse, Collins and Bradley2014). However, one of the components of this strategy, the removal of overshot thinning flakes, has been argued to be unintentional and occurring in different frequencies between the two assemblages, problematizing the proposed Clovis–Solutrean connection (Eren et al., Reference Eren, Patten, O'Brien and Meltzer2013, Reference Eren, Patten, O'Brien and Meltzer2014).

There are several potential examples of convergence in African MSA technologies (Figure 2). For example, the earliest evidence for blade production occurs roughly coeval in southern (Kathu Pan 1) and East Africa (Kapthurin sites) ~500 ka, and these blades were produced using different reduction strategies consistent with convergence (Wilkins & Chazan, Reference Wilkins and Chazan2012). ‘Nubian-like’ cores for the production of points from the Cedarberg region of southern Africa exhibit morphologies and technological characteristics similar to cores described in Egypt, such as those at Taramsa Hill (Will et al., Reference Will, Mackay and Phillips2015; Vermeersch et al., Reference Vermeersch, Paulissen, Van Peer, Stokes, Charlier, Stringer and Lindsay1998). In fact, similar kinds of cores, described generally as Levallois point cores, have been reported at other southern African MSA sites (Thompson et al., Reference Thompson, Williams and Minichillo2010; Wilkins & Chazan, Reference Wilkins and Chazan2012; Wilkins et al., Reference Wilkins, Brown, Oestmo, Pereira, Ranhorn, Schoville and Marean2017; Wurz, Reference Wurz2002). While impossible to prove conclusively, convergence in core reduction strategy is a more likely explanation for similarity between regions ~6500 km apart than cultural transmission. Archer et al. (Reference Archer, Pop, Gunz and McPherron2016) used three-dimensional geometric morphometrics to examine bifacial point variability in Still Bay-designated assemblages, finding a high degree of variability between points in the north-east and south-west of South Africa. They suggest that convergence is the most likely explanation for the occurrence of bifacial points across the region. ‘Miniaturization’ of stone tools can be advantageous in a variety of circumstances related to raw material availability and mobility, among others (Pargeter & Shea, Reference Pargeter and Shea2019). The relative importance of the production of bladelets and backed pieces fluctuates through the Late Pleistocene and Holocene across many regions of Africa and beyond (Pargeter & Shea, Reference Pargeter and Shea2019; Clarkson et al., Reference Clarkson, Hiscock, Mackay and Shipton2018). Backed pieces are known at Howiesons Poort-designated sites in South Africa dated to ~100–50 ka (Lombard et al., Reference Lombard, Wadley, Deacon, Wurz, Parsons, Mohapi and Mitchell2012; Porraz et al., Reference Porraz, Parkington, Rigaud, Miller, Poggenpoel, Tribolo and Texier2013; Jacobs et al., Reference Jacobs, Roberts, Galbraith, Deacon, Grun, Mackay and Wadley2008; Lukich et al., Reference Lukich, Porat, Faershtein, Cowling and Chazan2019) and also from Enkapune ya Muto and other sites in East Africa by ~50 ka (Ranhorn & Tryon, Reference Ranhorn and Tryon2018; Ambrose, Reference Ambrose2002).

Figure 2. Examples of technological convergence in the African Middle Stone Age.

Variability, non-conformity and Homo sapiens

The observations presented here highlight lithic technological variation at multiple scales – intra- and inter-assemblage and intra- and inter-regional – in the African MSA. Indeed, variation is the norm in the lithic technology of Homo sapiens (Shea, Reference Shea2011), and its meaning needs consideration as much as homogeneity does. The current literature on cultural transmission and innovation in lithic technology focuses largely on homogeneity, perhaps owing in part to academic biases about knowledge transmission, and is thus inadequate for understanding this variability. I suggest here that learner-driven innovation and bottom-up social learning processes contributed to the observed patterns of variability, and that the archaeological record of early Homo sapiens shows abundant evidence for this. Indeed, the Middle Range theory for how technological variations are developed and spread requires more rigorous development, especially through knapping and social transmission experiments (Ranhorn et al., Reference Ranhorn, Pargeter and Premo2020). Based on the variable archaeological record of early Homo sapiens discussed here, continued investigations should not continue to focus solely on top-down approaches like active teaching, imitation and high-fidelity cultural transmission. They should also consider why, when and how knappers choose to not conform.

