Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2brh9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T04:54:16.211Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Synergy between callous–unemotional traits and aggression in preschool children: Cross-informant and cross-cultural replication in the UK Wirral Child Health and Development Study, and the Colombian La Sabana Parent–Child Study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 March 2021

D. Obando
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Universidad de La Sabana, Chia, Colombia
J. Hill*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Clinical Language Sciences, University of Reading, Reading, UK
H. Sharp
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, Institute of Health & Life Sciences, University of Liverpool, Liverpool, UK
A. Pickles
Affiliation:
Department of Biostatistics & Health Informatics, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, Kings College London, London, UK
L. Fisher
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, Institute of Health & Life Sciences, University of Liverpool, Liverpool, UK
N. Wright
Affiliation:
Department of Biostatistics & Health Informatics, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, Kings College London, London, UK
*
Author for Correspondence: Jonathan Hill, Department of Psychology and Clinical Language Sciences, University of Reading, Reading, UK; E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Incremental prediction of aggression from callous–unemotional (CU) traits is well established, but cross-cultural replication and studies of young children are needed. Little is understood about the contribution of CU traits in children who are already aggressive. We addressed these issues in prospective studies in the United Kingdom and Colombia. In a UK epidemiological cohort, CU traits and aggression were assessed at age 3.5 years, and aggression at 5.0 years by mothers (N = 687) and partners (N = 397). In a Colombian general population sample, CU traits were assessed at age 3.5 years and aggression at 3.5 and 5.0 years by mother report (N = 220). Analyses consistently showed prediction of age-5.0 aggression by age-3.5 CU traits controlling for age-3.5 aggression. Associations between age-3.5 CU traits and age-5.0 aggression were moderated by aggression at 3.5 years, with UK interaction terms, same informant, β = .07 p = .014 cross-informant, β = .14 p = .002, and in Colombia, β = .09 p = .128. The interactions arose from stronger associations between CU traits and later aggression in those already aggressive. Our findings with preschoolers replicated across culturally diverse settings imply a major role for CU traits in the maintenance and amplification of already established aggression, and cast doubt on their contribution to its origins.

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Children with aggressive and disruptive behavior “conduct” problems who also show lack of remorse, absence of empathy with others, uncaring attitudes, and lack of concern about the consequences of one's own performance, “callous–unemotional” (CU) traits, differ in many respects from other children with conduct problems (CP) (Frick, Ray, Thornton, & Kahn, Reference Frick, Ray, Thornton and Kahn2014; Frick & White, Reference Frick and White2008). For example, the genetic contribution to their disorder is stronger (Henry, Pingault, Boivin, Rijsdijk, & Viding, Reference Henry, Pingault, Boivin, Rijsdijk and Viding2016; Viding, Blair, Moffitt, & Plomin, Reference Viding, Blair, Moffitt and Plomin2005), and they are less sensitive to punitive parenting (Dadds & Salmon, Reference Dadds and Salmon2003). Most studies of CU traits have been of children and adolescents. Furthermore, with few exceptions, studies of CU traits have been conducted in countries characterized by the World Bank as high-income countries (HICs), and it is yet to be established whether the findings generalize across cultures and socioeconomic conditions. Hypotheses regarding the mechanisms whereby CU traits create vulnerability for aggression envisage both that they may have a role in the emergence of aggression, or in perpetuating it in children who are already aggressive. In this study we examined in preschool children the incremental validity of two measures of CU traits widely used in older children and across contrasting cultures. We also tested the hypothesis that CU traits are associated with increasing child aggression specifically among children who are already aggressive using cross-cultural and cross-informant tests of robustness.

Given that “life course persistent” CP, which are associated with a wide range of adverse outcomes in adolescence and adulthood, typically start during the preschool period (Odgers et al., Reference Odgers, Moffitt, Broadbent, Dickson, Hancox, Harrington and Caspi2008), it is important to understand the role of CU traits in early-onset CP. This requires valid measurement of CU traits in young children. Several studies have provided support for validity in 3-year-olds based on incremental prediction of CP over time after accounting for baseline CP (Waller, Hyde, Grabell, Alves, & Olson, Reference Waller, Hyde, Grabell, Alves and Olson2015; Waller et al., Reference Waller, Dishion, Shaw, Gardner, Wilson and Hyde2016; but see Willoughby, Mills-Koonce, Gottfredson, & Wagner, Reference Willoughby, Mills-Koonce, Gottfredson and Wagner2014). However, evidence regarding measures widely used in older children, is limited. Furthermore most studies of childhood CP come from HIC such as the United States of America, the Netherlands, Australia, Cyprus, United Kingdom, and Canada. Replication across HIC and low- and middle-income countries (LMIC) would add to the generalizability of findings. A search conducted for this paper using the term “callous–unemotional traits” yielded 720 publications of which 15 were from LMIC (11 from China), 2% of the total. We were not been able to identify any previous publications that compared effect sizes for the same analyses across HIC and LMIC. In this study, we sought to establish incremental prediction as a test of validity of two measures widely used in older children, the Antisocial Process Screening Device (APSD) and the Inventory of Callous Unemotional Traits (ICU) in a HIC and in a LMIC.

Commonly in studies of young children, where teacher reports are not available, measurement relies on parent report, leading to the possibility of associations inflated by common method variance where the same parent reports on the key variables. Therefore, in a further test of robustness of our findings we examined in the UK study, incremental prediction by maternal report of CU traits to paternal report of child aggression.

