Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dsjbd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T20:16:23.777Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Significant association between intracranial volume and verbal intellectual abilities in patients with schizophrenia and a history of birth asphyxia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 March 2021

Laura Anne Wortinger*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatric Research, Diakonhjemmet Hospital, Oslo, Norway NORMENT, Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
Kjetil Nordbø Jørgensen
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatric Research, Diakonhjemmet Hospital, Oslo, Norway NORMENT, Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
Claudia Barth
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatric Research, Diakonhjemmet Hospital, Oslo, Norway NORMENT, Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
Stener Nerland
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatric Research, Diakonhjemmet Hospital, Oslo, Norway NORMENT, Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
Runar Elle Smelror
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatric Research, Diakonhjemmet Hospital, Oslo, Norway NORMENT, Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
Anja Vaskinn
Affiliation:
NORMENT, Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway Division of Mental Health and Addiction, NORMENT, Oslo University Hospital, Oslo, Norway
Torill Ueland
Affiliation:
NORMENT, Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway Division of Mental Health and Addiction, NORMENT, Oslo University Hospital, Oslo, Norway Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
Ole A. Andreassen
Affiliation:
NORMENT, Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway Division of Mental Health and Addiction, NORMENT, Oslo University Hospital, Oslo, Norway
Ingrid Agartz
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatric Research, Diakonhjemmet Hospital, Oslo, Norway NORMENT, Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Centre for Psychiatric Research, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
*
Author for correspondence: Laura Anne Wortinger, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

The etiology of schizophrenia (SZ) is proposed to include an interplay between a genetic risk for disease development and the biological environment of pregnancy and birth, where early adversities may contribute to the poorer developmental outcome. We investigated whether a history of birth asphyxia (ASP) moderates the relationship between intracranial volume (ICV) and intelligence in SZ, bipolar disorder (BD) and healthy controls (HC).

Methods

Two hundred seventy-nine adult patients (18–42 years) on the SZ and BD spectrums and 216 HC were evaluated for ASP based on information from the Medical Birth Registry of Norway. Participants underwent structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) to estimate ICV and intelligence quotient (IQ) assessment using the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI). Multiple linear regressions were used for analyses.

Results

We found a significant three-way interaction (ICV × ASP × diagnosis) on the outcome variable, IQ, indicating that the correlation between ICV and IQ was stronger in patients with SZ who experienced ASP compared to SZ patients without ASP. This moderation by ASP was not found in BD or HC groups. In patients with SZ, the interaction between ICV and a history of the ASP was specifically related to the verbal subcomponent of IQ as measured by WASI.

Conclusions

The significant positive association between ICV and IQ in patients with SZ who had experienced ASP might indicate abnormal neurodevelopment. Our findings give support for ICV together with verbal intellectual abilities as clinically relevant markers that can be added to prediction tools to enhance evaluations of SZ risk.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re- use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Schizophrenia (SZ) is a complex mental disorder that occurs globally with an international prevalence estimate of 0.33–0.75% (NIMH, 2018). A recent study reports that the most significant genetic variants that contribute to SZ risk were found to interact with the biological environment of pregnancy and birth (Ursini et al., Reference Ursini, Punzi, Chen, Marenco, Robinson, Porcelli and Weinberger2018). Specifically, SZ risk-associated genes differentially expressed in placentae were significantly enriched for pathways related to metabolic and cellular stress and hypoxia (Ursini et al., Reference Ursini, Punzi, Chen, Marenco, Robinson, Porcelli and Weinberger2018). These findings suggest that part of the genetic predisposition for SZ may relate to intolerance to hypoxic stress.

Worldwide, newborn infants suffer permanent brain damage or death as a result of hypoxia (i.e. deficiency in the amount of oxygen reaching tissues) caused by birth asphyxia (ASP) – a condition where the newborn has been deprived of oxygen before or around the time of birth (i.e. peripartum period). Neonatal hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy (HIE) is a profound systemic and neurological consequence of decreased blood flow and/or oxygen to the brain and the sequelae of ASP. Not all incidences of ASP end in a diagnosis of HIE, which can take a day or more to conclude, and a diagnosis of HIE is graded as mild, moderate or severe. Most infants with neonatal HIE and hypoxic-ischemic insult evident on magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) have an injury from the immediate peripartum period, and not from long-term antenatal damage (Inder & Volpe, Reference Inder and Volpe2018). Causes of ASP are complex and often relate to uterine rupture, placental and umbilical cord complications (abruptio placentae, cord prolapse) and infection (Inder & Volpe, Reference Inder and Volpe2018). In Sweden, the incidence of ASP was 7.2 per 1000 live births, wherein ASP with HIE was 1.8 per 1000 live births (Thornberg, Thiringer, Odeback, & Milsom, Reference Thornberg, Thiringer, Odeback and Milsom1995), an HIE rate similar to other developed countries (Kurinczuk, White-Koning, & Badawi, Reference Kurinczuk, White-Koning and Badawi2010). Empirical studies show that the prevalence of ASP is similar across psychiatric patient and healthy groups (Ursini et al., Reference Ursini, Punzi, Chen, Marenco, Robinson, Porcelli and Weinberger2018; Wortinger et al., Reference Wortinger, Engen, Barth, Andreassen, Jørgensen and Agartz2020). A population study from Sweden found that ASP increased the risk for SZ (odds ratio 4.4) (Dalman et al., Reference Dalman, Thomas, David, Gentz, Lewis and Allebeck2001). The perinatal period is a sensitive time in brain development, and a hypoxic insult due to ASP can have long-term consequences on brain function (Ducsay et al., Reference Ducsay, Goyal, Pearce, Wilson, Hu and Zhang2018; Volpe, Reference Volpe2012).