Human behaviour is complex, and it is reasonable to assume that we relied on a variety of learning mechanisms through the entirety of our evolutionary history. There may have been variable emphasis on top-down and bottom-up processes through time and across space, and dependent on the type of technology considered (Wilkins, Reference Wilkins, Deane-Drummond and Fuentes2020). For example, the production of backed bladelets and their probable incorporation into high-velocity projectile systems during the Howiesons Poort (Lombard & Pargeter, Reference Lombard and Pargeter2008; Pargeter, Reference Pargeter2007; Schoville et al., Reference Schoville, Wilkins, Ritzman, Oestmo and Brown2017) may have relied more heavily on high-fidelity information transfer than other kinds of Stone Age technology. Humans knap and produce stone tools in a huge variety of contexts around the world. Top-down social learning processes, at a cost to the teacher, can ensure the transmission of known fitness-enhancing information and skills. On the other hand, independent learning and learner-driven innovation offer some advantages that include reduced teaching costs and quicker rates of change in fluctuating environments. Thus, several factors – the nature of the goal, environmental variability, group and population size, subsistence, settlement, socio-cultural factors – may influence the relative advantages of top-down and bottom-up processes of cultural transmission.

Experimental work has hinted that the relative effectiveness of imitation vs. emulation in lithic reduction is goal-dependent. The results and observations of the skill transmission experiment by Putt et al. (Reference Putt, Woods and Franciscus2014) suggest that emulation is as effective as imitation for producing handaxes of a particular form, but that emulation can be more effective than imitation for producing large, thin flakes.

Ethnographic research on hunter–gatherer children adds support to the idea that role of adults in teaching is context dependent (Lew-Levy et al., Reference Lew-Levy, Kissler, Boyette, Crittenden, Mabulla and Hewlett2020). Child-to-child teaching is more frequent than adult-to-child teaching for many activities in many hunter–gatherer societies (Lew-Levy et al., Reference Lew-Levy, Reckin, Lavi, Cristóbal-Azkarate and Ellis-Davies2017, Reference Lew-Levy, Kissler, Boyette, Crittenden, Mabulla and Hewlett2020). Lew-Levy et al. (Reference Lew-Levy, Kissler, Boyette, Crittenden, Mabulla and Hewlett2020) found significant differences in teaching between two groups – the Hadza (Tanzania) and BaYaka (Congo). For the BaYaka, 5-year-olds receive almost as many hourly teaching events from other 5-year-olds as they do from 25-year-olds. The same pattern is not true for the Hadza, and the authors attribute this to differences in settlement and subsistence (Lew-Levy et al., Reference Lew-Levy, Kissler, Boyette, Crittenden, Mabulla and Hewlett2020).

Learners, children and youth, can be active innovative agents, contributing more than simply acquiring and passing on the knowledge of the generation before them. This must be considered when interpreting lithic assemblage variability and investigating social transmission in the past. From personal experience living in the modern world, we know that some of our richest learning experiences include independent learning, experiential learning, experimentation and creativity. We know that youth often drive technological change, fashion change and social change (e.g. smartphone apps), and we know that younger generations challenge older ones. Counter to the narrative that high-fidelity information transfer defines us, our ability to learn independently, to avoid conformity, and to play, rather than to simply follow top-down influences, also significantly influenced the success of human societies in the past.

Acknowledgements

Thank you to Michelle Langley, Felix Riede and April Nowell for the invitation to contribute to this special issue and the inspiration to think about the active, innovative role of youth in prehistory. I also thank two anonymous reviewers for critically reading the manuscript and suggesting valuable improvements.

Funding

Jayne Wilkins is a recipient an Australian Research Council Discovery Early Career Research Award (DE 190100160), a National Research Foundation (South Africa) Centre of Excellence (COE) in Palaeosciences Operational Grant, and a National Research Foundation (South Africa) Research Development Grant for Y-rated Researchers.

