Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jn8rn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-19T02:06:34.493Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Do Politically Irrelevant Events Cause Conflict? The Cross-continental Effects of European Professional Football on Protests in Africa

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 October 2022

Kyosuke Kikuta*
Affiliation:
Institute of Developing Economies, Japan External Trade Organization, Tokyo
Mamoru Uesugi
Affiliation:
Osaka School of International Public Policy, Osaka University, Japan
*
*Corresponding author. Email: [email protected]

Abstract

We examine whether politically irrelevant events can cause conflicts, by analyzing the effects of professional football games in Europe on protests in Africa—an unintended spillover across the continents. By expanding psychological theories, we argue that the outcomes of the football games in Europe can affect African people's subjective evaluation of domestic politicians, which in turn can trigger protests. By exploiting as-if random variation in the results of 15,102 close football games conditional on betting odds, we find that compared to draw games, close losses of African players’ teams increase peaceful protests in their original countries while not changing the likelihood of riots or armed conflicts. The effect is particularly large for non-ethnic protests targeted at a central government. Close losses also temporarily decrease people's trust in their country's leader. By contrast, close victories do not have equivalent or compensating effects on protests or public opinion. These results suggest asymmetric misattribution: people in Africa unreasonably blame domestic politicians for bad luck in European football games, prompting protests; but they do not credit politicians with football victories.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The IO Foundation

Karl Marx said “Religion is the opiate of the masses.” He was wrong. It's sports.

—Bill ScharrerFootnote 1

In recent decades, professional football in Europe has emerged as the “second religion” in Africa. In the words of Musa in Nigeria, “European football has become part of my life, it is more than just a source of entertainment but it's like my second religion.” Another Arsenal supporter, Jude, does not even deny the risk of escalating violence: “What often makes me angry in football centers [places to watch European football broadcasts] is the insult and unnecessary mockery. There are times people will tell you something that will annoy you just because their team beat yours … This kind of negative words sincerely stir anger in one, and I believe it can cause physical fights one day.”Footnote 2 Clearly, the outcomes of football games played over 5,000 kilometers away have a profound impact on the lives of Nigerians. Could this “second religion” cause conflicts—something more than just brawls—in Africa? If so, by what mechanisms?

We answer these questions by analyzing the effect of results in professional football in Europe on conflicts in Africa. In doing so, we challenge previous studies on conflict, which until recently have ignored the roles of politically irrelevant factors like sports events.Footnote 3 Most of the standard frameworks—including grievance theses,Footnote 4 contentious politics frameworks,Footnote 5 collective action theories,Footnote 6 and bargaining modelsFootnote 7—give the impression that politically irrelevant events should not matter, because they would not obviously affect the costs, benefits, information, or outcomes of conflict. We, however, argue that psychological cues do have an effect.

To this end, sports events provide unique analytical advantages. Other dramatic events, like natural disasters, are also influential. However, disasters provide citizens with objective information about the government's performance in mitigatory and response policies.Footnote 8 Thus, even without any effects from psychological cues, natural disasters can affect information, and thus citizens’ rational choices, and hence conflicts. By contrast, as Ashworth, Bueno de Mesquita, and Friedenberg acknowledge,Footnote 9 sports events provide no objective information about politicians’ competence.Footnote 10 The political irrelevance of sports events allows us to isolate the effects of psychological cues from those of rational updates.

Moreover, unlike Card and Dahl and related studies on the economic and social consequences of sports events,Footnote 11 we analyze a politically, geographically, and causally distant relationship between football in Europe and conflicts in Africa. While the literature focuses on domestic relationships, the effects of “local” sports events are rarely exclusively local; hundreds of millions of people in Africa, for instance, breathlessly watch football games played in Europe. Given the globalization of the sports industry, it is imperative to understand the intercontinental spillover of major sports events.

Last but not least, unlike recent studies on big international sports events such as the World Cup and the Africa Cup of Nations,Footnote 12 we focus on European professional football to ensure the political irrelevance of the events. Even though we do not find compelling evidence except for a few correlational studies,Footnote 13 one might still suspect that politicians in Africa could affect the results of international competitions via budgetary allocations and sports policies (for example, selection of managers and coaches).Footnote 14 By contrast, African politicians have no meaningful influence over the results of professional football in Europe. This means that if the professional football games in Europe affect conflicts in Africa, the reasons must be something other than rational updates—such as psychological factors.

One such factor is moods; losing a game can affect emotional status, causing anger, distress, shame, sadness, and anxiety.Footnote 15 People can join protests to vent their negative mood. Conversely, people may bask in the glory of their team's victory. Another factor is misattribution; people can directly or indirectly misattribute “bad luck” in a game to their politicians and thus stage protests.Footnote 16 By contrast, after a game victory, people may give undeserved credit to politicians. Finally, the collective experiences of victories and losses can unite football fans across ethnic cleavages, which may halt conflicts, especially those related to ethnic issues.Footnote 17

We examine these mechanisms in two sets of analyses. In the first, we identify the effect of game results in Europe on protests in Africa by exploiting as-if random variation in the results of close football games conditional on pre-game betting odds. Analysis of 15,087 close games and 40 African countries reveals that compared to draws, losses of African players’ teams increase peaceful demonstrations in their original countries while not changing the likelihood of riots or armed conflicts. The effect of close losses is particularly large for non-ethnic demonstrations targeted at a central government.

Once we establish the causal relationship, we dissect the mechanisms by using individual-level surveys. By exploiting as-if random coincidences of survey interviews and football games, we show that compared to draws, close losses temporarily decrease people's trust in their political leaders, while winning or losing games have almost no effect on people's moods. Overall, these findings, as well as extensive analysis of effect heterogeneity and robustness, indicate that football games in Europe have sizable effects on social conflicts in Africa.

Africa and European Professional Football

European professional football is an important entertainment in the daily lives of Africans. Partly due to the relative lack of alternatives, soccer is the most popular sport in Africa.Footnote 18 This feature distinguishes it from Asia, where other sports are also popular, and also from Latin America, which has vigorous domestic leagues. According to a 2011 survey, 71 percent of people in Africa have an interest in soccer.Footnote 19 And around 276 million people in Sub-Saharan Africa—about a quarter of the entire population—regularly watch the Premier League.Footnote 20 Moreover, as the anecdotes at the beginning of this paper reflect, many Africans are devoted fans of European professional football. As Ka, Od, and I describe, “The European football fellowship is a new ‘religion’, another kind of ‘ethnicity’, and other form of ‘ritual’.”Footnote 21 Due to the absence of cable networks and the high cost of good display devices, most Africans use so-called viewing centers, where they pay an entrance fee to view live football games on satellite television (or over the Internet from the 2010s). Because of the zealous fandom, ubiquitous coverage on satellite television, and low entrance fees, since the late 1990s viewing centers have spread across the continent, even to rural villages.Footnote 22

Fandom's rapid growth is the flip side of the increasing presence of African players in European football. In 1970 only one African player played in the top five leagues; in 2005, there were 217 who did (Figure 1). In the period of our analysis (the 2005 to 2018 seasons), 990 Africans play in the top five leagues, 79 percent of games have at least one African player, and two players per game originally come from Africa. Among those players, 63 percent are from Western Africa, followed by Eastern (24%), Northern (10%), and Southern Africa (3%).Footnote 23 Similarly, 47 percent of them play in Ligue 1, followed by Serie A (16%), Premier League (14%), Bundesliga (11%), and La Liga (11%). These patterns reflect soccer's overwhelming popularity in Western Africa,Footnote 24 where most countries are former French colonies.

Figure 1. The rapid increase of African players in European professional football

In this paper, we focus on the results for football teams to which an African player belongs. People can support teams without African players, but the presence of co-national players is a strong indicator of fandom. For example, the popularity of Chelsea in Côte d'Ivoire is often attributed to the presence of national players, such as Didier Drogba and Solomon Kalou.Footnote 25 Similarly, the presence of Nwankwo Kanu is said to make Arsenal popular in Nigeria.Footnote 26 Because this is an important assumption in the interpretation of our findings (though we did not come up with compelling alternatives), we test the relationship in Appendix A2. We find that the presence of co-national players is indeed a significant predictor of fandom. Later, we also check robustness by analyzing players’ appearance in games.

The Psychological Origins of Social Conflicts

We argue that the results of football games provide a psychological cue and thus affect the likelihood of conflict. To be clear, we do not argue that football games would cause full-fledged civil wars, which depend on organizational and strategic dynamics. However, social conflicts (like peaceful demonstrations and violent riots) depend on individuals’ participation and are hence subject to people's cognitive biases. In fact, many protests are small and thus do not require collective action.Footnote 27 Among the protests occurring in Africa in 1990 to 2017, 37 percent had less than a hundred participants, and only a third had more than a thousand.Footnote 28

Importantly, psychological explanations are complementary to standard theories of protests. For instance, deviant behavior by even just a few individuals can change the strategic calculus of other rational people or organizations, and thus spark large-scale protests. As Kuran states, the participation of a few deviant people can marginally improve the prospect of a protest and thus induce other rational people to join in, which in turn may lead to the participation of even more.Footnote 29 This can quickly snowball into unexpectedly large protests.Footnote 30 Similarly, the results of football games can provide opposition parties or movements with psychological cues, which can be used to mobilize people for protests.Footnote 31 Thus, even though psychological cues may or may not affect armed conflicts specifically, they can cause both small- and large-scale protests.Footnote 32

Null Expectation: Rational Update

From the perspective of a standard rational update model,Footnote 33 the results of professional football games in Europe should not affect protests in Africa because they do not change material payoffs or provide any new information about the competence of domestic politicians. Any effect sports and education policies might have on the performance of African players in Europe would only become meaningful over decades. This means that those policies do not immediately reveal the politicians’ competence. Thus, any immediate effect of European professional football should be explained by reasons other than rational update.