The neurodevelopmental hypothesis of SZ suggests a vulnerability for serious obstetric complications that interacts with genetic factors and influences the development of SZ (Birnbaum & Weinberger, Reference Birnbaum and Weinberger2017; Stilo & Murray, Reference Stilo and Murray2019). Empirical research linking greater disability and altered neuronal growth in patients with SZ who have a history of the ASP has been scarce (Davies et al., Reference Davies, Segre, Estrade, Radua, De Micheli, Provenzani and Fusar-Poli2020; Radua et al., Reference Radua, Ramella-Cravaro, Ioannidis, Reichenberg, Phiphopthatsanee, Amir and Fusar-Poli2018). However, we recently reported lower intelligence quotient (IQ) across adult patients with SZ and bipolar disorder (BD) and healthy controls (HC) who experienced more than one serious obstetric complication, wherein ASP was one of the complications registered in 81% of all cases (Wortinger et al., Reference Wortinger, Engen, Barth, Lonning, Jorgensen, Andreassen and Agartz2019). BD is a severe mental disorder characterized by affective episodes and psychotic symptoms present in 60% of cases. BD is considered as part of the psychosis continuum, with overlapping clinical characteristics, genetic risk and pharmacological treatment as SZ (Vieta et al., Reference Vieta, Berk, Schulze, Carvalho, Suppes, Calabrese and Grande2018). Additionally, we found that adult height was lower in both patients with SZ and HC who experienced ASP (Wortinger et al., Reference Wortinger, Engen, Barth, Andreassen, Jørgensen and Agartz2020). Birth and pregnancy information were prospectively obtained from the Medical Birth Registry of Norway in both studies (Wortinger et al., Reference Wortinger, Engen, Barth, Lonning, Jorgensen, Andreassen and Agartz2019, Reference Wortinger, Engen, Barth, Andreassen, Jørgensen and Agartz2020). These findings might not only indicate greater intellectual deficits in those who experience ASP, but also gross alterations to normal developmental trajectories.

MRI derived intracranial volume (ICV) estimates strongly correlate with head circumference measures, r = 0.70 (Hshieh et al., Reference Hshieh, Fox, Kosar, Cavallari, Guttmann, Alsop and Inouye2016) and can be used as a proxy for both head size and a marker of early brain development. ICV reaches about 90% of its full size around the age of 5 years, which suggests brain growth during the early years determines cranium size (Gilmore, Knickmeyer, & Gao, Reference Gilmore, Knickmeyer and Gao2018; Sgouros, Goldin, Hockley, Wake, & Natarajan, Reference Sgouros, Goldin, Hockley, Wake and Natarajan1999; Woods, Reference Woods1998). ICV matures while the brain goes through additional structural and functional development until mid-to-late adolescence (Mills et al., Reference Mills, Goddings, Herting, Meuwese, Blakemore, Crone and Tamnes2016; Sgouros et al., Reference Sgouros, Goldin, Hockley, Wake and Natarajan1999), making early childhood and adolescent periods particularly sensitive to injury.

A relatively smaller ICV is commonly reported for patients with SZ compared to HC (Gurholt et al., Reference Gurholt, Lonning, Nerland, Jorgensen, Haukvik, Alloza and Group2020; Haijma et al., Reference Haijma, Van Haren, Cahn, Koolschijn, Hulshoff Pol and Kahn2013; Hulshoff Pol et al., Reference Hulshoff Pol, van Baal, Schnack, Brans, van der Schot, Brouwer and Kahn2012; Okada et al., Reference Okada, Fukunaga, Yamashita, Koshiyama, Yamamori, Ohi and Hashimoto2016; van Erp et al., Reference van Erp, Hibar, Rasmussen, Glahn, Pearlson, Andreassen and Turner2016). We found smaller ICV across adult patients with SZ and BD and HC who experienced ASP compared to those who did not experience ASP (Wortinger et al., Reference Wortinger, Engen, Barth, Andreassen, Jørgensen and Agartz2020). In another study, offspring of patients with SZ had smaller ICV than both offspring of patients with BD and HC, but both offspring of patients with SZ and BD had lower IQs than HC offspring (van Haren et al., Reference van Haren, Setiaman, Koevoets, Baalbergen, Kahn and Hillegers2020). These findings imply that familial risk for SZ has a stronger association with impeded early brain development than the familial risk for BD.