Deficits in recognition of, and responsiveness to, fear and sadness, possibly related to lack of eye contact in social interactions, are thought to underpin indifference to others’ distress in CU traits (Blair, Colledge, Murray, & Mitchell, Reference Blair, Colledge, Murray and Mitchell2001; Dadds, Jambrak, Pasalich, Hawes, & Brennan, Reference Dadds, Jambrak, Pasalich, Hawes and Brennan2011). This deficient response to distress has been proposed to lead to persistent aggressive behavior because the normal inhibitory effect of another persons’ distress when harming them is reduced or absent (Blair et al., Reference Blair, Colledge, Murray and Mitchell2001; Blair, Reference Blair2013). However, when we study CU traits in general population samples, in contrast to clinical samples where all the children have CP, there are some indications that elevated CU traits may occur in the absence of CP (Fanti, Reference Fanti2013; Fontaine, McCrory, Boivin, Moffitt, & Viding, Reference Fontaine, McCrory, Boivin, Moffitt and Viding2011; Rowe et al., Reference Rowe, Maughan, Moran, Ford, Briskman and Goodman2010; Wall, Frick, Fanti, Kimonis, & Lordos, Reference Wall, Frick, Fanti, Kimonis and Lordos2016).

Our understanding of the processes that lead some children with high CU traits to develop aggressive and antisocial behavior, while others do not, is limited. One possibility is that the risk for aggression arises from the combination of unresponsiveness to others’ distress seen in CU traits and a second source of vulnerability such as under-arousal reflected in low cortisol reactivity (Wright, Hill, Pickles, & Sharp, Reference Wright, Hill, Pickles and Sharp2019), low observed behavioral inhibition (Waller, Hyde, Baskin-Sommers, & Olson, Reference Waller, Hyde, Baskin-Sommers and Olson2017), parent-reported need for stimulation and grandiose traits (Colins et al., Reference Colins, Andershed, Frogner, Lopez-Romero, Veen and Andershed2014) or low mentalization (Taubner, White, Zimmermann, Fonagy, & Nolte, Reference Taubner, White, Zimmermann, Fonagy and Nolte2013). These direct tests of synergy conducted using moderator analyses, are consistent with findings from studies using latent-profile analyses which have shown that children with elevated CU traits in the absence of CP have lower levels of possible sources of additional individual and environmental vulnerabilities than those with both elevated CU and CP (Fanti, Reference Fanti2013; Wall et al., Reference Wall, Frick, Fanti, Kimonis and Lordos2016). A further, and as yet relatively unexplored, possibility is that CU traits create vulnerability for CP or aggression specifically among children who are already aggressive, for example by reducing their response to, or concern for, the harm they cause to others, which in turn decreases their likelihood of desisting their aggressive behavior. If that were the case, we would expect the association between CU traits and future aggression to be greatest in the presence of current aggression. If CU traits only contribute to aggression among children who are already aggressive, then we expect no association between CU traits and future aggression among nonaggressive children.

There has been limited attention paid to these possible mechanisms in preschool children, a key period for early intervention and the starting point of life-course persistent antisocial behavior. In a one-year prospective study of one group of children recruited at ages 4–6 years, and another at ages 7–9 years, synergistic effects of baseline CU traits and CP were seen in boys in the younger group, and in girls in the older group (Dadds, Fraser, Frost, & Hawes, Reference Dadds, Fraser, Frost and Hawes2005). These findings indicate that there may be age-related differences in these processes, highlighting the need for studies that examine the mechanisms at specific ages, and test for replicability of findings. In the studies reported here of preschool children we were able to ask whether there is a synergistic effect of CU traits and aggression, that is seen in preschool children, in two contrasting cultural and socio-economic settings, and in same and cross-informant analyses.

Method

Overview of method

Sampling, measures, and data analyses in Study 1 from the UK Wirral Child Health and Development Study are described first, followed by sampling, measures and analyses in Study 2 from the Colombian La Sabana Parent and Child Study. We then compare the effects from each study in Table 3 and Figure 1, showing associations between age-3.5 CU traits and age-5 aggression at three levels of initial aggression, and use a combined dataset to test whether the effects in the UK and Colombian studies are different.

Figure 1. The prospective association between age-3.5 callous–unemotional (CU) traits and age-5.0 aggression at three levels of age 3.5 aggression (at mean and 1 SD above and below mean) in the mother and partner report Wirral Child Health and Development Study (WCHADS) sample and mother report La Sabana Parent–Child Study.

Study 1

Participants and procedure

The Wirral Child Health and Development Study (WCHADS) is a prospective epidemiological study starting in pregnancy designed to identify the earliest origins of childhood CP. (Further information about the data and conditions for access are available at the University of Liverpool Research Data Catalogue: doi.org/10.17638/datacat.liverpool.ac.uk/564.) Ethical approval was granted by the Cheshire North and West Research Ethics committee and all women gave written informed consent at the point of recruitment in the antenatal clinic.

The cohort comprised 1,233 first-time expectant mothers recruited in pregnancy with a live, singleton baby for long-term follow-up post-birth (see Sharp et al., Reference Sharp, Pickles, Meaney, Marshall, Tibu and Hill2012 for a detailed account of sampling). The participants were identified from consecutive first-time mothers who booked for antenatal care at 12 weeks’ gestation between 12/02/2007 and 29/10/2008. The booking clinic was administered by the Wirral University Teaching Hospital, which was the sole provider of universal prenatal care on the Wirral Peninsula. Socioeconomic conditions on the Wirral range between the deprived inner city and affluent suburbs, but with low numbers from ethnic minorities. The mean age at recruitment was 26.8 years (SD = 5.8, range 18–51), 41.8% of the sample were in the most deprived quintile of UK neighborhoods (Noble et al., Reference Noble, Wright, Dibben, Smith, McLennan, Anttila and Lloyd2004) and 96.1% were White British. The analysis that follows focuses on 687 children (326 boys) who provided questionnaire data by maternal report at both the age 3.5 years (mean age = 41.70 months, SD = 2.33) and 5.0 years (mean age = 58.18 months, SD = 2.83) waves, and on the 397 on whom partner reports were available at 5.0 years. The maternal report sample did not differ from the whole cohort (N = 1,233) on maternal depression scores at 20 weeks (t(1,208) = 1.157, p = .247) but was less deprived (χ 2 (1, 1,233) = 19.502, p < .001) and had a lower maternal age at pregnancy (t(1,230) = −1.834, p < .001). The paternal report sample was less deprived (χ 2 (1, 1,233) = 9.690, p < .001), had a lower maternal age (t(1,230) = −6.697, p < .001) and lower depression scores (t(1,208) = 2.217, p = .027) in pregnancy compared to the whole sample. Of the 687, the majority (80%) were two parent families and 32% of mothers had university degrees.