Mechanism 1: Mood

A straightforward explanation is that the results of the football games influence moods—that is, transitory feeling statesFootnote 34—which in turn affect the likelihood of protests. On the defeat of a favored team, people can lose excitement and confidence, and feel sad or even angry. Although people can vent their negative moods through various means (for example drinking, gambling, crime, and violence),Footnote 35 joining protests can also alleviate them.Footnote 36 Conversely, a victory for their team can relieve people's negative moods and provide positive feelings of enthusiasm and elation, and hence halt protests. Thus, the mood mechanism predicts that the loss (win) of an African player's team will increase (decrease) protests in his original country.

The literature on social psychology, however, implies asymmetric effects. The basking-in-reflected-glory and cutting-off-reflected-failure theses state that people tend to associate themselves with a glorious outcome of their favored team and thus obtain positive moods, while they tend to discount such an association to maintain their mental status when their favored team experiences a failure.Footnote 37 The asymmetric mood mechanism therefore predicts that winning games foster positive moods and hence reduce protests, while losing games have no effect.

Mechanism 2: Attribution

Other (not mutually exclusive) mechanisms are based on misattribution—that is, unjustified credit and blame. While the mood mechanism primarily pertains to emotion, attribution relates to (distorted) information processing. In its simplest form (direct attribution), people can attribute bad luck in football games not only to the players, coaches, and managers in Europe but also, unreasonably, to domestic politicians.Footnote 38 Although direct misattribution may or may not be realistic, attribution can also be indirect.Footnote 39 Because contemporary political processes are complicated, people often use their subjective welfare heuristically to evaluate politicians’ performance. From this perspective, losing matches can hurt the subjective welfare of people, which in turn can be blamed on politicians and thus lead to protests. Or, on the victory of favored teams, people might give politicians undeserved credit, forgiving their objective incompetence.Footnote 40 The misattribution mechanism predicts that losing games will trigger protests while winning games will curb them.

The social psychology literature implies asymmetric effects. Success/failure bias refers to the human tendency to blame others for failures (even when they are not responsible) while crediting successes to oneself (even when one is not responsible).Footnote 41 People can then perceive their favored team's victory as a personal success and thus may not credit politicians (or refrain from protesting), while they directly or indirectly attribute their favored team's losses to domestic politicians (and join protests). This egoistic attribution, which we call the asymmetric misattribution mechanism, suggests that losing games will cause blaming and protests, but winning games will not improve people's attitudes toward politicians or reduce protests.Footnote 42

Mechanisms 3: Identity

Finally, the glory or misery shared with co-national players may unite fans under the national identity, which in turn may make existing ethnic cleavages less salient and halt protests, especially those related to ethnic issues.Footnote 43 As the rally-around-the-flag effect suggests,Footnote 44 both collective glory and suffering unite people. The rally mechanism predicts that both winning and losing games will reduce (ethnic) protests. Table 1 summarizes the causal mechanisms and predictions.

Table 1. Causal mechanisms and predictions

Note: 0, +, and − indicate no, positive (higher likelihood of protests), and negative (lower likelihood) effects, respectively.

Event Data Analysis: Research Design

We test the hypotheses by conducting two sets of empirical analyses: an event data analysis at a macro level, and a survey analysis at the individual level. One empirical challenge is that the results of European football games are not randomly assigned. For instance, better sports and education policies may be associated with both a larger number of star players from a country and greater social stability. This endogeneity might create a spurious correlation. We address this problem by using a natural experiment in both analyses: we compare a close win, draw, and loss of an African player's team. Because the fates of close games are easily swayed by random chance, such as shots that hit the post and own goals, the treatment assignment should be closer to as-if random.Footnote 45

Another problem is that points are scored less frequently in soccer than in other sports. As a result, one goal means a big difference, and thus even winners and losers of close games can be systematically different. In fact, even though we compare the narrowest margin of wins and losses,Footnote 46 the later balance check indicates that the natural experiment improves the covariate balances but does not do so perfectly.

In the event data analysis, we therefore follow Card and Dahl's well-established approach by conditioning on pre-game bookmaker betting odds (decimal odds).Footnote 47 Betting odds are the inverse of expected probabilities of game results (a 50% probability of winning gives 2.00 odds). Thus, to the extent that betting odds provide accurate predictions of game results, we can directly observe the treatment assignment probability and hence identify the causal effect. Our identification assumption is therefore that conditional on the pre-game betting odds, the results of close games are randomly assigned. Substantively speaking, if two games have similar betting odds but their results are different, the difference can plausibly be considered “unexpected” and thus as-if random. Moreover, because inaccurate betting odds can cause large financial losses, bookmakers have strong incentives to measure them accurately.Footnote 48 Even though it is sometimes difficult to predict the results of football games, our design is valid to the extent that the bookmakers make an accurate prediction or the result of a close game is random. As a robustness check, we limit the comparison to close games of similar betting odds and similar numbers of shots on target to focus on even closer games.

We also use the difference-in-differences (DiD) by taking the first difference of the outcome variable between a few days before and after a football game. This eliminates confounders that do not change over that period (for example, reporting biases in conflict data). Because DiD can overstate effect sizes,Footnote 49 we also report the results without first differencing and those with lagged dependent variables (LDV) models in a robustness check.Footnote 50

Finally, we account for potential reverse causality (anticipation of protests might affect player performance and violent plays, which in turn could affect game results)Footnote 51 as well as alternative interpretation (policies in African countries might affect the performance of African players and thus game results) by controlling for player performance in a robustness check.Footnote 52 The remaining variation in the game results comes from the performance of the African players’ teammates. It is unlikely that the performance of non-African players would be affected by conflicts or policies in an African country.

Sample and Unit

The unit of analysis is a pair of a player i and a football game j. Figure 2 illustrates the configuration of our data. Because each player has a single birth country and each game has a single date, we can uniquely define the outcome variable: the incidence of protests in player i's birth country a few days before and after game j.Footnote 53 Similarly, because a player belongs to a single team on the day of a given football game, the treatment variable—the result of player i's team in game j—is also uniquely defined. We use a player, instead of his birth country, as a unit to control for individual performance in a robustness check.Footnote 54 Although the event data analysis largely rests on country-level variation and might therefore be subject to aggregation biases, we later supplement it with an analysis of individual-level surveys.

Figure 2. Unit of analysis and variables

Our sample includes all players whose birthplaces are African countries, and the football games in the top five European leagues (La Liga in Spain, Premier League in England, Serie A in Italy, Bundesliga in Germany, and Ligue 1 in France) and the Champions League (group and knockout stages; only teams from the top five leagues) between the 2005–06 and 2018–19 seasons.Footnote 55 In the following analysis, we drop 272 games in which both sides had players from the same African country,Footnote 56 and limit the sample to close games.Footnote 57 The resulting data contain 61,527 observations, including 951 players from 40 African countries in 184 teams playing 15,087 close games. For simplicity, we split the sample into the games in which African players’ teams drew or closely lost (N = 43,984), and the games in which they drew or closely won (N = 43,084).Footnote 58 The summary statistics are given in Appendix A4.

Treatment Variable

The treatment variable D ij is a dichotomous variable that is 1 if African player i's team loses or wins in game j and 0 if the result is a draw. We code the treatment status based on African players’ team affiliations instead of their appearance in games.Footnote 59 To be sure, we later conduct a robustness check by splitting the sample into those with and without African players’ appearance. The data were scraped from Transfermarkt.com on 7 April 2021.

Outcome Variables

The outcome variable ΔY ij is the difference in the daily probability (%) of conflict incidence in player i's birth country before and after football game j: $\Delta Y_{ij} = 100\left({{{\mathop \sum \nolimits_{t\in { {1, \ldots , T} } } y_{ijt}} \over T}-{{\mathop \sum \nolimits_{t\in { {-T, .., -1} } } y_{ijt}} \over T}} \right)$, where y ijt is a dummy of conflict incidence in player i's birth country t days after game j, and T is a time window.Footnote 60 We omit observations on the day of the game (t = 0) because the treatment status is indeterminate (hours before a game are controlled, while hours after a game are treated). We use a three-day window (T = 3) because the games should have only temporary effects and because a three-day window effectively contains an entire week (3 + 1 + 3 = 7).Footnote 61 In robustness checks, we use different time windows.

Incidences of protest are derived from Armed Conflict Location and Event Data (ACLED) and the Social Conflict Analysis Database (SCAD), which are standard data sets of conflict events.Footnote 62 For ACLED, we include all demonstrations and riots if the exact date information is available.Footnote 63 We also create a separate variable of battles between armed groups for comparison. For SCAD, we collect demonstrations and their types (e.g., sizes, targets, and issues).Footnote 64 Although SCAD contains richer information about each event, it substantially underreports the number of events. For instance, while according to ACLED, about 10 percent of the observations experienced demonstrations within three days after the football game, the number is less than 3 percent in SCAD. Moreover, SCAD does not contain information about the precision of event dates,Footnote 65 which is crucial for our daily-level analysis, and the data set has not been updated since 2018. Thus, we use ACLED in the main analysis and supplement it with SCAD to disaggregate the demonstration events by target, issue, and size.

Although both data sets depend on media reports and are thus subject to reporting biases, “as long as the measurement error is uncorrelated with the independent variables, measurement error in the dependent variable is not particularly problematic in a standard regression framework other than increasing the uncertainty around the estimates we obtain.”Footnote 66 Moreover, because we use the first differences of the outcome variables, the reporting biases do not matter unless there are systematic differences a few days before and after a football game.