Brain volume and intelligence positively correlate in the general population, r = 0.30 (Cox, Ritchie, Fawns-Ritchie, Tucker-Drob, & Deary, Reference Cox, Ritchie, Fawns-Ritchie, Tucker-Drob and Deary2019; Gignac & Bates, Reference Gignac and Bates2017). It has been suggested that this relationship is stronger in SZ (Jensen et al., Reference Jensen, Bak, Rostrup, Nielsen, Pantelis, Glenthoj and Fagerlund2019; Rais et al., Reference Rais, Cahn, Schnack, Hulshoff Pol, Kahn and van Haren2012; van Haren et al., Reference van Haren, Setiaman, Koevoets, Baalbergen, Kahn and Hillegers2020). Genome-wide association studies suggest that a large fraction of the genomic risk for SZ influences both intelligence and ICV (Smeland et al., Reference Smeland, Wang, Frei, Li, Hibar, Franke and Andreassen2018, Reference Smeland, Bahrami, Frei, Shadrin, O'Connell, Savage and Andreassen2019). If this genomic risk relates to a vulnerability to hypoxic stress, individuals who experience ASP at birth and later developmental SZ could have altered neurodevelopmental trajectories already from birth. Depending on the duration of oxygen insufficiencies to the brain, ASP may relate to varying degrees of neural harm in the offspring. The degree of neural harm caused by ASP might be represented by a greater relationship between ICV and IQ in SZ patients because of the susceptibility of these factors already present in the disorder. On this background, we hypothesized that a greater positive relationship between ICV and IQ exists, specifically among patients with SZ who have a history of the ASP. Effects of ASP on both IQ and ICV have been demonstrated in previous studies, but the extent to which these are linked in SZ has not been investigated.

In this study, we combined the data of two previous studies (Wortinger et al., Reference Wortinger, Engen, Barth, Lonning, Jorgensen, Andreassen and Agartz2019, Reference Wortinger, Engen, Barth, Andreassen, Jørgensen and Agartz2020) and investigated whether a history of the ASP interacts with the relationship between adult ICV and IQ in patients on the SZ and BD spectrums and HC. The aims of this study were (1) measure the prevalence of ASP across groups; (2) compare IQ and ICV, after controlling for the effects of ASP, between groups to investigate whether we can identify evidence of abnormal neurodevelopment; (3) examine the relationships between IQ, ICV and ASP between SZ, BD and HC; (4) explore which subcomponents of IQ (verbal or performance) as measured by WASI are most related to ICV and ASP.

Methods

This study is part of the Thematically Organized Psychosis (TOP) study, which is a thematic research effort focused on disease mechanisms of psychotic disorders and is the main study protocol at the Norwegian Center for Mental Disorders Research (NORMENT, Oslo, Norway). Adult patients were recruited from psychiatric units (outpatient and inpatient) of public hospitals in the Oslo region. The HC were randomly selected from the national population register in the same catchment area as the patients. After a complete description of the study, all participants gave written informed consent. The study was conducted in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration and the South East Regional Committee for Research Ethics (REC South-East). The Norwegian Data Inspectorate approved the study.

Participants

All patients underwent a thorough clinical investigation by trained psychologists and physicians. Clinical diagnoses were assessed using the Structured Clinical Interview for the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edition (DSM-IV), Axis 1 disorder (SCID-I) module A-E (Spitzer, Williams, Gibbon, & First, Reference Spitzer, Williams, Gibbon and First1988). The psychosocial function was assessed with the Global Assessment of Functioning scale, split version (GAF; Pedersen Hagtvet, & Karterud, Reference Pedersen, Hagtvet and Karterud2007). Current psychotic symptoms were rated by the use of the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS; Kay, Fiszbein, & Opler, Reference Kay, Fiszbein and Opler1987). The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT) (Saunders, Aasland, Babor, de la Fuente, & Grant, Reference Saunders, Aasland, Babor, de la Fuente and Grant1993) and Drug Use Disorders Identification Test (DUDIT) (Berman, Bergman, Palmstierna, & Schlyter, Reference Berman, Bergman, Palmstierna and Schlyter2005) were used to evaluate alcohol and drug use in patients. HC were interviewed by trained research assistants and examined with the Primary Care Evaluation of Mental Disorders (Prime-MD) to ensure no current or previous psychiatric disorders (Spitzer et al., Reference Spitzer, Williams, Kroenke, Linzer, deGruy III, Hahn, Brody and Johnson1994).

Exclusion criteria for both patients and HC were hospitalized for previous moderate or severe head injury, neurological disorder, medical conditions thought to interfere with brain function and age outside the range of 18–65 years. All MRI scans go through a thorough screening by a neuroradiologist and a graded scheme in the MR lab, and if pathology is detected in the MR images, the participant is excluded. Additional exclusion criteria for HC were current or previous somatic illness and substance misuse disorders or dependency within the last 6 months. HC were also excluded if they or a first-degree relative had a lifetime history of severe psychiatric disorder.