Data availability statement

As a conceptual study, this manuscript does not rely on shareable data, code or other resources.

Conflict of interest statement

The author reports no conflict of interest.

References

Adler, D. S., Wilkinson, K. N., Blockley, S., Mark, D. F., Pinhasi, R., Schmidt-Magee, B. A., … Gasparian, B. (2014). Early Levallois technology and the Lower to Middle Paleolithic transition in the Southern Caucasus. Science 345(6204), 16091613.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ambrose, S. H. (2002). Small things remembered: Origins of Early Microlithic industries in Sub-Saharan Africa. Archeological Papers of the American Anthropological Association 12(1), 929.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ambrose, S. H., & Lorenz, K. G. (1990). Social and ecological models for the Middle Stone Age in southern Africa. In Mellars, P. (Ed.), The emergence of modern humans. Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Andrefsky, W. (2005). Lithics: Macroscopic approaches to analysis: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Archer, W., Pop, C. M., Gunz, P., & McPherron, S. P. (2016). What is Still Bay? Human biogeography and bifacial point variability. Journal of Human Evolution, 97, 5872.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arthur, K. W. (2018). The lives of stone tools: Crafting the status, skill, and identity of flintknappers. University of Arizona Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bentley, R. (2018). The transparency of imitation versus emulation in the Middle Paleolithic. In O’Brien, M., Buchanan, B. and Eren, M.. (Eds.), Convergent Evolution in Stone-Tool Technology (pp. 4759).Google Scholar
Bishop, W. W., & Clark, J. D. (1967). Background to evolution in Africa. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (2005). The origin and evolution of cultures. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bril, B., Rein, R., Nonaka, T., Wenban-Smith, F., & Dietrich, G. (2010). The role of expertise in tool use: Skill differences in functional action adaptations to task constraints. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(4), 825.Google ScholarPubMed
Brown, K. S., Marean, C. W., Herries, A. I. R., Jacobs, Z., Tribolo, C., Braun, D., … Bernatchez, J. (2009). Fire as an engineering tool of early modern humans. Science, 325(5942), 859862.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, K. S., Marean, C. W., Jacobs, Z., Schoville, B. J., Oestmo, S., Fisher, E. C., … Matthews, T. (2012). An early and enduring advanced technology originating 71,000 years ago in South Africa. Nature, 491(7425), 590593.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buchanan, B., & Collard, M. (2007). Investigating the peopling of North America through cladistic analyses of Early Paleoindian projectile points. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 26(3), 366393.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caldwell, C. A., & Millen, A. E. (2009). Social learning mechanisms and cumulative cultural evolution: Is imitation necessary? Psychological Science, 20(12), 14781483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carpenter, M., Akhtar, N., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Fourteen- through 18-month-old infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental actions. Infant Behavior and Development, 21(2), 315330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cataldo, D. M., Migliano, A. B., & Vinicius, L. (2018). Speech, stone tool-making and the evolution of language. PLoS ONE, 13(1), e0191071.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charbonneau, M. (2015). All innovations are equal, but some more than others: (Re)integrating modification processes to the origins of cumulative culture. Biological Theory, 10(4), 322335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chazan, M. (1995). The language hypothesis for the Middle-to-Upper Paleolithic transistion. An examination based on a multiregional lithic analysis. Current Anthropology, 36, 749768.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, J. D. (1992). African and Asian perspectives on the origins of modern humans. Philosophical Transactions: Biological Sciences, 337, 201215.Google ScholarPubMed
Clark, J. D., Cole, G. H., Isaac, G. L., & Kleindienst, M. R. (1966). Precision and definition in African archaeology. South African Archaeological Bulletin, 21, 114121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clarkson, C. (2017). Teaching complex flint knapping strategies in the classroom using ‘potato knapping’. Lithic Technology, 42(4), 155160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clarkson, C., Hiscock, P., Mackay, A., & Shipton, C. (2018). Small, sharp, and standardized: Global convergence in backed-microlith technology. In Convergent evolution in stone-tool technology (pp. 175200). MIT Press.Google Scholar