Control Variable

The data on pre-game betting odds in the top five leagues are BetBrain's average bookmaker betting odds from Football-Data.co.uk (scraped 1 January 2022). These are averages of twenty bookmaker websites, providing the most consistent data on betting odds for every football game played in the 2005–2019 period. Because the data set does not cover the Champions League, we supplement it with Odds Portal's average betting odds (<https://www.oddsportal.com>, scraped 5 April 2021). The treatment assignment probability (%) is calculated as $P_{ij} = 100{{{\rm odd}{\rm s}_{{\rm draw}, ij}} \over {{\rm odd}{\rm s}_{v, \;ij} + {\rm odd}{\rm s}_{{\rm draw}, ij}}}$ for v ∈ {loss, win} and game j of player i's team.

Specification

With the directly measured assignment probabilities, we conduct nearest-neighbor matching without replacement.Footnote 67 We use the unstandardized caliper size of five percentage points (we later conduct robustness checks with different calipers). The matching yields 12,168 losing and draw games (91% of the sample) and 11,943 winning and draw games (90% of the sample). The resultant samples include 35,481 and 34,033 player-game observations, respectively.

With the matched samples, we estimate the average treatment effect that is local to a few days before and after a losing or winning game (local average treatment effect on the treated, LATT) by taking a difference:

(1)$${\rm LATT} = {\rm E}[ {\Delta Y_{ij}\vert D_{ij} = 1, \;\;P_{ij}\approx p_{ij}} ] -{\rm E}[ {\Delta Y_{ij}\vert D_{ij} = 0, \;P_{ij}\approx p_{ij}} ] $$

Here, i and j are a player from an African country and a football game, respectively. We condition on the inverse of betting odds P ij so that treated and control units have assignment probabilities centered on a value of p ij.

Because our design accounts for confounders, we do not use fixed effects in the main analysis and leave it to robustness checks. The unit fixed effects are redundant because DiD accounts for static factors. Time fixed effects (year, month, week, day of the week) are also unnecessary as far as the treatment is as-if randomly assigned. For instance, even though the football games are usually played on weekends, we compare losing, winning, and draw games. As a result, the weekend effects are canceled out in the comparison.Footnote 68 This means that day-of-the-week fixed effects are redundant and only reduce the power of analysis. In fact, as we show in robustness checks, the estimates are nearly identical across various specifications with fixed effects. The fixed effects also make the estimates less interpretable and more dependent on modeling assumptions,Footnote 69 exacerbate the attenuation biases of measurement errors,Footnote 70 and limit external validity.Footnote 71

The standard errors are two-way clustered by country and game. Clustering by country accounts for the cases where players from the same country belong to different teams, thereby yielding repeated values of the outcome variable. Clustering by game accounts for situations in which multiple players from Africa belong to teams in the same game, which also yields repeated values of the treatment variable.

Event Data Analysis: Results

The analyses with the main specification indicate that both barely losing and barely winning games increase the likelihood of demonstrations (Table 2). Substantively, a close loss (win) increases the probability of demonstrations by 0.67 (0.62) percentage points. Because the average value of the outcome is 0.60 percentage points, the close loss and win double the rate of demonstrations. By contrast, the effects on riots and battles are statistically and substantively minimal; the effect sizes are less than half of that of close losses on demonstrations, and all of the signs are negative.Footnote 72 The null effect on battles is consistent with our argument that armed conflicts are driven by organizational and strategic dynamics. The null or even slightly negative effect on riots implies that emotional cues can drive people to start protests, but they may avoid risky options and use relatively safe means, which is consistent with some of the previous findings about rationality in irrational behaviors.Footnote 73

Table 2. Effect of close losses and wins on conflict incidence

Notes: Coefficient estimates, with corresponding standard errors in parentheses. The standard errors are two-way clustered by player's birth country and game. The sample includes 12,168 (11,943) games, 931 (929) players, 184 (184) teams, and 40 (40) countries for the comparison of losing (winning) and draw games. *p < .05; †p < .10.

Figure 3 shows the results without first differencing. The outcomes are the incidence of demonstrations on each day from the game. The effects of close losses are pronounced one to three days after the game, and there is no significant effect on demonstrations before the game. By contrast, the effect of close wins is not statistically significant, and there are subtle (perhaps random) decreases in event probability one to two days before a game. DiD magnifies the subtle differences and makes the estimate positive in Table 2.Footnote 74 In fact, the effect of winning disappears in a later robustness check with an LDV model, which provides a conservative bound.Footnote 75 Overall, we find that barely losing games increase demonstrations, while the effect of winning games is uncertain. These results are not consistent with the rational update, asymmetric mood, or rally mechanism, leaving the mood, misattribution, and asymmetric misattribution mechanisms as possibilities.

Figure 3. Effect by days from a football game (event data analysis)

We also disaggregate the targets, issues, and sizes of demonstrations by using SCAD, which provides more detailed information about each event.Footnote 76 The findings hold with this alternative data set (first column of Figure 4). Moreover, the effects of close losses are particularly large for non-ethnic demonstrations targeted at a central government.Footnote 77 We do not see differences due to the size of the demonstration. These results are inconsistent with the rally mechanism (null effect on ethnic demonstrations) and provide suggestive evidence for the attribution mechanisms (the existence of targeting). Because central governments are more visible, they can be an easy target of misattribution.

Figure 4. Effect by target, issue, and size of demonstration (SCAD data)

Assumption Checks

To check the validity of the assumptions, we first regress the treatment variable on the assigned probabilities based on the betting odds (simple linear regression; Table 3). If the betting odds are accurate measures of assignment probability, the coefficient should be close to 1, while non-systematic measurement errors can cause biases toward 0. If the coefficient is larger than 1 or negative, it indicates the existence of systematic measurement errors and thus invalidates the design. In samples of all games, the coefficients are indeed close to 1, indicating the accuracy of the measurement. Predicting close games is slightly more difficult, reflecting the randomness of their results.

Table 3. Prediction of game results by observed assignment probability

Notes: Coefficient estimates, with corresponding standard errors in parentheses. The samples are those before matching. The standard errors are two-way clustered by player's birth country and game. *p < .05; †p < .10.

Second, we check whether African players’ teams in the matched pairs have similar characteristics in a covariate that is not used in the matching.Footnote 78 To this end, we compare their differences in rank within football leagues. The team ranks are standardized to a 0–100 percentile scale (the smaller the number, the higher the rank), and from the rank of a losing or winning team we subtract the rank of a matched team in a draw game. Teams with high ranks are more likely to win and less likely to lose (Figure 5). Subsetting to close games shrinks the differences (second column of each pane), but there remain differences of over seven percentile points. Matching on betting odds, however, greatly reduces the differences. Even with non-close games (third column of each pane), the imbalances become small, though the team ranks are still different by one percentile point and the p-value is only marginally above the conventional threshold (p = .12). Combining the natural experiment and matching (last column of each pane) eliminates the remaining imbalances.

Figure 5. Differences in pre-game team ranks

Finally, we conduct placebo tests. In Appendix A5, we confirm that close losses or wins have null effects on past demonstrations (in-time placebos). In Appendix A6, we also find that an African player's game has null effects on demonstrations in countries other than his original country (African countries except for his original country, and countries in another continent; in-place placebos). These results lend further credence to the main findings.

Effect Heterogeneity

We also check face validity by subsetting the samples to substantively relevant cases—that is, games of players who are regularly on the pitch (regular players gain more attention), and games in the Champions League (high-stakes games). Figure 6 reports the estimated effects of close losses and wins when we subset the samples by the terciles of players’ season appearances (left) and by league (right). In the left pane, the results are maintained only when players are regularly on the pitch. In the right pane, the point estimates are over three times larger for the Champions League, though the effects are not statistically significant due to the small sample size (only about 3% of the observations are games in the Champions League). In Appendix A8, we further assess effect heterogeneity by splitting the Champions League games into group and knockout stages, looking at the teams’ participation in the Champions League, and incorporating the popularity of football teams in African countries.Footnote 79

Figure 6. Effect heterogeneity (event data analysis)

Robustness Checks

Finally, we conduct a series of robustness checks, which are summarized in Table 4 and detailed in Appendix A11. The results of losing games are robust to those changes, while the results of barely winning games are less stable. Importantly, when we use the LDV model, which provides a conservative bound on the causal effect,Footnote 80 the effect of winning becomes null, and the point estimate shrinks to one-third. These results imply that our findings about winning could be an artifact of DiD.

Table 4. Robustness checks (event data analysis)

Notes: *p < .05; †p < .10.

1. Significant at a 10% level with player-specific week fixed effects, and null with all of the player-specific time fixed effects.

2. Significant at a 10% level with player-specific year fixed effects.

3. Statistically significant at a 10% level for T = 1.

4. Null for T = 1.

5. Significant at a 10% level for 4 out of 40 cases.

Overall, the event data analysis indicates that barely losing games increase peaceful demonstrations, while the effect of barely winning games is uncertain. These results are consistent with the mood, misattribution, and asymmetric misattribution mechanisms (Table 1). The additional analyses—especially those about targets—provide some support for the asymmetric misattribution mechanism. The caveats are that we have not yet directly quantified attribution, moods, or identity, and that the country-level analyses can suffer from aggregation biases. In the following sections, we therefore analyze individual-level surveys.

Survey Analysis: Research Design

We examine the effect of the football games on mood, attitude toward politicians, and nationalistic sentiment by analyzing individual-level surveys from Afrobarometer.Footnote 81 However, as we mentioned, the natural experiment with close games is useful but insufficient for causal identification. Moreover, conditioning on pre-game betting odds is not reliable because there are so few football games during the periods of the Afrobarometer surveys.