For the current study, participants were included if they had a birth registry, IQ and MRI data. No matching procedures were performed to ensure similarity between the groups in, e.g. age, sex or education. The total subject sample (N = 495, age range: 18–42 years) consisted of patients with a DSM-IV diagnosis within the SZ spectrum: SZ (DSM-IV 295.1, 295.3, 295.6 and 295.9; n = 93), schizophreniform disorder (DSM-IV 295.4; n = 15), schizoaffective disorder (DSM-IV 295.7; n = 10) or psychosis not otherwise specified (DSM-IV 298.9; n = 53); or within the BD spectrum: bipolar I disorder (DSM-IV 296.0–7; n = 67), bipolar II disorder (DSM-IV 296.89; n = 35) or BD not otherwise specified (DSM-IV 296.80; n = 6); and HC (n = 216).

Birth asphyxia

Birth data were collected from the Medical Birth Registry of Norway (MBRN; https://www.fhi.no/en/hn/health-registries/medical-birth-registry-of-norway) and assessed for the presence of ASP. ASP was defined as having had a report of a complication coded AS53 (asphyxia without other signs), AS54 (asphyxia with poor fetal sound), AS55 (asphyxia and discolored amniotic fluid), AS56 (asphyxia with poor fetal sound and discolored amniotic fluid) or AS61 (asphyxia) on the MBRN registry form, all of which state the presence of newborn ASP.

IQ measure

The IQ was assessed with the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI) (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2007), a Norwegian version translated from the original WASI (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1999). The WASI provides a total IQ score as well as scores for verbal and performance IQ. The WASI was developed as a short measure based on the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS), which is considered the full IQ test. The WASI provides a good estimate of intelligence that is similar to WAIS (Bosnes, Reference Bosnes2009; McCrimmon & Smith, Reference McCrimmon and Smith2013). The WASI IQ score is derived from four tests (vocabulary, similarities, block design and matrix reasoning) comprised of measures of verbal and nonverbal intelligence. The vocabulary and similarities tests form the verbal IQ subcomponent, which is an estimate of an individual's overall verbal intellectual ability. The two nonverbal tests (block design and matrix reasoning) form the performance IQ subcomponent, which is an estimate of perceptual and visuospatial organization.

Image acquisition and processing

All participants underwent MRI scanning on a 1.5 T Siemens Magnetom Sonata scanner (Siemens Medical Solutions, Erlangen, Germany) equipped with an eight-channel head coil. After a conventional three-plane localizer, two sagittal T1-weighted magnetization prepared rapid gradient echo (MPRAGE) volumes were acquired with the Siemens tfl3d1_ns pulse sequence (echo time,TE = 3.93 ms; repetition time, TR = 2730 ms; inversion time, TI = 1000 ms; flip angle = 7°; field-of-view, FOV = 24 cm; voxel size = 1.33 × 0.94 × 1 mm3; number of partitions = 160).

The FreeSurfer software package (version 5.3) was used to process images (Dale, Fischl, & Sereno, Reference Dale, Fischl and Sereno1999; Fischl et al., Reference Fischl, Salat, Busa, Albert, Dieterich, Haselgrove and Dale2002). FreeSurfer estimates ICV by using the relationship between the ICV and the linear transform to MNI305 space [talairach.xfm; Buckner et al. (Reference Buckner, Head, Parker, Fotenos, Marcus, Morris and Snyder2004)].

Statistical analysis

Asp and demographic variables

Between-group comparisons were performed with one-way ANOVAs for continuous variables, and Pearson χ2 tests for categorical variables. Post hoc, pairwise group comparisons were performed in cases of significant main effects of group. Pearson correlations were calculated for verbal IQ and performance IQ in each group. The correlations were compared using multiple regression analysis, controlling for age and sex. Pearson correlations were also calculated for verbal IQ and negative symptoms in each group, a relationship often viewed as interrelated (Harvey, Koren, Reichenberg, & Bowie, Reference Harvey, Koren, Reichenberg and Bowie2006). The correlations were compared using multiple regression analysis, controlling for age and sex. Statistical analyses were performed using the Statistical Package of Social Sciences (SPSS) version 26 (IBM SPSS Inc.).

IQ and ICV between groups

IQ and ICV were compared between groups using ANCOVAs with the group as a fixed factor (α = 0.05). Age, sex and ASP were used as covariates. Post hoc, pairwise group comparisons were performed.