Measures

Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach & Rescorla, Reference Achenbach and Rescorla2000). The parent report CBCL for children aged 1.5 to 5 years was used to assess aggressive behaviors at age 3.5 and 5.0 years. It has 99 items to assess internalizing and externalizing behaviors in the past 6 months, rated on a 3-point scale (0 = not true, 1 = somewhat true/sometimes true, and 2 = very true/often true). The sum of the 19 aggressive behavior items was used in the analyses. In addition to maternal reports at age 3.5 and 5.0 years, we collected reports from biological fathers or mothers’ current partner who had regular contact with the child at age 5.0. These are referred to as partner reports.

Antisocial Processes Screening Device (APSD; Frick & Hare, Reference Frick and Hare2001). The parent-report CU subscale was used to assess CU traits. This measure has been widely used in studies of CU traits in childhood, including a sample of children aged 3 years (Kimonis et al., Reference Kimonis, Frick, Boris, Smyke, Cornell, Farrell and Zeanah2006). The six items are rated on a 3-point scale (0 = not at all true, 1 = sometimes true, and 2 = definitely true). Internal consistency in this sample was α = .60, similar to that reported by Kimonis et al. (α = .54). In previous reports we have generated a factor score with more satisfactory psychometric properties by adding items from the CBCL aggression scale (Wright, Sharp, Pickles, & Hill, Reference Wright, Sharp, Pickles and Hill2017; Wright, Hill, Sharp, & Pickles, Reference Wright, Hill, Sharp and Pickles2018), but we did not use this here to avoid item overlap with our outcome measure across the UK and Colombian studies, CBCL aggression.

Confounders. Potential sources of mood-biased reporting at age 5 years were accounted for in mothers using the 20-item Centre for Epidemiological Studies – Depression (CES-D; Radloff, Reference Radloff1977) and in partners with the 12-item General Health Questionnaire (Goldberg & Williams, Reference Goldberg and Williams1988). Two indices of family demographic status were included as covariates: (a) socioeconomic status, which was derived from post code data using the English Index of Multiple Deprivation (IMD; Noble et al., Reference Noble, Wright, Dibben, Smith, McLennan, Anttila and Lloyd2004) and converted to quintile categories with a binary variable (1 = most deprived, 0 = all four other quintiles) used for analysis and (b) mother's age at pregnancy. Child sex (0 = female, 1 = male) was also included as a covariate.

Data analyses

SPSS v.24 was used for the statistical analyses. Following square root transformations for skewed variables, bivariate associations between the study variables were examined using Pearson, point-biserial and tetrachoric correlations. In validity-checking and hypothesis-testing analyses, regression models predicting age-5.0 aggression were examined in three blocks. Block 1 included demographic covariates, age-3.5 child aggression, and parental mood at age 5 years, Block 2 included age-3.5 CU traits, and Block 3 included the age-3.5 aggression by CU traits interaction; testing was carried out at each stage to assess whether the addition of a block significantly increased the explained variance. The age-3.5 CU Traits × Aggression interactions were then explored in each sample, by using online computational tools to calculate regions of significance (Roisman et al., Reference Roisman, Newman, Fraley, Haltigan, Groh and Haydon2012), and testing the simple slopes and intercepts at three levels of age-3.5 aggression: 1 SD above the mean, mean, and 1 SD below the mean. In Supplementary Appendix 1 and Table A3 we report the results of exploring the interaction with CU traits as the moderator.

Results

Bivariate associations between the transformed study variables and descriptive statistics for untransformed variables are presented in Supplementary Table A1 (Appendix).

Table 1 shows separate models for maternal report of CBCL aggression regressed on to Block 1 variables, then Block 2 after accounting for Block 1, and similarly Block 3 accounting for Block 1 and 2 variables, so that all the reported coefficients are directly interpretable. It can be seen that CU traits at age 3.5 years explained significant additional variance after accounting for contemporary aggression and after controlling for possible biasing effects of maternal mood on child report at age 5.0 years. There was also an age-3.5 CU Traits × Aggression interaction, which arose as predicted by a progressively stronger association between age-3.5 CU traits and age-5.0 aggression, with increasing aggression at age 3.5 years, as shown in Table 3 and Figure 1.

Table 1. Summary of linear regression models predicting maternal and partner report of aggression in the Wirral Child Health and Development Study (WCHADS)

Note: Block 1 effects were generated from a model with only Block 1, Block 2 effect from a model with Block1 plus Block 2, and Block 3 effect from a model with all three blocks.

Very similar effects were seen when father-reported CBCL aggression at age 5.0 years was regressed on to mother-reported age-3.5 CU traits and aggression (Tables 1 and 3, Figure 1). The regions of significance analysis indicated that the association between age-3.5 CU traits and age-5.0 aggression became significant at .67 SDs below the mean for mother-reported outcomes (equating to a score of 3 and above on age-3.5 aggression, N = 524/687 children) and .13 SDs below the mean for partner-reported outcome (equating to a score of 6 and above on age-3.5 aggression, N = 384/687). In analyses of the associations between age-3.5 and age-5.0 aggression at different levels of CU traits, shown in Supplementary Appendix 1 and Table A3, there were progressively stronger associations with increasing CU traits, which were nevertheless significant at all levels.