We address these problems by exploiting as-if random coincidences of survey interviews and football games. That is, we compare people who are interviewed just a few days before or after a close football game.Footnote 82 Thus, the critical assumption is that the dates of survey interviews are not affected by the results of close football games in Europe. As Depetris-Chauvin, Durante, and Campante argue, this is plausible as “the logistics involved in the implementation of the Afrobarometer survey (selection of the enumeration sites, setting up of the field teams, etc.) requires many months if not years of preparation, and are hardly related to the occurrence of sports events let alone to their unpredictable result.”Footnote 83 The dates of the games are also set at the start of each season and are unlikely to be affected by the Afrobarometer surveys. But because the games might affect non-responses in the surveys, we conduct balance checks, placebo tests, and density tests.

Sample and Unit

The unit of analysis is a triplet of a survey respondent, a player from a country in Africa, and a football game. The pairs of players and football games are organized like those in the event data analysis.Footnote 84 We then link a survey respondent to the football game if the interview was held within three days before or after the game (we also use different time windows in robustness checks), and if players from the respondent's country belong to either team in the game.Footnote 85 The resultant sample of barely losing (winning) and draw games contains 7,659 (7,011) respondents in 15 (12) African countries who are interviewed around the dates of 27 (26) football games of 23 (23) players in 20 (21) teams between the 2005–06 and 2018–19 seasons.Footnote 86 Due to the lack of survey interviews held within the time window, the samples do not cover the Champions League. There are 10,398 observations for the sample of close losses and draws and 9,410 observations for the sample of close wins and draws. The summary statistics are available in Appendix 12.

Treatment Variables

The first treatment variable is the same as that in the event data analysis: a dichotomous variable D ij that takes 1 if African player i's team barely loses or wins game j and 0 if the result is a draw.Footnote 87 The second treatment variable, R jk, takes a value of 1 if respondent k was interviewed after game j and 0 otherwise.

Outcome Variables

The outcome variable W k is respondent k's answer to a given survey question. Following and expanding on Depetris-Chauvin, Durante, and Campante,Footnote 88 we select eighteen Afrobarometer indicators that are relevant to the causal mechanisms and available in a majority of the survey rounds in the period of analysis (2005–2019; third to seventh rounds).Footnote 89 For the attribution mechanisms, we select eight indicators about trust in a leader (president or premier), members of parliament (MPs), local councils, ruling parties, opposition parties, police, army, and courts, as well as three indicators of the performance of leaders, MPs, and local councils.

For the mood mechanisms, we use the interviewer's evaluation of the respondent's attitude during the interview, including friendliness, interest in the survey questions, cooperativeness, patience, ease, and honesty. Although ideally we would like to directly measure the positive and negative moods (e.g., excitement, elation, sadness, and anger) as in experimental studies,Footnote 90 the direct measures are not available. We therefore use the indirect attitudinal measures.Footnote 91 Even though each item does not directly relate to mood and only captures respondents’ positive attitudes, respondents’ moods can be reflected in their attitude, and thus the attitudinal measures can collectively capture respondents’ mood. For this reason, we aggregate the attitudinal measures by calculating the average (“overall mood”). Although this measurement may not be perfect, we believe it is the best available in our observational setup. Finally, following Depetris-Chauvin, Durante, and Campante, we use an item about national identity—a one-dimensional scale of ethnic-national identity. All of the survey answers are rescaled to a 0 to 10 range.Footnote 92

Specification

With these variables, we estimate the average treatment effect local to respondents who answered questions a few days before and after close losses or victories, by taking double differences:

(2)$$\eqalign{& {\rm LATT} = ( {{\rm E}[ {W_k\vert D_{ij} = 1, \;\;R_{\,jk} = 1} ] -{\rm E}[ {W_k\vert D_{ij} = 1, \;\;R_{\,jk} = 0} ] } ) \cr & \quad\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,-\;( {{\rm E}[ {W_k\vert D_{ij} = 0, \;\;R_{\,jk} = 1} ] -{\rm E}[ {W_k\vert D_{ij} = 0, \;\;R_{\,jk} = 0} ] } ) .} $$

The main unit of analysis is respondent k. Player i ≡ i (k) refers to a player born in respondent k's country. Football game j ≡ j (i,k,h) refers to a game player i's team played within h days before or after respondent k's interview. For the reasons we mentioned in the event data analysis, the time window is set to three days (h = 3; see the Specification subsection of the event data analysis and note 61), and we conduct robustness checks with smaller windows. The observations are triangularly weighted by the days from a football game (respondents closer to games have larger weights). Even though this can also be considered a regression discontinuity design (RDD) with a discrete running variable (that is, survey date), we do not include the running variable because the time window is small and thus the regression can cause overfitting problems (see later robustness checks).Footnote 93 Finally, for the reasons we mentioned in the event data analysis (see the Specification subsection of the event data analysis), we do not use fixed effects or control variables and leave it for robustness checks.

We address the problems of multiple hypothesis testing by controlling the false discovery rate.Footnote 94 The standard errors are two-way clustered by country and game. As in the event data analysis, this accounts for repeated observations of games and players. Because each respondent belongs to a single country, clustering by country also accounts for repeated observations of survey respondents (see note 85).

Survey Analysis: Results

Figure 7 reports the results of the survey analysis.Footnote 95 People who answered the questions immediately after barely losing games tend to place less trust in a leader and under-evaluate the leader's performance (Figure 7, left). Because the average value of trust in a leader is 5.33, a close loss decreases it by 23 percent. Barely losing games also sours people's attitudes toward MPs and ruling parties, and, to a lesser extent, local councils and courts. The effect of close losses is nearly zero for trust in opposition parties, police, and the army. These results suggest that people tend to blame visible politicians (leaders, MPs, ruling parties) who are supposed to be responsible for their welfare.

Figure 7. Effect of close losses and wins on public opinion

By contrast, a close victory has little effect on trust or performance evaluation. Although close victories are estimated to decrease trust in a leader by 0.28 points, the effect is not statistically significant, and the effects on some of the other outcomes (for example, trust in ruling parties and performance of MPs) are even positive. Thus, at least statistically, there is no evidence that people blame or credit politicians for close victories. These asymmetric effects of losses and wins are consistent with the asymmetric misattribution mechanism.

Turning to the mood mechanisms (Figure 7, top right), we do not see any large effect on respondents’ observed attitudes. In fact, most of the point estimates and confidence intervals are tightly centered around 0, indicating precise nulls. Although we cannot exclude the mood mechanisms given the limitations of the measurement, we do not find strong evidence. Moreover, the mood mechanisms cannot explain why losing matches lowers trust in a leader while not changing trust in opposition parties, police, or the army. Without any attribution, bad moods should lower trust in any actors.

Similarly, we find only an inconclusive result for the rally mechanism (Figure 7, bottom right). Although the point estimates are positive and relatively large, they are not statistically significant. Thus, even though we cannot deny the possibility that both losing and winning games would unite people through nationalism, the statistical evidence is weak. Overall, the results are the most consistent with the asymmetric misattribution mechanism. The differential effects on political trust indicate that people directly or indirectly attribute bad luck in football games to incumbent politicians.

In Figure 8, we decompose the effect on trust in a leader by days from the game.Footnote 96 The effect of close losses is pronounced one day after the game. This result is consistent with the event data analysis, in which the effect of close losses is large one to three days after the game (Figure 3). This means that a close loss in a football game (which is usually played in the evening) affects people's perceptions the next day (t + 1) and then triggers demonstrations on days t + 1 to t + 3. The lag of up to two days may indicate the time during which tiny unreported events grow into those reported in the media.

Figure 8. Effect by days from a football game (survey analysis)

Mechanisms

The main results are consistent with the asymmetric misattribution mechanism. However, this may or may not mean that people directly attribute bad luck in football games to domestic politicians. A more realistic interpretation is that losing matches reduce respondents’ subjective welfare, which in turn is blamed on politicians (indirect attribution). We therefore analyze the effect of football games on respondents’ evaluation of the future, present, and past economy.Footnote 97 Losing games significantly lowers this evaluation (Figure 9). The fact that football games affect evaluations of the past economy implies that the results do not capture effects on the actual economy but that the games changed respondents’ subjective evaluation of their economic welfare.

Figure 9. Effect of close losses and wins on welfare evaluation

An alternative explanation is that the games attract people to viewing centers, allow them to talk with other people or politicians, and thus create opportunities for mass mobilization. We probe this channel by analyzing the effect on social interactions: the frequency of discussing politics, raising issues, and contacting government officials, MPs, or political parties. Neither losing nor winning games significantly affects the frequency of social interactions (Figure 10). Overall, even though the mobilization mechanism might be consistent with the results of the event data analysis, it is not supported by the survey data, or it cannot fully explain the asymmetric effects on leader approval.

Figure 10. Effect of close losses and wins on social interactions

Assumption Checks

To check the plausibility of the identification assumption, we conduct balance checks, placebo tests, and density tests, all of which are summarized in Table 5. As covariates, we use the eleven objective indicators: age, female, Muslim, Christian, primary education, employment, and access to food, water, medical care, cooking fuel, and cash (all dummies except for age). The third to sixth columns of Table 5 show the number of observations and average values of the covariates for the treated (those interviewed immediately after barely losing or winning games) and control (other respondents in a sample) groups. The seventh and eighth columns show the standardized mean differences and variance ratios of the treated and control groups. As a rule of thumb, a covariate is said to be balanced if the standardized mean difference is between −0.2 and 0.2 and the variance ratio is between 0.5 and 2.Footnote 98 The last column shows the p-values of placebo tests, in which the outcome variable in (2) is replaced by each of the covariates. Finally, the p-values of the density tests are reported at the bottom of each pane.Footnote 99 Although there are a few minor imbalances in four of the sixty-six balance metrics (6%), there is no consistent evidence of imbalance, and the density tests indicate no evidence of sorting. To be sure, we later check robustness by controlling for the covariates.