IQ and ICV relationship

IQ was added as a dependent variable in a multiple regression model to assess its relationship with ICV between groups and with the effects of ASP. To assess the effects of patient groups, BD and SZ were dummy coded with HC as a reference and entered, together with ICV and ASP, as independent variables. ICV and patient group interactions were added as BD-by-ICV and SZ-by-ICV. We used an interaction term with ASP to determine whether ASP moderated the relationship between ICV (ASP-by-ICV) and the outcome variable, IQ. Also, interaction terms with ASP were used to determine whether ASP interacted with the variable, group, that was driven by the contrast in associations between the two groups (BD-by-ASP, SZ-by-ASP) or whether ASP interacted with both group and ICV variables (BD-by-ASP-by-ICV and SZ-by-ASP-by-ICV) to predict IQ. Age and sex were included as covariates in the model.

IQ subcomponents and ICV relationships

Follow-up analyses were performed for both verbal IQ and performance IQ using the same regression model as the IQ analysis above.

Results

Asp and demographic variables

ASP was prevalent in 14.3% of our sample (Table 1). More patients with BD experienced ASP, compared to the SZ group, but the prevalence of ASP did not significantly differ between patient groups and HC. There was a larger proportion of females in the BD group compared to the other groups. HC were older and had more years of education than the patient groups. The SZ group acquired less education than the BD group. A greater degree of clinical symptoms and reduced general functioning was found in the SZ group using GAF and PANSS scores compared to the BD group (Table 2). There were no differences in AUDIT (Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test) and DUDIT (Drug Use Disorders Identification Test) scores between patient groups.

Table 1. ASP, demographics, ICV and IQ between groups

s.d. (standard deviation); df (degrees of freedom); CI (confidence intervals); HC (healthy controls); BD (bipolar disorder spectrum); SZ (schizophrenia spectrum); IQ (intelligence quotient); ICV (intracranial volume).

Bold values denote statistical significance at the p < 0.05 level.

a Effect of a group, corrected for sex, age and birth asphyxia.

Table 2. Clinical information between patient groups

s.d. (standard deviation); df (degrees of freedom); BD (bipolar disorder spectrum); SZ (schizophrenia spectrum).

Bold values denote statistical significance at the p < 0.05 level.

IQ and ICV between groups

A marginally significant main effect of group was found for ICV (Table 1), after controlling for age, sex and ASP. Pairwise comparisons revealed significantly smaller ICV in the SZ group than in the HC and BD groups (albeit trend level in the SZ–BD comparison). There was no difference in the ICV of BD and HC groups.

A significant main effect of group was found for IQ, verbal IQ and performance IQ (Table 1), after controlling for age, sex and ASP. Pairwise comparisons revealed significantly lower IQs in the SZ and BD groups as compared with HC and in SZ compared to BD. Verbal IQ was lower in patients with SZ compared to both HC and BD groups, but verbal IQ did not differ between BD and HC groups. Performance IQ was significantly lower in both patient groups compared to HC, but it did not differ between BD and SZ groups.

The correlations between verbal IQ and performance IQ within each group were all significant (verbal IQ-performance IQ: r HC = 0.37, p = <0.001; r BD = 0.38, p = <0.001; r SZ = 0.55, p = <0.001). In a multiple regression analysis with verbal IQ as the outcome variable, we found that the correlation between verbal IQ and performance IQ was significantly greater in the SZ group compared to HC (SZ × performance IQ, t = 2.11, p = 0.036), controlling for age and sex, which was not the case for the BD group.

The correlations between verbal IQ and negative symptoms within each group were significant (r BD = −0.23, p = 0.018; r SZ = −0.24, p = 0.002). In a multiple regression analysis with verbal IQ as the outcome variable, we found the correlation between verbal IQ and negative symptoms was significantly greater in both BD and SZ patients combined and with ASP compared to patients without ASP (ASP × negative symptoms, t = −2.60, p = 0.010), controlling for age and sex.

IQ and ICV relationship

In the regression analysis, we found a significant main effect of ICV across diagnostic groups (larger ICV associated with higher IQ). The slope of the relationship between ICV and IQ in patient groups did not differ from HC. There was no main effect of ASP across the whole sample and a non-significant interaction between ICV and ASP. However, a significant interaction between ASP and the SZ group indicated that within the SZ group, specifically, patients with a history of the ASP had lower IQs than patients without a history of the ASP. Furthermore, a significant three-way interaction between ASP, ICV and SZ showed that the relationship between ICV and IQ differed by ASP in patients with SZ. Specifically, patients with SZ and ASP displayed a stronger relationship compared to patients with SZ, but without ASP (Supplemental materials: online Supplementary Fig. 1A; online Supplementary Table 3).

IQ subcomponents and ICV relationships

Verbal IQ

Similar to the model for IQ, we found a significant main effect of ICV across diagnostic groups in the model predicting verbal IQ. The slope of the relationship between ICV and verbal IQ in patient groups did not differ from HC. There was no main effect of ASP across the whole sample and a non-significant interaction between ICV and ASP. However, a significant interaction between ASP and the SZ group indicated that within the SZ group, specifically, patients with a history of the ASP had lower verbal IQ than patients without a history of the ASP. Furthermore, a significant three-way interaction between ASP, ICV and SZ showed that the relationship between ICV and verbal IQ differed by ASP in patients with SZ. Specifically, patients with SZ and ASP displayed a stronger relationship compared to patients with SZ, but without ASP (Fig. 1; Table 3).