Eren, M., Buchanan, B., & O'Brien, M. (2018). Why convergence should be a potential hypothesis for the emergence and occurrence of stone-tool form and production processes: An illustration using replication. In O’Brien, M., Buchanan, B. and Eren, M. (Eds.), Convergent Evolution in Stone-Tool Technology (pp. 6171).Google Scholar
Eren, M., Kollecker, M., Clarkson, C., & Bradley, B. (2010). Dynamic approaches to teaching lithic technology. Ethnoarchaeology, 2(2), 223234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eren, M. I., Patten, R. J., O'Brien, M. J., & Meltzer, D. J. (2013). Refuting the technological cornerstone of the Ice-Age Atlantic crossing hypothesis. Journal of Archaeological Science, 40(7), 29342941.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eren, M. I., Patten, R. J., O'Brien, M. J., & Meltzer, D. J. (2014). More on the rumor of ‘intentional overshot flaking’ and the purported Ice-Age atlantic crossing. Lithic Technology, 39(1), 5563.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foley, R., & Lahr, M. M. (2003). On stony ground: Lithic technology, human evolution, and the emergence of culture. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 12(3), 109122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foley, R. A., & Lahr, M. M. (1997). Mode 3 technologies and the evolution of modern humans. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 7, 336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frieman, C. J. (2013). Innovation and identity: The language and reality of prehistoric imitation and technological change. Hybrid Material Culture: The Archaeology of Syncretism and Ethnogenesis, 318341.Google Scholar
Gamble, C. (1999). The Palaeolithic societies of Europe. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gärdenfors, P., & Högberg, A. (2017). The archaeology of teaching and the evolution of Homo docens. Current Anthropology, 58(2), 188208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gergely, G., Bekkering, H., & Király, I. (2002). Rational imitation in preverbal infants. Nature, 415(6873), 755755.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geribàs, N., Mosquera, M., & Vergès, J. M. (2010). What novice knappers have to learn to become expert stone toolmakers. Journal of Archaeological Science, 37(11), 28572870.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Groucutt, H. S. (Ed.) (2020). Culture history and convergent evolution: Can we detect populations in prehistory? Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harmand, S., Lewis, J. E., Feibel, C. S., Lepre, C. J., Prat, S., Lenoble, A., … Roche, H. (2015). 3.3-Million-year-old stone tools from Lomekwi 3, West Turkana, Kenya. Nature, 521(7552), 310315.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henrich, J. (2017). The secret of our success: How culture is driving human evolution, domesticating our species, and making us smarter. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 6183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hiscock, P. (2014). Learning in lithic landscapes: A reconsideration of the hominid ‘toolmaking’ niche. Biological Theory, 9(1), 2741.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobs, Z., Roberts, R. G., Galbraith, R. F., Deacon, H. J., Grun, R., Mackay, A., … Wadley, L. (2008). Ages for the Middle Stone Age of Southern Africa: Implications for human behavior and dispersal. Science, 322(5902), 733735.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jennings, T., & Smallwood, A. (2018). Clovis and Toyah: Convergent blade technologies on the southern plains periphery of North America. In O’Brien, M., Buchanan, B. and Eren, M. (Eds.), Convergent Evolution in Stone-Tool Technology (pp. 229251).Google Scholar
Johnson, L. L. (1978). A history of flint-knapping experimentation, 1838–1976. Current Anthropology, 19(2), 337372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kline, M. A. (2015). How to learn about teaching: An evolutionary framework for the study of teaching behavior in humans and other animals. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 38, e31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kooyman, B. P. (2000). Understanding stone tools and archaeological sites. UNM Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, S. L., & Zwyns, N. (2018). Convergence and continuity in the Initial Upper Paleolithic of Eurasia. In Convergent evolution in stone-tool technology. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Lancy, D. F. (2010). Learning ‘from nobody’: The limited role of teaching in folk models of children's development. Childhood in the Past, 3(1), 79106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langley, M. C., Benítez-Burraco, A., & Kempe, V. (2020). Playing with language, creating complexity: Has play contributed to the evolution of complex language? Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 29(1), 2940.