Table 5. Balance checks, placebo tests, and density tests

Note: If the standardized mean difference is larger than 0.2 or smaller than –0.2, if the variance ratio is larger than 2 or smaller than 0.5 (Rubin 2001), or if p is less than 0.1, the number is bolded.

Effect Heterogeneity 1: Substantive Relevance

As in the event data analysis, we check face validity by subsetting to substantively relevant cases. Because the samples do not contain games in the Champions League (no game was played within three days before or after survey interviews), we only report the results by players’ season appearances.Footnote 100 Consistent with the event data analysis, the effects of losses are large for the games of regular players, while the effects of victories are null (Figure 11).Footnote 101

Figure 11. Effect heterogeneity 1 (survey analysis)

Effect Heterogeneity 2: Demographic Covariates

For explorative purposes, we subset the data by demographic indicators (Figure 12).Footnote 102 First, we use educational attainment to see whether education can reduce cognitive biases. The results provide some evidence—secondary education reduces misattribution—but we do not find equivalent results for higher education. This may be because people with higher education tend to learn liberal ideas and hence are more motivated to blame leaders.

Figure 12. Effect heterogeneity 2 (survey analysis)

Second, we split the data by gender to see whether the results are driven by male respondents. Although the point estimate is slightly larger for men, the difference is small, and close losses affect both men and women (Figure 12, second pane). One possibility is that after losing games, husbands might use violence to vent frustration,Footnote 103 and their wives could attribute their sufferings to political leaders. Because Afrobarometer does not contain information about marital status, we leave it to future studies to further explore this channel.

Third, we subset the samples by Islamic religiosity to indirectly quantify the role of drinking; that is, the results of football games might affect alcohol consumption,Footnote 104 which in turn might induce cognitive biases. Because the Quran prohibits drinking, if there is an effect on Muslim people, the explanation should come from other reasons. The effect of close losses is indeed even more pronounced for Muslim people (Figure 12).

Fourth, we explore whether media usage induces any difference in the causal effects. To this end, we conduct subsample analyses for television and Internet use—primarily as a means to watch live football games. Although only items about media access to news are available in Afrobarometer,Footnote 105 they may still be used as proxies. There are no large differences due to media usage (Figure 12, right). However, this could be due to the coarseness of the proxies. Note also that none of these covariates are exogenous, so the effect heterogeneity is not rigorously identified.

Robustness Checks

Finally, we conduct an array of robustness checks, which are summarized in Table 6 and detailed in Appendix A18. As seen in the table, while the results for close wins are unstable and mostly null, the effects of close losses are robust to most of the changes. The only exception is the inclusion of player-specific fixed effects but this is due to overfitting. In the survey analysis, we examine twenty-seven football games, of which only four are played by the same players. This means that the within-player variation in the treatment variable (the results of the football games) is tiny, and thus that the player-specific fixed effects are overly restrictive. Because the fixed effects are not essential in our identification strategy (see the Specification subsection of the event data analysis), we do not think this would pose particular problems.

Table 6. Robustness checks (survey analysis)

Notes: *p < .05; †p < .10.

1. Due to the numerical instability with the fixed effects, the standard errors are two-way clustered by player (instead of country) and game.

2. Statistically significant at a 5% level with week fixed effects, and with all of the time fixed effects.

3. Statistically significant at a 5% level with player-specific week fixed effects; statistically significant at a 10% level with player-specific month fixed effects.

4. Null with player-specific week fixed effects.

5. Null for h = 3.

6. Significant at a 5% level for 1 of 16 cases, and at a 10% level for 1 of 16 cases.

Discussion

In this paper, we have analyzed the effects of psychological cues on conflicts and built hypotheses about the effects of far-away sports events on local protests. We find that compared to draw games, close losses of African players’ teams increase peaceful demonstrations in their original countries, while they do not change the likelihood of riots or armed conflicts. Such losses are also followed by a temporary decline in people's trust in their political leaders. By contrast, close victories have no equivalent or compensating effects on conflict or public opinion.

These findings provide both evidence and counterevidence for the various hypotheses we have mentioned. First, the rational update mechanism is unlikely to be sufficient; it cannot fully explain why politically irrelevant events affect protests or people's opinion in Africa. Second, the mood and asymmetric mood mechanisms are not strongly supported by our analyses. Data are limited, but we see no discernible effect of football results on the observed attitudes of the respondents. Also, these moods cannot explain the differential effects on political trust. Third, the misattribution mechanism cannot explain the weakly positive and mostly null effect of close wins on demonstrations, or the asymmetric effects on political trust. Fourth, the rally mechanism is also not supported by solid evidence; European football may or may not help people in Africa overcome ethno-religious divisions, but our analysis shows that it does not decrease protests, including those related to ethnic issues. This implies that we cannot simply extend the findings on international sports events to European professional football.

That leaves asymmetric misattribution. The near-null effect of close victories is consistent with the proposition that people tend to perceive victories as their own success and thus do not credit politicians. By contrast, since people tend to unreasonably blame others—including not only coaches and managers but also local politicians—for the failures of their favored teams, close losses should lower their trust in politicians and thus increase protests. The additional analysis of subjective welfare implies that the asymmetric misattribution can be indirect: losing games lower people's subjective welfare, which in turn is blamed on politicians. Finally, the weak finding that close victories increase demonstrations might be explained by self-confidence. That is, if people perceive a victory as their own success, they may become more confident of their own capabilities, which in turn may motivate them to challenge authorities. This, however, is conjecture; the effect of close wins is not robust and may be a false positive.

Substantively, our findings imply that European professional football has an unintended externality across the continents: football in Europe makes people in Africa blame their governments and leads to protests. Fortunately, the effect is limited to peaceful demonstrations; we do not observe equivalent effects on violent riots or armed conflicts. Moreover, we cannot deny the possibility that European football provides a psychological cue for peaceful demonstration and hence incentivizes governments to address social problems. Given these possibilities, we refrain from making hasty judgments. It is a task of future studies to analyze the welfare consequences of the spillover.

Moreover, to the best of our knowledge, this is the first study that extends the insights in the behavioral literature of votingFootnote 106—that is, the effects of seemingly irrelevant events—to conflict studies and international relations. Although previous studies have analyzed the psychological causes of conflicts by conceptual discussion, correlational analyses, and survey experiments,Footnote 107 they lack causal evidence with real-world data. We have filled this gap by analyzing events that are not directly relevant to rational updates. Future studies will likely apply the research design to other topics and provide further evidence on the psychological origins of conflict.

Finally, to be clear, we do not intend to imply that people in Africa are particularly irrational. Previous studies have shown that people in the United States and elsewhere also use irrational attribution.Footnote 108 We extend the analysis to Africa. What this paper implies is therefore that human beings are similarly irrational, whether they live in Africa or elsewhere, so conflict studies and theories of international relations need to account for it.

Data Availability Statement

Replication files for this article may be found at <https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/HWVUER>.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary material for this article is available at <https://doi.org/10.1017/S0020818322000261>.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Ross Buchanan, Charles Crabtree, Michael Denly, Michael G. Findley, John Gerring, Takuma Kamada, Jay C. Kao, Tetsuya Matsubayashi, Takeshi Murooka, Xin Nong, the two anonymous reviewers, and the editors of IO for thoughtful comments. This paper was presented at the 2021 Summer Meeting Program of the Japanese Society for Quantitative Political Science.

Footnotes

2. Quoted in Ka, Od, and I Reference Ka, Od and Shadrach2018, 7, 8.

3. Bertoli Reference Bertoli2017; Depetris-Chauvin, Durante, and Campante Reference Depetris-Chauvin, Durante and Campante2020.

5. McAdam, Tarrow, and Tilly Reference McAdam, Tarrow and Tilly2003.

8. Ashworth, Bueno de Mesquita, and Friedenberg Reference Ashworth, de Mesquita and Friedenberg2018. See also Achen and Bartels Reference Achen and Bartels2017, Reference Achen and Bartels2018; Fowler and Hall Reference Fowler and Hall2016; Healy and Malhotra Reference Healy and Malhotra2009, Reference Healy and Malhotra2010, Reference Healy and Malhotra2013; Malhotra and Kuo Reference Malhotra and Kuo2008; Ramos and Sanz Reference Ramos and Sanz2020. Other studies examine Christmas lotteries (Bagues and Esteve-Volart Reference Bagues and Esteve-Volart2016), sunshine (Bassi Reference Bassi2013, Reference Bassi2019), and foreign aid reception (Cruz and Schneider Reference Cruz and Schneider2017).

9. Ashworth, Bueno de Mesquita, and Friedenberg Reference Ashworth, de Mesquita and Friedenberg2018.

11. Card and Dahl Reference Card and Dahl2011. Previous studies analyze the effects of sports on court judgments (Eren and Mocan Reference Eren and Mocan2018), electoral turnout (Potoski and Urbatsch Reference Potoski and Urbatsch2016), stock returns (Edmans, García, and Norli Reference Edmans, García and Norli2007), sexual assaults (Lindo, Siminski, and Swensen Reference Lindo, Siminski and Swensen2018), hooliganism (Priks Reference Priks2010; Wann et al. Reference Wann, Melnick, Russell and Pease2001), crimes (Ge, Barbieri, and Schneider Reference Ge, Barbieri and Schneider2021; Kalist and Lee Reference Kalist and Lee2016; Marie Reference Marie2016; Munyo and Rossi Reference Munyo and Rossi2013; Rees and Schnepel Reference Rees and Schnepel2009), and unhealthy eating (Cornil and Chandon Reference Cornil and Chandon2013), among other things.