Study 2

Participants and procedure

The La Sabana Parent and Child Study participants were recruited through Facebook groups likely to be used by young mothers, such as “Latin Women League” and “More Moms Colombia”. It is estimated that 91% of those between the ages of 14 and 65 in Colombia access Facebook (Ministerio de Tecnologías de la Información y las Comunicaciones, 2018). Parents who responded to the online study information (n = 344) were contacted to discuss participation. Of these, 40 were excluded as the children did not meet the age inclusion criteria of age 3–4 years. Of the remaining 304, 235 (77.3%) provided informed consent to take part in the study and full data. For the baseline assessment 235 families with children of 3.5 years (mean age = 3.31, SD = .48) participated, 48% girls, and the mothers’ average age was 30.04 years (SD = 6.29). The study was approved by the Research and Ethical Committee of the Psychology Department at La Sabana University.

Participants were recruited from three Colombian regions, each with different cultural and demographic features. The Pacific (N = 69) and Caribbean (N = 70) regions are characterized by high levels of poverty and extensive numbers of Afro-Colombian and Indigenous inhabitants, while the Central region (N = 96) has the lowest levels of poverty in the country and it is predominantly mestizo (mix of European and Indigenous) (Ministerio de Ambiente y Desarrollo Sostenible, 2013). Overall, 15% of the participants lived in rural areas. The majority (77%) were two-parent families and 50% of mothers and 48% of fathers had university degrees; 45% of the sample belonged to the lowest two household income classifications, based on the Colombian government system that classifies households into six categories (1 the lowest) determined by housing conditions and basic public services, such as sewerage and water supply (Departamento Administrativo Nacional de Estadística - DANE, 2011). Classifications 1 and 2 receive subsidies from the Colombian government. Asked to identify their ethnicity, 38% mothers identified as mestizo, 9% Afro-Colombian, 5% Indigenous, 13% from “other” ethnic groups, and 35% did not identify themselves as belonging to a specific ethnic group.

At follow-up, 18 months later, 220 (93%) participants provided data for the analyses presented here (mean age = 4.86; SD = .42; 51% girls).

Measures

CBCL (Achenbach & Rescorla, Reference Achenbach and Rescorla2000). The Spanish version of the CBCL for children aged 1.5 to 5 years was used to assess aggressive behaviors. For the present study this scale had an internal consistency of .85 for the baseline and .88 for the follow-up, values that are similar to those previously reported with Colombian children (α = .86; Hewitt, Vila, & Juárez, Reference Hewitt, Vila and Juárez2016).

ICU (Frick, Reference Frick2004). CU traits were measured using the parent report version for preschool children. This inventory has 24 items scored using a 4-point scale (0 = not at all true, 1 = somewhat true, 3 = very true, and 4 = definitely true). Evidence for the reliability and validity of the ICU total score in preschool children has been provided from a cross-sectional study by Kimonis et al. (Reference Kimonis, Fanti, Anastassiou-Hadjicharalambous, Mertan, Goulter and Katsimicha2016). The Spanish version of the inventory was shared by the authors, who also approved its use in the present study. The internal consistency of the total score was good in the current sample (α = .82). Previous studies with Latin-American (Rigatti et al., Reference Rigatti, DeSouza, Salum, Alves, Bottan and Heldt2017) and Spanish children (Ezpeleta, Osa, Granero, Penelo, & Domènech, Reference Ezpeleta, Osa, Granero, Penelo and Domènech2013) have used the ICU. We have previously supported the factor structure of the ICU in this sample (Obando, Wright, & Hill, Reference Obando, Wright and Hill2021). Similar to other studies of childhood, a two-correlated factor structure showed the best fit, but in line with the recommendations of Ray, Frick, Thornton, Steinberg, and Cauffman (Reference Ray, Frick, Thornton, Steinberg and Cauffman2016) we conduct the main analysis using the full 24-item ICU total score, but report the results using the 12-item total in the Supplementary Appendix.

Confounders. The Spanish version of the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS; Cox, Holden, & Sagovsky, Reference Cox, Holden and Sagovsky1987) was used to assess maternal mood. The EPDS has 10 items scored on a 4-point scale. This measure is widely used during the prenatal and postnatal periods with parents of young children (Huntley, Wright, Pickles, Sharp, & Hill, Reference Huntley, Wright, Pickles, Sharp and Hill2017). Internal consistency of the EPDS was good in a previous study of Colombian women (α = .78; Campo-Arias, Ayola-Castillo, Peinado-Valencia, Amor-Parra, & Cogollo, Reference Campo-Arias, Ayola-Castillo, Peinado-Valencia, Amor-Parra and Cogollo2007) and also in this study (α = .83). Child sex, maternal age at pregnancy, household income (operationalized as a binary variable, 1 = lowest two DANE family income classifications, 0 = all other income classifications), and dummy variables for Caribbean and Pacific regions were included as covariates in regression analyses.

Data analyses

Data analyses in Study 2 were the same as in Study 1 with the additional confounder of dummy variables for Caribbean and Pacific regions to the variables of Block 1.

Results

The bivariate associations between the study variables and descriptive statistics are presented in Supplementary Table A2 (Appendix). Mean levels of aggression were somewhat higher than in the UK sample (age-3.5 aggression mean = 11.83, SD = 6.35, compared to mean = 7.43, SD 5.83 in WCHADS). Overall associations were very similar to those in the WCHADS, although in contrast to the UK sample, in the La Sabana study the association between male sex and aggression at age 5.0 years was nonsignificant.