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Langley, M. C., & Litster, M. (2018). Is it ritual? Or is it children?: Distinguishing consequences of play from ritual actions in the prehistoric archaeological record. Current Anthropology, 59(5), 616643.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lew-Levy, S., Kissler, S. M., Boyette, A. H., Crittenden, A. N., Mabulla, I. A., & Hewlett, B. S. (2020). Who teaches children to forage? Exploring the primacy of child-to-child teaching among Hadza and BaYaka Hunter–Gatherers of Tanzania and Congo. Evolution and Human Behavior, 41(1), 1222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lew-Levy, S., Reckin, R., Lavi, N., Cristóbal-Azkarate, J., & Ellis-Davies, K. (2017). How do hunter–gatherer children learn subsistence skills? Human Nature, 28(4), 367394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lohse, J. C., Collins, M. B., & Bradley, B. (2014). Controlled overshot flaking: A response to Eren, Patten, O'Brien, and Meltzer. Lithic Technology, 39(1), 4654.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lombard, M., & Pargeter, J. (2008). Hunting with Howiesons Poort segments: Pilot experimental study and the functional interpretation of archaeological tools. Journal of Archaeological Science, 35(9), 25232531.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lombard, M., Wadley, L. Y. N., Deacon, J., Wurz, S., Parsons, I., Mohapi, M., … Mitchell, P. (2012). South African and Lesotho Stone Age Sequence updated. The South African Archaeological Bulletin, 67(195), 123144.Google Scholar
Lukich, V., Porat, N., Faershtein, G., Cowling, S., & Chazan, M. (2019). New chronology and stratigraphy for Kathu Pan 6, South Africa. Journal of Paleolithic Archaeology, 2(3), 235257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lycett, S. J. (2009). Are Victoria West cores ‘proto-Levallois’? A phylogenetic assessment. Journal of Human Evolution, 56(2), 175191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mackay, A., Stewart, B. A., & Chase, B. M. (2014). Coalescence and fragmentation in the late Pleistocene archaeology of southernmost Africa. Journal of Human Evolution, 72, 2651.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marks, A., Hietala, H. J., & Williams, J. K. J. C. A. J. (2001). Tool standardization in the Middle and Upper Palaeolithic: A closer look (with comments). Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 11(1), 1744.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McBrearty, S., & Brooks, A. S. (2000). The revolution that wasn't: A new interpretation of the origin of modern human behavior. Journal of Human Evolution, 39(5), 453563.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgan, T. J., Uomini, N. T., Rendell, L. E., Chouinard-Thuly, L., Street, S. E., Lewis, H. M., … de la Torre, I. (2015). Experimental evidence for the co-evolution of hominin tool-making teaching and language. Nature Communications, 6(1), 18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mourre, V., Villa, P., & Henshilwood, C. S. (2010). Early use of pressure flaking on lithic artifacts at Blombos Cave, South Africa. Science, 330(6004), 659662.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nowell, A. (2016). Childhood, play and the evolution of cultural capacity in Neanderthals and modern humans. In The nature of culture (pp. 8797). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nowell, A. S. (2000). The archaeology of mind: Standardization and symmetry in lithics and their implications for the study of the evolution of the human mind. PhD thesis, University of Pennsylvania, ProQuest. AAI9976462.Google Scholar
O'Brien, M. J., Buchanan, B., & Eren, M. I. (2018). Convergent evolution in stone-tool technology. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Brien, M. J., Darwent, J., & Lyman, R. L. (2001). Cladistics is useful for reconstructing archaeological phylogenies: Palaeoindian points from the southeastern united states. Journal of Archaeological Science, 28(10), 11151136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Brien, M. J., & Lyman, R. L. (2003). Cladistics and archaeology. University of Utah Press.Google Scholar
Pargeter, J. (2007). Howiesons Poort segments as hunting weapons: Experiments with replicated projectiles. The South African Archaeological Bulletin, 62(186), 147153.Google Scholar