12. Bertoli Reference Bertoli2017; Depetris-Chauvin, Durante, and Campante Reference Depetris-Chauvin, Durante and Campante2020; Rosenzweig and Zhou, Reference Rosenzweig and Zhou2021.

14. There are a few counterexamples in history, such as prewar Italy, which dominated the World Cup in the 1930s but was defeated in World War II. Moreover, FIFA bans government interference in its countries’ football associations.

15. Hirshleifer and Shumway Reference Hirshleifer and Shumway2003; Meier, Schmid, and Stutzer Reference Meier, Schmid and Stutzer2016.

21. Ka, Od, and I Reference Ka, Od and Shadrach2018, 2.

22. Kombol and Kombol Reference Kombol and Kombol2015.

23. In Appendix A1, we also provide the number of players from each country.

24. Ungruhe and Schmidt Reference Ungruhe and Schmidt2020.

27. Harris and Hern Reference Harris and Hern2019.

28. Salehyan and Hendrix Reference Salehyan and Hendrix2017.

30. Bikhchandani, Hirshleifer, and Welch Reference Bikhchandani, Hirshleifer and Welch1992; Chenoweth and Belgioioso Reference Chenoweth and Belgioioso2019.

31. McAdam, Tarrow, and Tilly Reference McAdam, Tarrow and Tilly2003.

32. This certainly does not mean that the effects of European football would be limited to protests; as numerous studies show, sports events have broad consequences (see note 11). Our argument is that sports events can affect even costlier political actions—protests.

33. Ashworth, Bueno de Mesquita, and Friedenberg Reference Ashworth, de Mesquita and Friedenberg2018; Lohmann Reference Lohmann1993.

34. Hirshleifer and Shumway Reference Hirshleifer and Shumway2003; Meier, Schmid, and Stutzer Reference Meier, Schmid and Stutzer2016.

35. See note 11 for previous studies.

38. Gilbert and Malone Reference Gilbert and Malone1995 call this the correspondence bias.

39. Achen and Bartels Reference Achen and Bartels2017 call this blind retrospection; see note 8 for debate.

40. Healy, Malhotra, and Mo Reference Healy, Malhotra and Mo2010.

41. Miller and Ross Reference Miller and Ross1975.

42. To be sure, any attribution of this type is irrational; people unreasonably blame or praise politicians for irrelevant events.

46. Games with penalty shoot-outs (only in the Champions League) are also included.

47. Card and Dahl Reference Card and Dahl2011. See Anderson Reference Anderson2017 for methodological details, and note 11 for applications.

48. Wunderlich and Memmert Reference Wunderlich and Memmert2018.

49. Angrist and Pischke Reference Angrist and Pischke2009.

50. Even though recent studies have proposed various extensions of DiD (see Xu Reference Xu2022 for a review), especially those related to the problems in two-way fixed effects (de Chaisemartin and D'Haultfœuille Reference De Chaisemartin and D'Haultfœuille2020; Imai and Kim Reference Imai and Kim2019, Reference Imai and Kim2021; Imai, Kim, and Wang, Reference Imai, Kim and Wang2021), DiD is the least important element in our identification (the natural experiment and conditioning are the critical parts). Moreover, we use a simple two-by-two DiD, which is free from the problems related to two-way fixed effects. Finally, as later analyses indicate, the effects of football games disappear in three days, so it is unlikely that the effect of a game would be carried over to the next game.

51. Miguel, Saiegh, and Satyanath Reference Miguel, Saiegh and Satyanath2011.

52. We control for African players’ goals, assists, yellow cards, and red cards in a robustness check.

53. We use birth countries because each player has only one birth country and thus the data configuration becomes simple. We later conduct a robustness check using player citizenship.

54. If there are multiple players from a given African country on a single team, each player-game is counted as a separate observation. We account for the repetition by clustering the standard errors. In a robustness check, we also conduct an analysis aggregated at the country-game level.

55. The betting odds data are available only after 2005, and COVID-19 started in 2020.

56. We later conduct a robustness check by further dropping observations if there are both close losses and wins on the same day for a given country.

57. We later conduct a robustness check by including non-close games as well.

58. The sample contains (1) games in which African players’ teams closely won, (2) draw games, and (3) games in which African players’ team closely lost. The split is then between (1) + (2), versus (3) + (2). We split the sample because matching on a trichotomous variable is not straightforward.

59. In Appendix A2, we show that the affiliation of an African player with a team significantly increases the number of fans in Africa.

60. We later conduct a robustness check by using alternative measures of the outcome variable.

61. Because football games are played every week, time windows larger than 3 result in overlaps.

63. For the definitions and measurements, refer to ACLED 2019. Exact dates are available for 89 percent of the protest and riot events.

64. For definition and measurement, refer to Appendix A3 and to Salehyan and Hendrix Reference Salehyan and Hendrix2017.

65. The event dates in SCAD are best estimates by the coders. But SCAD does not indicate the precision of these estimates.

66. Weidmann Reference Weidmann2016, 208.

67. Because the assignment probabilities are directly measured, we use the simple matching method.

68. In equation (1) the weekend effects, say $\rho $, exist both in the first and second terms of the right-hand side, and thus they cancel out $(\rho -\rho = 0)$.

70. Angrist and Pischke Reference Angrist and Pischke2009, 225.

71. Hill et al. Reference Hill, Davis, Roos and French2020. For similar reasons, we use DiD instead of the lagged dependent variable model, even though DiD can overstate effect sizes (Angrist and Pischke Reference Angrist and Pischke2009; Keele Reference Keele, Curini and Franzese2020).

72. The average values for riots and battles are 0.67 and 0.44, respectively.

73. Ge, Barbieri, and Schneider Reference Ge, Barbieri and Schneider2021.

74. See Angrist and Pischke Reference Angrist and Pischke2009, 243–47 and Keele Reference Keele, Curini and Franzese2020 on the problems of DiD.

75. Angrist and Pischke Reference Angrist and Pischke2009.

76. For details of the event categories, see Appendix A3.

77. We obtain similar results on ethnic conflicts with ACLED (Appendix A7).

78. Matching eliminates differences in the inverse of betting odds. The mean differences are 0.16 percentage points for close losses and draws and −0.02 percentage points for close wins and draws.

79. The effect heterogeneity by prior expectations (reference dependence; Köszegi and Rabin Reference Köszegi and Rabin2006) is reported in Appendix A9. The effect heterogeneity by the margins of losses and wins is reported in Appendix A9 as well. The effect heterogeneity by leagues, regions, and time periods is reported in Appendix A10.

80. Angrist and Pischke Reference Angrist and Pischke2009.

81. Afrobarometer 2019.

82. Muñoz, Falcó-Gimeno, and Hernández Reference Muñoz, Falcó-Gimeno and Hernández2020.

83. Depetris-Chauvin, Durante, and Campante Reference Depetris-Chauvin, Durante and Campante2020, 1581. See Afrobarometer 2019 for details of survey implementation. See also Eifert, Miguel, and Posner Reference Eifert, Miguel and Posner2010.

84. We later conduct a robustness check by using players’ citizenships.

85. If there are multiple games within three days before or after an interview, we include the interview only if all of the games are played either before or after the interview. (If there are games both before and after an interview, the treatment is ill-defined. If we were to include such observations as treated units, it would artificially increase the number of treated units and result in bunching in the density tests.) We then select the nearest games and assign their treatment status accordingly. If multiple games are selected, or if there are multiple co-national players in a game, we count each respondent-player-game as a separate observation. We account for the repeated observations by clustering the standard errors. We also check the robustness with the data aggregated at a respondent-game level.

86. As in the event data analysis, we drop games if both sides have players from the same country in Africa. We later conduct a robustness check by dropping observations where there are both close losses and wins on the same day for a given respondent. We also drop observations on the day of the game because their treatment status is indeterminate. Finally, we drop a game if all relevant interviews were conducted either before or after the game, because all respondents are either treated or control units.

87. We later conduct a robustness check by splitting the sample into those with and without African players’ appearance.

88. Depetris-Chauvin, Durante, and Campante Reference Depetris-Chauvin, Durante and Campante2020.

89. The exact survey questions are given in Appendix A13.

90. Busby and Druckman Reference Busby and Druckman2018; Busby, Druckman, and Fredendall Reference Busby, Druckman and Fredendall2016.

91. Unlike Depetris-Chauvin, Durante, and Campante Reference Depetris-Chauvin, Durante and Campante2020, we do not use respondents’ evaluation of the economy as a measure of mood. As we show in an additional analysis, the item is more closely related to the evaluation of welfare—a key factor in indirect attribution.

92. We also standardize the outcomes, and it does not change the results.

93. Unlike continuous RDDs, automatic bandwidth selection cannot be used in discrete RDDs (Imbens and Kalyanaraman Reference Imbens and Kalyanaraman2012). Imbens and Wager Reference Imbens and Wager2018 and Kolesár and Rothe Reference Kolesár and Rothe2018 propose inferential frameworks for discrete RDDs, but they do not provide methods for bandwidth selection. Cattaneo, Frandsen, and Titiunik Reference Cattaneo, Frandsen and Titiunik2015 propose a covariate-based method, but the method does not account for clustering. Without clustering SEs, the null hypotheses are over-rejected.

94. Benjamini and Hochberg Reference Benjamini and Hochberg1995. Using the other methods of adjustment does not change the results. Applying the same adjustment to the event data analysis also does not change the results.

95. The detailed tables are available in Appendix A14. We do not find that having a football game itself has significant effects on the outcomes.

96. We estimate the effect of close losses and wins for each day from a football game. Because the natural experiment with close games alone can produce biased estimates, we reduce the estimate by the pre-game average of the estimated effect to account for any nonrandom assignment of the game results. The results of the other outcomes are provided on request.