Table 2 shows that, as in WCHADS, CU traits at age 3.5 years explained significant additional variance after accounting for aggression at age 3.5 years. The age-3.5 CU Traits × Aggression interaction effect was slightly larger than in WCHADS, but in this smaller sample it was nonsignificant. It arose as in WCHADS from a progressively stronger association between age-3.5 CU traits and age-5 aggression, with increasing aggression at age 3.5 years (Table 3) (Figure 1). The results were very similar when using the 12-item ICU total (see Supplementary Appendix Table A4).

Table 2. Summary of multiple linear regression models predicting mother report aggression in the La Sabana Parent–Child Study

Note: Block 1 effects were generated from a model with only Block 1, Block 2 effect from a model with Block1 plus Block 2, and Block 3 effect from a model with all three blocks.

Table 3. Simple intercepts and slopes for the association between age-3.5 callous–unemotional (CU) traits and age-5.0 aggression at three levels of age-3.5 aggression (at mean and 1 SD above and below mean) in the two samples

WCHADS: Wirral Child Health and Development Study

The regions of significance analysis indicated that the association between age-3.5 CU traits and age-5.0 aggression became significant at .37 SDs below the mean on age-3.5 aggression (equating to a score of 9 and above on age-3.5 aggression, N = 149/235 children). As in WCHADS, in analyses of the associations between age-3.5 and age-5.0 aggression at different levels of CU traits, shown in Supplementary Appendix 1 and Table A3, there were progressively stronger associations with increasing CU traits which were significant at all levels.

Comparison of main and interactive effects in the Wirral and La Sabana studies

Data analyses

The data from the two cohorts with maternal report of age-5 aggression were then standardized and pooled to test whether the main and interactive effects of CU traits differed between the United Kingdom and Colombia.

Results

Table 4 presents the results from the combined dataset. There was a significant main effect of age-3.5 CU traits after accounting for age-3.5 aggression, and a significant CU Traits × Aggression interaction. Neither of these effects was moderated by country. There was a two-way aggression by country interaction reflecting a stronger continuity between age-3.5 and age-5.0 aggression in the WCHADS sample. The results were very similar when using the 12-item ICU total in the Colombian study (see Supplementary Appendix Table A5).

Table 4. Summary of multiple linear regression model predicting age-5 maternal report aggression from in the combined Wirral Child Health and Development Study (WCHADS) and La Sabana dataset

Note: the variables main effects and two-way interactions were taken from their individual blocks. Confounds not shown.

Discussion

In two longitudinal general population samples of preschool-aged children, we found support for the validity of two measures of CU traits, widely used in older children, at age 3.5 years. The robustness of this finding was supported by incremental prediction by CU traits at 3.5 years, accounting for concurrent aggression, of aggression at age 5.0 years across two cultures, in the United Kingdom and in Colombia, and by cross-informant prospective associations in the United Kingdom. This provided the basis for testing the hypothesis that CU traits are associated with increasing child aggression specifically among children who are already aggressive. Using the same cross-cultural and cross-informant tests of robustness, we found that the association between CU traits and later aggression was much greater in children who were already aggressive at age 3.5 years than among those with lower levels of aggression. In all cases the association between CU traits and later aggression was not significant at 1 SD below the mean on concurrently assessed aggression. This study represents the first cross-cultural comparison of effect sizes in testing both the validity of CU traits in relation to aggression and in exploring the synergistic relationship between CU traits and aggression predicting future aggression.

The findings add to the evidence that CU traits may act in synergy with other vulnerabilities, such as low arousal or behavioral inhibition (Waller, Shaw et al., 2017; Wright et al., Reference Wright, Hill, Pickles and Sharp2019), sensation-seeking and grandiose traits (Colins et al., Reference Colins, Andershed, Frogner, Lopez-Romero, Veen and Andershed2014), or low mentalizing ability (Taubner et al., Reference Taubner, White, Zimmermann, Fonagy and Nolte2013) to increase the risk for aggression in children. As we noted earlier, these direct tests of synergy are complemented by studies using latent-profile analyses to identify groups of children with common characteristics which have revealed differences between high CU traits low CP and high CU traits high CP groups indicative of further synergies. For example in a study of Cypriot children aged 7–11 years, those with both elevated CU traits and CP had poorer executive function and had experienced less positive parenting than those with elevated CU traits without CP, consistent with the idea that CU traits confer risk for aggression only in the presence of additional vulnerability (Wall et al., Reference Wall, Frick, Fanti, Kimonis and Lordos2016). While studies such as these point to ways in which CU traits may contribute to CP or aggression in synergy with other factors, they do not examine for their dynamic interplay in the same way as the moderation analyses presented here, or as studies using latent transition analyses (Haltigan & Vaillancourt, Reference Haltigan and Vaillancourt2018).

In relation to the synergy between CU traits and aggression, we can envisage mechanisms whereby CU traits either decrease the likelihood of reduction, or increase the likelihood of escalation. The aggressive child with low CU traits may be expected to be concerned about the distress they cause in other children, leading them to alter their behaviors, while the child with elevated CU traits lacks this mechanism for desisting. If other children respond with aggression as well as with distress, this may create conditions for escalation unrestrained by concern. We were not, however, able to demonstrate the counterfactual, that CU traits do not predict later aggression in low aggression children. While the effects were much smaller and nonsignificant in low-aggression children across all three models, absence of effect could not be demonstrated given the sample sizes. It remains to be established therefore, whether early CU traits, or their precursors, play a role in the emergence of aggressive behaviors in young children. Another possibility, which we did not examine, is that CU traits in the absence of aggression predict other adverse outcomes. Rowe et al. (Reference Rowe, Maughan, Moran, Ford, Briskman and Goodman2010) reported that children with high CU traits but without Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) conduct disorder had higher rates of peer difficulties and emotional problems than those with neither CU traits nor conduct disorder. It may also be the case that CU traits in the absence of overt aggressive behavior may be predictive of later more covert forms of antisocial behavior, such as relational aggression.