Fig. 1. In the SZ group, the relationship between ICV and verbal IQ was significantly greater in the presence of ASP compared to when ASP was not present, which was not found in the other groups. SZ (schizophrenia spectrum); IQ (intelligence quotient); ICV (intracranial volume); ASP (birth asphyxia).

Table 3. Multiple linear regression analyses by the level of IQ performance

BD (bipolar disorder spectrum); SZ (schizophrenia spectrum); IQ (intelligence quotient); ICV (intracranial volume); ASP (birth asphyxia).

Bold values denote statistical significance at the p < 0.05 level.

Performance IQ

In the regression analysis, we found a significant main effect of age. There was no main effect of ASP across the whole sample, but a marginally significant interaction between ICV and ASP (Supplemental materials: online Supplementary Fig. 1B; online Supplementary Table 3).

Discussion

ICV of adult patients with SZ who experienced ASP showed a significantly greater positive association with IQ compared to SZ patients without ASP, a relationship that was statistically significant but less pronounced in the other groups. We found verbal IQ, not performance IQ, was the subcomponent that was positively associated with ICV and ASP in the SZ group.

The results support our initial hypothesis that a greater positive relationship between ICV and IQ exists in the subgroup of patients with SZ who have a history of ASP, compared to ASP effects in BD and HC. The reason for this association with the SZ group can be due to a vulnerability towards hypoxic stress in individuals genetically predisposed to develop SZ (Ursini et al., Reference Ursini, Punzi, Chen, Marenco, Robinson, Porcelli and Weinberger2018), which results in a diminished resilience to the ASP. Cognitive consequences of the ASP might also be specific to SZ because the genetic variants associated with SZ risk overlap with genetic variants for both ICV (Smeland et al., Reference Smeland, Wang, Frei, Li, Hibar, Franke and Andreassen2018) and intelligence, where most of the SZ risk alleles were associated with poorer cognitive performance and opposite to what was found with BD risk alleles (i.e. better cognitive performance) (Smeland et al., Reference Smeland, Bahrami, Frei, Shadrin, O'Connell, Savage and Andreassen2019).

The heterogeneity in the stronger correlation between ICV and IQ of patients with SZ and ASP might be due to the ASP complication in the MBRN being unspecific to the amount of neural harm present in the infant. The degree of neural harm due to ASP might be reflected in the greater degree of relationship between ICV and IQ (i.e. great harm in those with small-ICV-low-IQ and less harm in those with large-ICV-high-IQ), but so might the variations in the genetic risk for SZ development, rendering some participants more or less resilient to hypoxic insult. Future studies might address the relationship between genetic risk for SZ development and ASP.

Smaller ICV has been reported in adolescent offspring of SZ compared to BD and HC offspring, together with significant associations between ICV and IQ in the patient offspring, but not in the HC offspring (van Haren et al., Reference van Haren, Setiaman, Koevoets, Baalbergen, Kahn and Hillegers2020). In our study, ICV of patients with SZ was smaller compared to both patients with BD and HC, after adjusting for ASP, but the association between IQ and ICV did not differ between groups. IQ showed a consistent dependence on ICV in both adult patient groups (reported here) and adolescent offspring of patients with SZ and BD (van Haren et al., Reference van Haren, Setiaman, Koevoets, Baalbergen, Kahn and Hillegers2020). Our findings raise the possibility that the varying degrees of impeded brain growth and cognitive development in SZ may be caused by neural insult due to ASP. Further, a small ICV and low IQ might serve as neurodevelopmental markers of SZ, and because both ICV and IQ measures can be assessed early in development, they have the potential to be included in multivariate prediction tools aiming to assess SZ risk at an early stage.

ICV being of particular importance for IQ in SZ might be related to greater hypoxic insult at birth, but an incidence of ASP reported in the MBRN does not provide information on the severity (i.e. mild, moderate or severe). We did not find a difference in the prevalence of ASP between patient groups and the HC. ASP is a primary cause of newborn death and neurological impairment, worldwide. Cerebral palsy is the neurological sequelae of fetal hypoxia occurring in pregnancy or around childbirth, which causes abnormal development or damage to parts of the brain that control movement, balance and posture (Volpe, Reference Volpe2012). Even if hypoxic injury due to ASP did not present early with motor and learning impairments, a primary neurological insult might be unmasked with later neural maturation challenges during adolescence and manifest as psychosis and cognitive and subtle neurological impairments in early adulthood (Murray, Bhavsar, Tripoli, & Howes, Reference Murray, Bhavsar, Tripoli and Howes2017).