Pargeter, J., Khreisheh, N., & Stout, D. (2019). Understanding stone tool-making skill acquisition: Experimental methods and evolutionary implications. Journal of Human Evolution, 133, 146166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pargeter, J., & Shea, J. J. (2019). Going big versus going small: Lithic miniaturization in hominin lithic technology. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 28(2), 7285.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patterson, L. W. (1981). Instruction in lithic technology in US universities. North American Archaeologist, 2(1), 1924.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pellegrini, A. D., Dupuis, D., & Smith, P. K. (2007). Play in evolution and development. Developmental Review, 27(2), 261276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pleurdeau, D. (2006). Human technical behavior in the African Middle Stone Age: The lithic assemblage of Porc-Epic Cave (Dire Dawa, Ethiopia). African Archaeological Review, 22, 177197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Porraz, G., Parkington, J. E., Rigaud, J.-P., Miller, C. E., Poggenpoel, C., Tribolo, C., … Texier, P.-J. (2013). The MSA sequence of Diepkloof and the history of southern African Late Pleistocene populations. Journal of Archaeological Science, 40(9), 35423552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Potts, R. (1998). Variability selection in hominid evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology, 7, 8196.3.0.CO;2-A>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putt, S. S., Wijeakumar, S., Franciscus, R. G., & Spencer, J. P. (2017). The functional brain networks that underlie Early Stone Age tool manufacture. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(6), 18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putt, S. S., Woods, A. D., & Franciscus, R. G. (2014). The role of verbal interaction during experimental bifacial stone tool manufacture. Lithic Technology, 39(2), 96112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ranhorn, K. L. (2017). Cultural transmission and lithic technology in Middle Stone Age eastern Africa. The George Washington University,Google Scholar
Ranhorn, K. L., Pargeter, J.Premo, L. S., & PAST Network Collaborators. (2020). Investigating the evolution of human social learning through collaborative experimental archaeology. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 29, 5355.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ranhorn, K., & Tryon, C. A. (2018). New radiocarbon dates from Nasera Rockshelter (Tanzania): Implications for studying spatial patterns in Late Pleistocene technology. Journal of African Archaeology, 16(2), 211222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reber, A. S. (1989). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118(3), 219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Riede, F., Johannsen, N. N., Högberg, A., Nowell, A., & Lombard, M. (2018). The role of play objects and object play in human cognitive evolution and innovation. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 27(1), 4659.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scerri, E. M. L., Gravina, B., Blinkhorn, J., & Delagnes, A. (2016). Can lithic attribute analyses identify discrete reduction trajectories? A quantitative study using refitted lithic sets. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory, 23(2), 669691.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schillinger, K., Mesoudi, A., & Lycett, S. J. (2015). The impact of imitative versus emulative learning mechanisms on artifactual variation: implications for the evolution of material culture. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(6), 446455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schillinger, K., Mesoudi, A., & Lycett, S. J. (2016). Copying error, evolution, and phylogenetic signal in artifactual traditions: An experimental approach using ‘model artifacts’. Journal of Archaeological Science, 70, 2334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schoville, B. J., Wilkins, J., Ritzman, T., Oestmo, S., & Brown, K. S. (2017). The performance of heat-treated silcrete backed pieces in actualistic and controlled complex projectile experiments. Journal of Archaeological Science: Reports, 14, 302317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seger, C. A. (1994). Implicit learning. Psychological Bulletin, 115(2), 163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sharon, G. (2019). Early convergent cultural evolution. In Overmann, K. A. & Coolidge, F. L. (Eds.), Squeezing minds from stones: Cognitive archaeology and the evolution of the human mind. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Shea, J. (2011). Homo sapiens is as Homo sapiens was: Behavioral variability versus ‘behavioral modernity’ in Paleolithic Archaeology. Current Anthropology, 52(1), 135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shea, J. J. (2006). Child's play: Reflections on the invisibility of children in the paleolithic record. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 15(6), 212216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shea, J. J. (2014). Sink the Mousterian? Named stone tool industries (NASTIES) as obstacles to investigating hominin evolutionary relationships in the Later Middle Paleolithic Levant. Quaternary International, 350, 169179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shea, J. J. (2015). Making and using stone tools: Advice for learners and teachers and insights for archaeologists. Lithic Technology, 40(3), 231248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shennan, S. (2002). Genes, memes, and human history: Darwinian archaeology and cultural evolution. London: Thames & Hudson.Google Scholar