This study builds on our previous findings where we supported the factor structure of the ICU in this Colombian sample found in HIC samples (Obando et al., Reference Obando, Wright and Hill2021) by providing evidence for the validity of the ICU in relation to aggression in a LMIC setting. The associations between CU traits and aggression, both cross-sectionally and prospectively, the incremental validity of CU traits predicting future aggression, and the interaction between CU traits and aggression, were remarkedly similar across the Colombian and UK settings. When testing for differences in the effects, only the prospective association between aggression at age 3.5 and 5.0 years was significantly different across countries, with the UK sample showing high continuity. As far as we are aware, this is the first study to provide a formal test of whether there are cross-cultural differences in the associations between CU traits and aggression.

The strengths of the study were that we examined associations prospectively in general population samples, testing for replication, and for common method variance explanations using cross-informant measurement. This was particularly important given the relative paucity of evidence in young children, and in the light of concerns regarding the validity and measurement of CU traits in 3–4-year-olds. Developmental processes in the origins of child CP in general, and of the role of CU traits in particular, have not been studied extensively outside of HICs, so the inclusion of a demographically diverse cohort from Colombia was a distinctive strength. A limitation of the replication is that, although it was somewhat higher than the interaction in the UK sample, the age-3.5 CU Traits × Aggression interaction in the smaller La Sabana study was nonsignificant. Both studies aimed to recruit samples that were representative of general populations; however, in the UK study attrition over the period from pregnancy to age 5 was selective, with more deprived families and younger mothers more likely to be lost from the study. Also, in the Colombian study, despite a similar proportion of families belonging to the lowest government family income classifications as reported from previous studies in Colombia (Buitrago-Lopez et al., Reference Buitrago-Lopez, Van den Hooven, Rueda-Clausen, Serrano, Ruiz, Pereira and Mueller2015), around half of the sample had university degrees, contrasted with 22% in the general population (Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development – OECD, 2018).

The modest internal consistency of APSD in the UK sample was a concern, raising the possibility that it may not have provided a good index of CU traits. This was mitigated to some degree by replication of validity and hypothesis-testing findings in the Colombian sample where the ICU showed good internal consistency in 3.5-year-olds. The APSD has been repeatedly found to have a low Cronbach's alpha, especially in studies with younger children (e.g., Dadds et al., Reference Dadds, Fraser, Frost and Hawes2005; Kimonis et al., Reference Kimonis, Frick, Boris, Smyke, Cornell, Farrell and Zeanah2006) and likely influenced by the small number of items on the scale (Tavakol & Dennick, Reference Tavakol and Dennick2011). The APSD was the only measure of CU traits available at the time of the age 3.5 data collection wave in WCHADS as the ICU had not been adapted or validated for use with preschool children. We included parental mood at the time of reporting the outcome to attempt to address the risk of biasing by mood; unfortunately different measures were used in the two samples and for the mother and partner reporters. Finally, while we took steps to check the possible role of same-informant effects, the robustness of the findings would have been further enhanced through the use of observational measures, such as of child empathy, and additional informants for child behaviors.

The findings have implications for the study of the link between CU traits and child aggression. On the one hand they could point to as yet unidentified heterogeneity whereby CU traits associated with later aggression are underpinned by different processes from CU traits with little or no implications for later aggression. This may be an alternative explanation for the moderator effect we report. The presence of aggressive behaviors at baseline in our studies may be a marker for such heterogeneity, whereby for example the CU traits of those who are already aggressive may be a manifestation of an unmeasured additional vulnerability, such as low arousal, which accounts for the stronger association with later aggression. Studying the dynamic interplay between CU traits and aggression over time could help clarify this further. On the other hand, if there is synergy of the kind we have hypothesized, specific mechanisms could be examined; for example, experimental approaches that examine the moment-by-moment responses of high-aggression/high-CU-traits children to others’ distress resulting from peer aggression. Furthermore the role of different kinds of interpersonal sensitivity needs to be understood. The finding in adolescents, of synergy between CU traits and low mentalization, suggests each may have distinct functions (Taubner et al., Reference Taubner, White, Zimmermann, Fonagy and Nolte2013), with further questions to be addressed regarding ways they may modify aggression. Regarding early intervention, the findings strongly suggest targeting high-aggression/high-CU-traits children, and also that interventions either to reduce CU traits or to reduce aggression in these children may be effective. Equally, interventions may need to address specifically whether aggressive children with high CU traits can be provided with alternative strategies for understanding the emotional impact of their behaviors.

Supplementary Material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579420002114

Acknowledgments

We are very grateful to all participating families and to the research staff who contributed to WCHADS and the La Sabana Parent–Child Study. The WCHADS study authors are grateful to Wirral University Teaching Hospital NHS Foundation Trust, Wirral Community NHS Foundation Trust, Cheshire and Wirral Partnership NHS Foundation Trust who supported the recruitment into the study in pregnancy and to the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust. The views expressed are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health and Social Care.

Financial Support

The WCHADS was funded by grants from the UK Medical Research Council (JH, AP, HS; G0400577, G0900654). The La Sabana Parent–Child Study was funded by La Sabana University as part of the University's Professor training program being completed by DO. AP was partially supported by NIHR Senior Investigator award NF-SI-0617-10120.

Conflicts of Interest

None.