97. See Appendix A13 for exact survey questions. See note 91 about the relationship to moods.

99. In the density tests, we use the respondent as the unit of analysis because it is not straightforward to cluster the standard errors. We also include observations on the day of the football game because we cannot otherwise implement the density tests.

100. The other analyses on effect heterogeneity at a team level (for example, a team's participation in the Champions League or its popularity) are not feasible as the sample contains less than twenty teams.

101. The effect heterogeneity by prior expectations (reference dependence) is reported in Appendix A15. The effect heterogeneity by the margins of losses and wins is reported in Appendix A15 as well. The effect heterogeneity by regions and time periods is reported in Appendix A16.

102. The effect heterogeneity by other (less relevant) covariates is reported in Appendix A17.

103. Card and Dahl Reference Card and Dahl2011.

104. Lindo, Siminski, and Swensen Reference Lindo, Siminski and Swensen2018; Rees and Schnepel Reference Rees and Schnepel2009; Wood, McInnes, and Norton Reference Wood, McInnes and Norton2011.

105. The survey question is “How often do you get news from the following sources: [television/internet]” (Afrobarometer 2019).

106. Healy and Malhotra Reference Healy and Malhotra2013.

References

Achen, Christopher H., and Bartels, Larry M.. 2017. Democracy for Realists: Why Elections Do Not Produce Responsive Government. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Achen, Christopher H., and Bartels, Larry M.. 2018. Statistics As If Politics Mattered: A Reply to Fowler and Hall. Journal of Politics 80 (4):1438–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
ACLED. 2019. Armed Conflict Location and Event Data Project (ACLED) Codebook. Available at <https://acleddata.com/acleddatanew/wp-content/uploads/dlm_uploads/2019/01/ACLED_Codebook_2019FINAL.docx.pdf>..>Google Scholar
Afrobarometer. 2019. Afrobarometer Data: Rounds 2–7. Available at <https://www.afrobarometer.org/data/merged-data/>..>Google Scholar
Alrababah, Ala, Marble, William, Mousa, Salma, and Siegel, Alexandra. 2019. Can Exposure to Celebrities Reduce Prejudice? The Effect of Mohamed Salah on Islamophobic Behaviors and Attitudes. Stanford Immigration Policy Lab, Working Paper No. 19–04. <https://doi.org/10.31235/osf.io/eq8ca>.CrossRef.>Google Scholar
Anderson, Michael L. 2017. The Benefits of College Athletic Success: An Application of the Propensity Score Design. Review of Economics and Statistics 99 (1):119–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Angrist, Joshua D., and Pischke, Jörn-Steffen. 2009. Mostly Harmless Econometrics: An Empiricist's Companion. Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ashworth, Scott, de Mesquita, Ethan Bueno, and Friedenberg, Amanda. 2018. Learning About Voter Rationality. American Journal of Political Science 62 (1):3754.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bagues, Manuel, and Esteve-Volart, Berta. 2016. Politicians’ Luck of the Draw: Evidence from the Spanish Christmas Lottery. Journal of Political Economy 124 (5):1269–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bassi, Anna. 2013. Weather, Mood, and Voting: An Experimental Analysis of the Effect of Weather Beyond Turnout. <https://papers.ssrn.com/abstract=2273189>.CrossRef.>Google Scholar
Bassi, Anna. 2019. Weather, Risk, and Voting: An Experimental Analysis of the Effect of Weather on Vote Choice. Journal of Experimental Political Science 6 (1):1732.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benjamini, Yoav, and Hochberg, Yosef. 1995. Controlling the False Discovery Rate: A Practical and Powerful Approach to Multiple Testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Methodological) 57 (1):289300.Google Scholar
Bertoli, Andrew D. 2017. Nationalism and Conflict: Lessons from International Sports. International Studies Quarterly 61 (4):835–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bikhchandani, Sushil, Hirshleifer, David, and Welch, Ivo. 1992. A Theory of Fads, Fashion, Custom, and Cultural Change as Informational Cascades. Journal of Political Economy 100 (5):9921026.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Busby, Ethan C., and Druckman, James N.. 2018. Football and Public Opinion: A Partial Replication and Extension. Journal of Experimental Political Science 5 (1):410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Busby, Ethan C., Druckman, James N., and Fredendall, Alexandria. 2016. The Political Relevance of Irrelevant Events. Journal of Politics 79 (1):346–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Card, David, and Dahl, Gordon B.. 2011. Family Violence and Football: The Effect of Unexpected Emotional Cues on Violent Behavior. Quarterly Journal of Economics 126 (1):103–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cattaneo, Matias D., Frandsen, Brigham R., and Titiunik, Rocío. 2015. Randomization Inference in the Regression Discontinuity Design: An Application to Party Advantages in the US Senate. Journal of Causal Inference 3 (1):124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chenoweth, Erica, and Belgioioso, Margherita. 2019. The Physics of Dissent and the Effects of Movement Momentum. Nature Human Behaviour 3 (10):1088–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cialdini, Robert, Borden, Richard, Thorne, Avril, Walker, Marcus, Freeman, Stephen, and Sloan, Lloyd. 1976. Basking in Reflected Glory: Three (Football) Field Studies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 34 (September): 366–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cornil, Yann, and Chandon, Pierre. 2013. From Fan to Fat? Vicarious Losing Increases Unhealthy Eating, but Self-Affirmation Is an Effective Remedy. Psychological Science 24 (10):1936–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cruz, Cesi, and Schneider, Christina J.. 2017. Foreign Aid and Undeserved Credit Claiming. American Journal of Political Science 61 (2):396408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, James W., and McDermott, Rose. 2021. The Past, Present, and Future of Behavioral IR. International Organization 75 (1):147–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Chaisemartin, Clément, and D'Haultfœuille, Xavier. 2020. Two-Way Fixed Effects Estimators with Heterogeneous Treatment Effects. American Economic Review 110 (9):2964–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Depetris-Chauvin, Emilio, Durante, Ruben, and Campante, Filipe. 2020. Building Nations Through Shared Experiences: Evidence from African Football. American Economic Review 110 (5):15721602.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edmans, Alex, García, Diego, and Norli, Øyvind. 2007. Sports Sentiment and Stock Returns. Journal of Finance 62 (4):1967–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eifert, Benn, Miguel, Edward, and Posner, Daniel N.. 2010. Political Competition and Ethnic Identification in Africa. American Journal of Political Science 54 (2):494510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eren, Ozkan, and Mocan, Naci. 2018. Emotional Judges and Unlucky Juveniles. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics 10 (3):171205.Google Scholar
Fearon, James D. 2004. Why Do Some Civil Wars Last So Much Longer than Others? Journal of Peace Research 41 (3):275301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fowler, Anthony, and Hall, Andrew B.. 2016. Do Shark Attacks Influence Presidential Elections? Reassessing a Prominent Finding on Voter Competence. Journal of Politics 80 (4):1423–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fowler, Anthony, and Montagnes, B. Pablo. 2015. College Football, Elections, and False-Positive Results in Observational Research. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 112 (45):13800–804.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ge, Qi, Barbieri, Ignacio Sarmiento, and Schneider, Rodrigo. 2021. Sporting Events, Emotional Cues, and Crime: Spatial and Temporal Evidence from Brazilian Soccer Games. Economic Inquiry 59 (1):375–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilbert, Daniel, and Malone, Patrick. 1995. The Correspondence Bias. Psychological Bulletin 117 (February):2138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graham, Matthew, Huber, Gregory, Malhotra, Neil, and Mo, Cecilla Hyunjung. Forthcoming. Irrelevant Events and Voting Behavior: Replications Using Principles from Open Science. Journal of Politics.Google Scholar
Gurr, Ted Robert. 1970. Why Men Rebel. Paradigm.Google Scholar
Hafner-Burton, Emilie M., Haggard, Stephan, Lake, David A., and Victor, David G.. 2017. The Behavioral Revolution and International Relations. International Organization 71 (S1):S131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, Adam S., and Hern, Erin. 2019. Taking to the Streets: Protest as an Expression of Political Preference in Africa. Comparative Political Studies 52 (8):1169–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Healy, Andrew, and Malhotra, Neil. 2009. Myopic Voters and Natural Disaster Policy. American Political Science Review 103 (3):387406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Healy, Andrew, and Malhotra, Neil. 2010. Random Events, Economic Losses, and Retrospective Voting: Implications for Democratic Competence. Quarterly Journal of Political Science 5 (2):193208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Healy, Andrew, and Malhotra, Neil. 2013. Retrospective Voting Reconsidered. Annual Review of Political Science 16 (1):285306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Healy, Andrew, Malhotra, Neil, and Mo, Cecilia Hyunjung. 2010. Irrelevant Events Affect Voters’ Evaluations of Government Performance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 107 (29):12804–809.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Healy, Andrew, Malhotra, Neil, and Mo, Cecilia Hyunjung. 2015. Determining False-Positives Requires Considering the Totality of Evidence. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 112 (48):E6591.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hill, Terrence D., Davis, Andrew P., Roos, J. Micah, and French, Michael T.. 2020. Limitations of Fixed-Effects Models for Panel Data. Sociological Perspectives 63 (3):357–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hirshleifer, David, and Shumway, Tyler. 2003. Good Day Sunshine: Stock Returns and the Weather. Journal of Finance 58 (3):1009–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Imai, Kosuke, and Kim, In Song. 2019. When Should We Use Unit Fixed Effects Regression Models for Causal Inference with Longitudinal Data? American Journal of Political Science 63 (2):467–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Imai, Kosuke, and Kim, In Song. 2021. On the Use of Two-Way Fixed Effects Regression Models for Causal Inference with Panel Data. Political Analysis 29 (3):405–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Imai, Kosuke, Kim, In Song, and Wang, Erik H.. 2021. Matching Methods for Causal Inference with Time-Series Cross-Sectional Data. American Journal of Political Science. <https://doi.org/10.1111/ajps.12685>.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Imbens, Guido, and Kalyanaraman, Karthik. 2012. Optimal Bandwidth Choice for the Regression Discontinuity Estimator. Review of Economic Studies 79 (3):933–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Imbens, Guido, and Wager, Stefan. 2018. Optimized Regression Discontinuity Designs. Review of Economics and Statistics 101 (2):264–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jasper, James M. 2011. Emotions and Social Movements: Twenty Years of Theory and Research. Annual Review of Sociology 37 (1):285303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jiménez, Luis F. 2016. Politics on the Pitch: The Impact Political Regimes Have on FIFA World Cup Success at Younger and Senior Levels. Social Science Quarterly 97 (5):1295–314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ka, Dogari, Od, Apuke, and Shadrach, I. 2018. Comfort or Conflict? Investigating the Attitude and Experiences of European Football Fans in Television Viewing Centers in Nigeria. Global Media Journal 16 (30):110.Google Scholar
Kalist, David E., and Lee, Daniel Y.. 2016. The National Football League: Does Crime Increase on Game Day? Journal of Sports Economics 17 (8):863–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keele, Luke. 2020. Differences-in-Differences: Neither Natural nor an Experiment. In The Sage Handbook of Research Methods in Political Science and International Relations, edited by Curini, Luigi and Franzese, Robert, 822–34. Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kertzer, Joshua D., and Tingley, Dustin. 2018. Political Psychology in International Relations: Beyond the Paradigms. Annual Review of Political Science 21 (1):319–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kolesár, Michal, and Rothe, Christoph. 2018. Inference in Regression Discontinuity Designs with a Discrete Running Variable. American Economic Review 108 (8):2277–304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kombol, Michael Aondo-verr, and Kombol, Esther Wuese. 2015. Significance of Satellite Television Viewing Centres in Makurdi, Nigeria. New Media and Mass Communication 39:6375.Google Scholar
Köszegi, Botond, and Rabin, Matthew. 2006. A Model of Reference-Dependent Preferences. Quarterly Journal of Economics 121 (4):1133–65.Google Scholar
Kuran, Timur. 1991. Now out of Never: The Element of Surprise in the East European Revolution of 1989. World Politics 44 (1):748.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leeds, Michael A., and Leeds, Eva Marikova. 2009. International Soccer Success and National Institutions. Journal of Sports Economics 10 (4):369–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lefgren, Lars J., Platt, Brennan, Price, Joseph, and Higbee, Samuel. 2019. Outcome Based Accountability: Theory and Evidence. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization 160 (April):121–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lindo, Jason M., Siminski, Peter, and Swensen, Isaac D.. 2018. College Party Culture and Sexual Assault. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics 10 (1):236–65.Google Scholar
Lohmann, Susanne. 1993. A Signaling Model of Informative and Manipulative Political Action. American Political Science Review 87 (2):319–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malhotra, Neil, and Kuo, Alexander G.. 2008. Attributing Blame: The Public's Response to Hurricane Katrina. Journal of Politics 70 (1):120–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malik, Sohai. 2021. English Clubs Battle for Africa Fanbase. Aljazeera, 16 July. Available at <https://www.aljazeera.com/sports/2012/7/16/english-clubs-battle-for-africa-fanbase>..>Google Scholar
Marie, Olivier. 2016. Police and Thieves in the Stadium: Measuring the (Multiple) Effects of Football Matches on Crime. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. Series A: Statistics in Society 179 (1):273–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McAdam, Doug, Tarrow, Sidney, and Tilly, Charles. 2003. Dynamics of Contention. Social Movement Studies 2 (1):99102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meier, Armando, Schmid, Lukas, and Stutzer, Alois. 2016. Rain, Emotions and Voting for the Status Quo. <https://papers.ssrn.com/abstract=2868316>.CrossRef.>Google Scholar
Miguel, Edward, Saiegh, Sebastián M., and Satyanath, Shanker. 2011. Civil War Exposure and Violence. Economics and Politics 23 (1):5973.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, Dale T., and Ross, Michael. 1975. Self-Serving Biases in the Attribution of Causality: Fact or Fiction? Psychological Bulletin 82 (2):213–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, Michael K. 2013. For the Win! The Effect of Professional Sports Records on Mayoral Elections. Social Science Quarterly 94 (1):5978.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Monks, Kieron. 2016. New TV Deal Brings Free Premier League Games to Fifty African Countries. CNN, 29 March. Available at <https://edition.cnn.com/2016/03/29/football/premier-league-econet-africa/>..>Google Scholar
Mousa, Salma. 2020. Building Social Cohesion Between Christians and Muslims Through Soccer in Post-ISIS Iraq. Science 369 (6505):866–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mueller, John E. 1970. Presidential Popularity from Truman to Johnson. American Political Science Review 64 (1):1834.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muñoz, Jordi, Falcó-Gimeno, Albert, and Hernández, Enrique. 2020. Unexpected Event During Survey Design: Promise and Pitfalls for Causal Inference. Political Analysis 28 (2):186206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Munyo, Ignacio, and Rossi, Martín A.. 2013. Frustration, Euphoria, and Violent Crime. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization 89 (May):136–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Potoski, Matthew, and Urbatsch, R.. 2016. Entertainment and the Opportunity Cost of Civic Participation: Monday Night Football Game Quality Suppresses Turnout in US Elections. Journal of Politics 79 (2):424–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Priks, Mikael. 2010. Does Frustration Lead to Violence? Evidence from the Swedish Hooligan Scene. Kyklos 63 (3):450–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raleigh, Clionadh, Linke, Andrew, Hegre, Håvard, and Karlsen, Joakim. 2010. Introducing ACLED: An Armed Conflict Location and Event Dataset Special Data Feature. Journal of Peace Research 47 (5):651–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramos, Roberto, and Sanz, Carlos. 2020. Backing the Incumbent in Difficult Times: The Electoral Impact of Wildfires. Comparative Political Studies 53 (3-4):469–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rees, Daniel I., and Schnepel, Kevin T.. 2009. College Football Games and Crime. Journal of Sports Economics 10 (1):6887.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenzweig, Leah R., and Zhou, Yang-Yang. 2021. Team and Nation: Sports, Nationalism, and Attitudes Toward Refugees. Comparative Political Studies 54 (12):<https://doi.org/10.1177/0010414021997498>.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rubin, Donald B. 2001. Using Propensity Scores to Help Design Observational Studies: Application to the Tobacco Litigation. Health Services and Outcomes Research Methodology 2 (3-4):169–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salehyan, Idean, and Hendrix, Cullen S.. 2017. Social Conflict Analysis Database (SCAD) Version 3.3: Codebook and Coding Procedures. Available at <https://www.strausscenter.org/wp-content/uploads/SCAD_33_Codebook.pdf>..>Google Scholar
Scharrer, Bill. 2017. Letter: Sports Is a Drug. Roanoke Times, 26 August.Google Scholar
Tullock, Gordon. 1971. The Paradox of Revolution. Public Choice 11 (1):8999.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ungruhe, Christian, and Schmidt, Mads Backer. 2020. Why Are East African Players Absent in European Football? Localizing African Football Migration Along Structural Constraints, Colonial Legacies and Voluntary Immobility. Journal of Sport and Social Issues 44 (5):397420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wann, Daniel L., Melnick, Merrill J., Russell, Gordon W., and Pease, Dale G.. 2001. Sport Fans: The Psychology and Social Impact of Spectators. Routledge.Google Scholar
Weidmann, Nils B. 2016. A Closer Look at Reporting Bias in Conflict Event Data. American Journal of Political Science 60 (1):206–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wood, Stacy, McInnes, Melayne, and Norton, David. 2011. The Bad Thing About Good Games: The Relationship Between Close Sporting Events and Game-Day Traffic Fatalities. Journal of Consumer Research 38 (December):611–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wunderlich, Fabian, and Memmert, Daniel. 2018. The Betting Odds Rating System: Using Soccer Forecasts to Forecast Soccer. PLOS ONE 13 (6):e0198668.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xu, Yiqing. 2022. Causal Inference with Time-Series Cross-Sectional Data: A Reflection. <https://papers.ssrn.com/abstract=3979613>.CrossRef.>Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. The rapid increase of African players in European professional football