Shipton, C. (2019). The evolution of social transmission in the Acheulean. Squeezing minds from Stones. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Soriano, S., Villa, P., Delagnes, A., Degano, I., Pollarolo, L., Lucejko, J. J., … Wadley, L. (2015). The Still Bay and Howiesons Poort at Sibudu and Blombos: Understanding Middle Stone Age technologies. PLoS ONE, 10(7), e0131127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soriano, S., Villa, P., & Wadley, L. (2007). Blade technology and tool forms in the Middle Stone Age of South Africa: The Howiesons Poort and post-Howiesons Poort at Rose Cottage Cave. Journal of Archaeological Science, 34(5), 681703.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanford, D. J., & Bradley, B. A. (2012). Across Atlantic ice: The origin of America's Clovis culture. University of California Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sterelny, K. (2012). The evolved apprentice: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stout, D. (2002). Skill and Cognition in Stone Tool Production: An Ethnographic Case Study from Irian Jaya. Current Anthropology, 43, 693722.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stout, D. (2018). Archaeology and the evolutionary neuroscience of language: The technological pedagogy hypothesis. Interaction Studies, 19(1–2), 256271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stout, D. & Hecht, E. E. (2017). Evolutionary neuroscience of cumulative culture. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(30), 78617868.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stout, D., Hecht, E., Khreisheh, N., Bradley, B., & Chaminade, T. (2015). Cognitive demands of Lower Paleolithic toolmaking. PLoS ONE, 10(4).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stout, D., Rogers, M. J., Jaeggi, A. V., & Semaw, S. (2019). Archeology and the origins of human cumulative culture. Current Anthropology, 60(3), 000.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stout, D., Toth, N., Schick, K., & Chaminade, T. (2008). Neural correlates of Early Stone Age toolmaking: Technology, language and cognition in human evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363(1499), 19391949.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Subiaul, F., Patterson, E. M., & Barr, R. (2016). The cognitive structure of goal emulation during the preschool years. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 34, 132149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sun, R., & Zhang, X. (2004). Top-down versus bottom-up learning in cognitive skill acquisition. Cognitive Systems Research, 5(1), 6389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tehrani, J. J., & Riede, F. (2008). Towards an archaeology of pedagogy: Learning, teaching and the generation of material culture traditions. World Archaeology, 40(3), 316331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tennie, C., Braun, D. R., Premo, L. S., & McPherron, S. P. (2016). The Island Test for cumulative culture in the Paleolithic. In Haidle, N. M., Conard, J. N., & Bolus, M. (Eds.), The nature of culture: Based on an interdisciplinary symposium ‘The Nature of Culture’, Tübingen, Germany (pp. 121133). Springer Netherlands.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tennie, C., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Ratcheting up the ratchet: On the evolution of cumulative culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1528), 24052415.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thompson, E., Williams, H. M., & Minichillo, T. (2010). Middle and late Pleistocene Middle Stone Age lithic technology from Pinnacle Point 13B (Mossel Bay, Western Cape Province, South Africa). Journal of Human Evolution, 59(3–4), 358377.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2016). The ontogeny of cultural learning. Current Opinion in Psychology, 8, 14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C., & Ratner, H. H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 495511.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tryon, C. A. (2006). ‘Early’ Middle Stone Age lithic technology of the Kapthurin Formation (Kenya). Current Anthropology, 47, 367375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tryon, C. A., & Faith, J. T. (2013). Variability in the Middle Stone Age of Eastern Africa. Current Anthropology, 54(S8), S234S254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tryon, C. A., & McBrearty, S. (2006). Tephrostratigraphy of the Bedded Tuff Member (Kapthurin Formation, Kenya) and the nature of archaeological change in the later Middle Pleistocene. Quaternary Research, 65(3), 492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tryon, C. A., McBrearty, S., & Texier, P.