References

Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2000). Manual for the ASEBA preschool forms and profiles: An integrated system of multi-informant assessment. Burlington: University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry.Google Scholar
Blair, R. J. R. (2013). The neurobiology of psychopathic traits in youths. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14, 786799.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, R., Colledge, E., Murray, L., & Mitchell, D. (2001). A selective impairment in the processing of sad and fearful expressions in children with psychopathic tendencies. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 29, 491498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buitrago-Lopez, A., Van den Hooven, E. H., Rueda-Clausen, C. F., Serrano, N., Ruiz, A. J., Pereira, M. A., & Mueller, N. T. (2015). Socioeconomic status is positively associated with measures of adiposity and insulin resistance, but inversely associated with dyslipidaemia in Colombian children. Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health, 69, 580587.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campo-Arias, A., Ayola-Castillo, C., Peinado-Valencia, H. M., Amor-Parra, M., & Cogollo, Z. (2007). Escala de Edinburgh para depresión posparto: Consistencia interna y estructura factorial en mujeres embarazadas de cartagena, Colombia. Revista Colombiana de Obstetricia y Ginecología, 58, 277283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colins, O. F., Andershed, H., Frogner, L., Lopez-Romero, L., Veen, V., & Andershed, A. K. (2014). A new measure to assess psychopathic personality in children: The child problematic traits inventory. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 36, 421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cox, J. L., Holden, J. M., & Sagovsky, R. (1987). Detection of postnatal depression: Development of the 10-item Edinburgh postnatal depression scale. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 782786.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dadds, M. R., Fraser, J., Frost, A., & Hawes, D. J. (2005). Disentangling the underlying dimensions of psychopathy and conduct problems in childhood: A community study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dadds, M. R., Jambrak, J., Pasalich, D., Hawes, D. J., & Brennan, J. (2011). Impaired attention to the eyes of attachment figures and the developmental origins of psychopathy. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, 238245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dadds, M. R., & Salmon, K. (2003). Punishment insensitivity and parenting: Temperament and learning as interacting risks for antisocial behavior. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 6, 6986.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Departamento Administrativo Nacional de Estadistica - DANE. (2011). Nueva metodología de estratificación socioeconómica. Mimeo: Departamento Administrativo Nacional de Estadistica. Dirección de Geoestadística. Grupo de Diseño Nueva Metodología de Estratificación.Google Scholar
Ezpeleta, L., Osa, N. D. L., Granero, R., Penelo, E., & Domènech, J. M. (2013). Inventory of callous-unemotional traits in a community sample of preschoolers. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 42, 91105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fanti, K. A. (2013). Individual, social, and behavioral factors associated with co-occurring conduct problems and callous-unemotional traits. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41, 811824.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fontaine, N. M., McCrory, E. J., Boivin, M., Moffitt, T. E., & Viding, E. (2011). Predictors and outcomes of joint trajectories of callous–unemotional traits and conduct problems in childhood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120, 730.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frick, P. J. (2004). Inventory of callous-unemotional traits. Unpublished rating scale. University of New Orleans, New Orleans.Google Scholar
Frick, P. J., & Hare, R. D. (2001). The psychopathy screening device. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Multi-Health Systems.Google Scholar
Frick, P. J., Ray, J. V., Thornton, L. C., & Kahn, R. E. (2014). Can callous-unemotional traits enhance the understanding, diagnosis, and treatment of serious conduct problems in children and adolescents? A comprehensive review. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frick, P. J., & White, S. F. (2008). Research review: The importance of callous-unemotional traits for developmental models of aggressive and antisocial behavior. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 49, 359375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldberg, D., & Williams, P. (1988). General health questionnaire (GHQ). Swindon, Wiltshire, UK: nferNelson.Google Scholar
Haltigan, J. D., & Vaillancourt, T. (2018). The influence of static and dynamic intrapersonal factors on longitudinal patterns of peer victimization through mid-adolescence: A latent transition analysis. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 46, 1126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henry, J., Pingault, J. B., Boivin, M., Rijsdijk, F., & Viding, E. (2016). Genetic and environmental aetiology of the dimensions of callous-unemotional traits. Psychological Medicine, 46, 405414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hewitt, N., Vila, J., & Juárez, F. (2016). Predicción de los problemas de banda ancha y banda estrecha mediante la integración de los modelos transaccional, cognitivo-social y de regulación emocional (Doctoral dissertation, Tesis Doctoral). Retrieved from http://hdl.handle.net/10481/42435.Google Scholar
Huntley, F., Wright, N., Pickles, A., Sharp, H., & Hill, J. (2017). Maternal mental health and child problem behaviours: Disentangling the role of depression and borderline personality dysfunction. BJPsych Open, 3, 300305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kimonis, E. R., Fanti, K. A., Anastassiou-Hadjicharalambous, X., Mertan, B., Goulter, N., & Katsimicha, E. (2016). Can callous-unemotional traits be reliably measured in preschoolers? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 44, 625638.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kimonis, E. R., Frick, P. J., Boris, N. W., Smyke, A. T., Cornell, A. H., Farrell, J. M., & Zeanah, C. H. (2006). Callous-unemotional features, behavioral inhibition, and parenting: Independent predictors of aggression in a high-risk preschool sample. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 15, 741752.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ministerio de Ambiente y Desarrollo Sostenible. (2013). Caracterización de las regiones para la preparación de la ENREDD (Anexo 1b). Recovered from http://www.minambiente.gov.co/images/BosquesBiodiversidadyServiciosEcosistemicos/pdf/Documentos-Redd/021013_anexo_1b2_caracterizacion_regiones.pdf.Google Scholar