Figure 1

Table 1. Causal mechanisms and predictions

Figure 2

Figure 2. Unit of analysis and variables

Figure 3

Table 2. Effect of close losses and wins on conflict incidence

Figure 4

Figure 3. Effect by days from a football game (event data analysis)

Figure 5

Figure 4. Effect by target, issue, and size of demonstration (SCAD data)

Figure 6

Table 3. Prediction of game results by observed assignment probability

Figure 7

Figure 5. Differences in pre-game team ranks

Figure 8

Figure 6. Effect heterogeneity (event data analysis)

Figure 9

Table 4. Robustness checks (event data analysis)

Figure 10

Figure 7. Effect of close losses and wins on public opinion

Figure 11

Figure 8. Effect by days from a football game (survey analysis)

Figure 12

Figure 9. Effect of close losses and wins on welfare evaluation

Figure 13

Figure 10. Effect of close losses and wins on social interactions

Figure 14

Table 5. Balance checks, placebo tests, and density tests

Figure 15

Figure 11. Effect heterogeneity 1 (survey analysis)

Figure 16

Figure 12. Effect heterogeneity 2 (survey analysis)

Figure 17

Table 6. Robustness checks (survey analysis)

Supplementary material: Link

Kikuta and Uesugi Dataset

Link
Supplementary material: PDF

Kikuta and Uesugi supplementary material

Online Appendix

Download Kikuta and Uesugi supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 1.8 MB