-J. (2005). Levallois lithic technology from the Kapthurin Formation, Kenya: Acheulian Origin and Middle Stone Age diversity. African Archaeological Review, 22(4), 199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vermeersch, P. M., Paulissen, E., Van Peer, P., Stokes, S., Charlier, C., Stringer, C., & Lindsay, W.. (1998). A Middle Palaeolithic burial of a modern human at Taramsa Hill, Egypt. Antiquity, 72, 475484.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wadley, L. (2001). What is cultural modernity? A general view and a South African perspective from Rose Cottage Cave. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 11, 201221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wadley, L. (2010). Compound-adhesive manufacture as a behavioral proxy for complex cognition in the Middle Stone Age. Current Anthropology, 51(S1), S111S119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wasielewski, H. (2014). Imitation is necessary for cumulative cultural evolution in an unfamiliar, opaque task. Human Nature, 25(1), 161179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weedman, K. (2006). An ethnoarchaeological study of hafting and stone tool diversity among the Gamo of Ethiopia. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory, 13(3), 188237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whiten, A., McGuigan, N., Marshall-Pescini, S., & Hopper, L. M. (2009). Emulation, imitation, over-imitation and the scope of culture for child and chimpanzee. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1528), 24172428.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whittaker, J. C. (1994). Flintknapping: Making & understanding stone tools. University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
Wiessner, P. (1983). Style and social information in Kalahari San projectile points. American Antiquity, 48, 253276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilkins, J. (2010). Style, Symboling, and Interaction in Middle Stone Age Society. Vis-à-vis: Explorations in Anthropology, 10, 102125.Google Scholar
Wilkins, J. (2018). The Point is the Point: Emulative social learning and weapon manufacture in the Middle Stone Age of South Africa. In O'Brien, M., Buchanan, B., & Eren, M. (Eds.), Convergent evolution and stone tool technology (pp. 153174). MIT PressGoogle Scholar
Wilkins, J. (2020). Archaeological evidence for human social learning and sociality in the Middle Stone Age of South Africa. In Deane-Drummond, C. & Fuentes, A. (Eds.), Theology and evolutionary anthropology: Dialogues in wisdom, humility, and grace. Routledge.Google Scholar
Wilkins, J., Brown, K. S., Oestmo, S., Pereira, T., Ranhorn, K. L., Schoville, B. J., & Marean, C. W. (2017). Lithic technological responses to Late Pleistocene glacial cycling at Pinnacle Point Site 5–6, South Africa. PLoS ONE, 12(3), e0174051.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilkins, J., & Chazan, M. (2012). Blade production ~500 thousand years ago at Kathu Pan 1, South Africa: Support for a multiple origins hypothesis for early Middle Pleistocene blade technologies. Journal of Archaeological Science, 39(6), 18831900.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilkins, J., Pollarolo, L., & Kuman, K. (2010). Prepared core reduction at the site of Kudu Koppie in northern South Africa: Temporal patterns across the Earlier and Middle Stone Age boundary. Journal of Archaeological Science, 37(6), 12791292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilkins, J., Schoville, B. J., Brown, K. S., & Chazan, M. (2012). Evidence for early hafted hunting technology. Science, 338, 942946.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Will, M., Mackay, A., & Phillips, N. (2015). Implications of Nubian-like core reduction systems in southern Africa for the identification of early modern human dispersals. PLoS ONE, 10(6), e0131824.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willoughby, P. (2007). The evolution of modern humans in Africa: A comprehensive guide. AltaMira Press.Google Scholar
Wurz, S. (2002). Variability in the Middle Stone Age lithic sequence, 115,000–60,000 years ago at Klasies River, South Africa. Journal of Archaeological Science, 29(9), 10011015.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wurz, S. (2013). Technological trends in the Middle Stone Age of South Africa between MIS 7 and MIS 3. Current Anthropology, 54(S8), S305S319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wurz, S. (2019). Human evolution, archaeology and the South African Stone Age landscape during the last 100,000 years. In The Geography of South Africa (pp. 125132). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Summary of bottom-up vs. top-down social learning processes and lithic technological correlates proposed here.

Figure 1

Table 1. Summary of MSA assemblages that document coeval diverse core reduction strategies

Figure 2

Table 2. Summary of population connectedness during the Middle and Later Stone Age in South Africa based on lithic technological evidence, after Mackay et al. (2014)

Figure 3

Figure 2. Examples of technological convergence in the African Middle Stone Age.