Ministerio de Tecnologías de la Información y las Comunicaciones. (2018). ¿Cuáles son las redes sociales que más se usan en su región? Recovered from http://www.mintic.gov.co/portal/604/w3-article-70369.html.Google Scholar
Noble, M., Wright, G., Dibben, C., Smith, G., McLennan, D., Anttila, C., … Lloyd, M. (2004). The English indices of deprivation 2004 (revised). Report to the office of the deputy prime minister. London: Neighbourhood Renewal Unit.Google Scholar
Obando, D., Wright, N., & Hill, J. (2021). Positive and negative parenting, callous-unemotional traits and oppositional defiant disorder behaviors in preschool Colombian children. Under review.Google Scholar
Odgers, C. L., Moffitt, T. E., Broadbent, J. M., Dickson, N., Hancox, R. J., Harrington, H., … Caspi, A. (2008). Female and male antisocial trajectories: From childhood origins to adult outcomes. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 673.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development – OECD. (2018). Education at a Glance 2018. Retrieved from https://read.oecd-ilibrary.org/education/education-at-a-glance-2018_eag-2018-en#page1.Google Scholar
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ray, J. V., Frick, P. J., Thornton, L. C., Steinberg, L., & Cauffman, E. (2016). Positive and negative item wording and its influence on the assessment of callous-unemotional traits. Psychological Assessment, 28(4), 394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rigatti, R., DeSouza, D. A., Salum, G., Alves, P. F. O., Bottan, G., & Heldt, E. (2017). Cross-cultural adaptation of the inventory of callous-unemotional traits for evaluation of callous-unemotional traits in Brazilian adolescents. Revista Gaúcha de Enfermagem, 38(3).Google Scholar
Roisman, G. I., Newman, D. A., Fraley, R. C., Haltigan, J. D., Groh, A. M., & Haydon, K. C. (2012). Distinguishing differential susceptibility from diathesis–stress: Recommendations for evaluating interaction effects. Development & Psychopathology, 24, 389409.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowe, R., Maughan, B., Moran, P., Ford, T., Briskman, J., & Goodman, R. (2010). The role of callous and unemotional traits in the diagnosis of conduct disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 688695.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sharp, H., Pickles, A., Meaney, M., Marshall, K., Tibu, F., & Hill, J. (2012). Frequency of infant stroking reported by mothers moderates the effect of prenatal depression on infant behavioral and physiological outcomes. PLoS One, 7, e45446.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taubner, S., White, L. O., Zimmermann, J., Fonagy, P., & Nolte, T. (2013). Attachment-related mentalization moderates the relationship between psychopathic traits and proactive aggression in adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41, 929938.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tavakol, M., & Dennick, R. (2011). Making sense of Cronbach's alpha. International Journal of Medical Education, 2, 53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Viding, E., Blair, R. J. R., Moffitt, T. E., & Plomin, R. (2005). Evidence for substantial genetic risk for psychopathy in 7-year-olds. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 592597.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wall, T. D., Frick, P. J., Fanti, K. A., Kimonis, E. R., & Lordos, A. (2016). Factors differentiating callous-unemotional children with and without conduct problems. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57, 976983.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waller, R., Dishion, T. J., Shaw, D. S., Gardner, F., Wilson, M. N., & Hyde, L. W. (2016). Does early childhood callous-unemotional behavior uniquely predict behavior problems or callous-unemotional behavior in late childhood? Developmental Psychology, 52, 18051819.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waller, R., Hyde, L. W., Baskin-Sommers, A. R., & Olson, S. L. (2017). Interactions between callous unemotional behaviors and executive function in early childhood predict later aggression and lower peer-liking in late-childhood. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 45, 597609.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waller, R., Hyde, L. W., Grabell, A. S., Alves, M. L., & Olson, S. L. (2015). Differential associations of early callous-unemotional, oppositional, and ADHD behaviors: Multiple domains within early-starting conduct problems? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56, 657666.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willoughby, M. T., Mills-Koonce, W. R., Gottfredson, N. C., & Wagner, N. (2014). Measuring callous unemotional behaviors in early childhood: Factor structure and the prediction of stable aggression in middle childhood. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 36, 3042.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wright, N., Hill, J., Pickles, A., & Sharp, H. (2019). Callous-unemotional traits, low cortisol reactivity and physical aggression in children: Findings from the Wirral child health and development study. Translational Psychiatry, 9, 79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wright, N., Hill, J., Sharp, H., & Pickles, A. (2018). Maternal sensitivity to distress, attachment and the development of callous-unemotional traits in young children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 59, 790800.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wright, N., Sharp, H., Pickles, A., & Hill, J. (2017). Measurement of callous-unemotional traits in very young children: A psychometric and validity study from 2.5 to 5.0 years. Retrieved from https://osf.io/t7fn9.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. The prospective association between age-3.5 callous–unemotional (CU) traits and age-5.0 aggression at three levels of age 3.5 aggression (at mean and 1 SD above and below mean) in the mother and partner report Wirral Child Health and Development Study (WCHADS) sample and mother report La Sabana Parent–Child Study.

Figure 1

Table 1. Summary of linear regression models predicting maternal and partner report of aggression in the Wirral Child Health and Development Study (WCHADS)

Figure 2

Table 2. Summary of multiple linear regression models predicting mother report aggression in the La Sabana Parent–Child Study

Figure 3

Table 3. Simple intercepts and slopes for the association between age-3.5 callous–unemotional (CU) traits and age-5.0 aggression at three levels of age-3.5 aggression (at mean and 1 SD above and below mean) in the two samples

Figure 4

Table 4. Summary of multiple linear regression model predicting age-5 maternal report aggression from in the combined Wirral Child Health and Development Study (WCHADS) and La Sabana dataset

Supplementary material: File

Obando et al. supplementary material

Obando et al. supplementary material

Download Obando et al. supplementary material(File)
File 27.4 KB