ASP was associated with ICV and was specifically related to verbal IQ in the SZ group, which might indicate that impaired language abilities observed in SZ are partly a consequence of ASP. Language impairment in SZ has long been recognized (Fraser, King, Thomas, & Kendell, Reference Fraser, King, Thomas and Kendell1986; King, Fraser, Thomas, & Kendell, Reference King, Fraser, Thomas and Kendell1990; Morice & Ingram, Reference Morice and Ingram1982) and considered a key diagnostic feature of SZ spectrum disorders (de Boer, Brederoo, Voppel, & Sommer, Reference de Boer, Brederoo, Voppel and Sommer2020; DeLisi, Reference DeLisi2001; Kuperberg, Reference Kuperberg2010a, Reference Kuperbergb). In fact, verbal IQ was the subcomponent that differentiated patients with SZ and BD and might be a primary indicator of disease development. The language function is generally specialized to the left hemisphere of the human brain, and several studies have found a reduction of leftward language lateralization in patients with SZ (Hahn et al., Reference Hahn, Neuhaus, Pogun, Dettling, Kotz, Hahn and Gunturkun2011; Loberg, Hugdahl, & Green, Reference Loberg, Hugdahl and Green1999; Ocklenburg, Westerhausen, Hirnstein, & Hugdahl, Reference Ocklenburg, Westerhausen, Hirnstein and Hugdahl2013; Rossell & Boundy, Reference Rossell and Boundy2005). An ASP might be related to the failure of the brain to specialize in language function (Crow, Reference Crow1997). Our findings give further support for low verbal intellectual abilities as one of the main areas of cognition implicated in SZ development.

Genetic risk for the development of SZ might hinder cellular resilience to hypoxia in the fetal brain (Murray et al., Reference Murray, Bhavsar, Tripoli and Howes2017). Ischemia–hypoxia response genes (set of genes that respond to ischemia, insufficient blood flow to provide adequate oxygenation, which leads to tissue hypoxia) showed a threefold enrichment for genes associated with SZ risk (Schmidt-Kastner, van Os, Esquivel, Steinbusch, & Rutten, Reference Schmidt-Kastner, van Os, Esquivel, Steinbusch and Rutten2012). In addition, a study found that severe obstetric complications interact with ischemia–hypoxia response genes and further increase the risk for SZ (Nicodemus et al., Reference Nicodemus, Marenco, Batten, Vakkalanka, Egan, Straub and Weinberger2008). A reduced up-regulatory effect on HILPDA (hypoxia-inducible lipid droplet-associated) expression was found in SZ (Akkouh et al., Reference Akkouh, Ueland, Hansson, Inderhaug, Hughes, Steen and Djurovic2020). The functional HILPDA protein is expressed only under hypoxic stress (Jiang et al., Reference Jiang, Sun, Dai, Cao, Zhang, Peng and Xiang2015), and the failure to sufficiently up-regulate HILPDA expression might represent a cellular deficiency in SZ that fails to protect fetal neurons from any level of hypoxic insult. There is a need for further studies to better understand the hypoxic cascade behind neural injury and advance neuroprotective interventions such as hypothermia (Gluckman et al., Reference Gluckman, Wyatt, Azzopardi, Ballard, Edwards, Ferriero and Gunn2005), xenon gas (Thoresen, Hobbs, Wood, Chakkarapani, & Dingley, Reference Thoresen, Hobbs, Wood, Chakkarapani and Dingley2009) and oxytocin (Boksa, Zhang, & Nouel, Reference Boksa, Zhang and Nouel2015), in efforts to decrease the risk for SZ development.

Strengths of this study were the use of the Medical Birth Registry of Norway, as a prospective study it is possible to establish a causal relationship between ASP and individuals who later develop SZ. This study provides evidence that ASP and ICV impact verbal IQ in SZ, which is currently missing in the field.

A limitation might be the FreeSurfer estimation of the ICV. From a T1-weighted scan, accurate measurement of the ICV is difficult since the signal intensities of cerebrospinal fluid and skull are not separable. FreeSurfer's registration-based estimates might be less accurate than segmentation-based estimates of ICV (Ma et al., Reference Ma, Popuri, Bhalla, Sangha, Lu, Cao and Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging2019). Whereas it is most common to use IQ <70 as an exclusion criterion in studies of cognition in SZ, we included participants with IQ <70, which may have altered group proportions and means. However, in doing so better reflects patients with low intellectual abilities potentially attributed to ASP in the SZ population (Wells et al., Reference Wells, Swaminathan, Sundram, Weinberg, Bruggemann, Jacomb and Weickert2015; Wortinger et al., Reference Wortinger, Engen, Barth, Lonning, Jorgensen, Andreassen and Agartz2019). A limitation of this study might also be the lower percentage of ASP + SZ patients with MRI data in our sample. We found ASP to be more prevalent in the BD group than the SZ group, which is unexpected if SZ risk predisposes patients to early insults around birth. Since OCs in SZ were associated with lower IQ (Wortinger et al., Reference Wortinger, Engen, Barth, Lonning, Jorgensen, Andreassen and Agartz2019), it could be that subgroups of SZ patients with lower IQ are less likely to participate in additional MRI studies. Furthermore, ASP + SZ patients might have had obvious organic pathology/brain lesions in their MR images, which would have excluded them from this study. With the performance IQ analysis, we did not find statistically significant main effects or interactions with ASP, ICV and group variables. However, the trend-level ASP and ICV interaction in the model suggests ASP effects on ICV might have a general impact on performance IQ and this relationship may not be disease specific. Future studies with larger samples are needed to clarify and replicate these results.

Small sample size in the BD group might be a limitation in this study, which may lead to difficulty in finding an interaction effect between ASP and ICV on IQ (false negative). Even so, we found a greater prevalence of ASP in the BD group compared the SZ group, and previously reported smaller ICV in BD patients with ASP compared to BD patients without ASP (Wortinger et al., Reference Wortinger, Engen, Barth, Andreassen, Jørgensen and Agartz2020). Yet, we did not find an interaction between ASP and ICV on IQ in BD. Thus, despite the non-significant statistical difference, the findings suggest that IQ is less dependent on ICV in BD, and that BD is more resilient to hypoxic injury than SZ.

Conclusion

We found a significantly stronger relationship between intracranial volume and IQ, specifically verbal IQ, in patients with SZ who had experienced perinatal or birth asphyxia compared to SZ patients who did not. This moderation by ASP was not demonstrated in the patients with BD or in HC. The greater positive association between ICV and IQ in patients with SZ who had experienced ASP might indicate abnormal neurodevelopment. Our findings give support for ICV together with verbal intellectual abilities as clinically relevant markers that can be added to prediction tools to enhance evaluations of risk in SZ development. Improved pre- and perinatal care and ready neuroprotective interventions for vulnerable pregnancies may lessen interactive risks caused by ASP.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721000489.

Acknowledgements

We appreciate the support from the Research Council of Norway (grant number 223273) and the South-Eastern Norway Regional Health Authority (grant number 2017–093). We thank the participants of the TOP study and express gratitude to clinicians, research assistants and referring hospital units involved in recruitment and assessment for the Norwegian Centre for Mental Disorders (NORMENT).

Financial support

The authors report no financial relationships with commercial interest, other than Dr Andreassen who received speaker's honoraria from Lundbeck and is a consultant for HealthLytix.

References

Akkouh, I. A., Ueland, T., Hansson, L., Inderhaug, E., Hughes, T., Steen, N. E., … Djurovic, S. (2020). Decreased IL-1beta-induced CCL20 response in human iPSC-astrocytes in schizophrenia: Potential attenuating effects on recruitment of regulatory T cells. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 87, 634644. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2020.02.008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berman, A. H., Bergman, H., Palmstierna, T., & Schlyter, F. (2005). Evaluation of the Drug Use Disorders Identification Test (DUDIT) in criminal justice and detoxification settings and in a Swedish population sample. European Addiction Research, 11(1), 2231. doi:10.1159/000081413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Birnbaum, R., & Weinberger, D. R. (2017). Genetic insights into the neurodevelopmental origins of schizophrenia. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 18(12), 727740. doi:10.1038/nrn.2017.125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boksa, P., Zhang, Y., & Nouel, D. (2015). Maternal oxytocin administration before birth influences the effects of birth anoxia on the neonatal rat brain. Neurochemical Research, 40(8), 16311643. doi:10.1007/s11064-015-1645-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bosnes, O. (2009). Norsk versjon av wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence: Hvor godt er samsvaret mellom WASI og norsk versjon av wechsler adult intelligence scale-III. Tidsskrift for Norsk psykologforening, 46(6), 4.Google Scholar
Buckner, R. L., Head, D., Parker, J., Fotenos, A. F., Marcus, D., Morris, J. C., & Snyder, A. Z. (2004). A unified approach for morphometric and functional data analysis in young, old, and demented adults using automated atlas-based head size normalization: Reliability and validation against manual measurement of total intracranial volume. Neuroimage, 23(2), 724738. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.018.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cox, S. R., Ritchie, S. J., Fawns-Ritchie, C., Tucker-Drob, E. M., & Deary, I. J. (2019). Structural brain imaging correlates of general intelligence in UK Biobank. Intelligence, 76, 101376. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2019.101376.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crow, T. J. (1997). Is schizophrenia the price that homo sapiens pays for language? Schizophrenia Research, 28(2–3), 127141. doi:10.1016/s0920-9964(97)00110-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dale, A. M., Fischl, B., & Sereno, M. I. (1999). Cortical surface-based analysis. I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage, 9(2), 179194. doi:10.1006/nimg.1998.0395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dalman, C., Thomas, H. V., David, A. S., Gentz, J., Lewis, G., & Allebeck, P. (2001). Signs of asphyxia at birth and risk of schizophrenia. Population-based case-control study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 179, 403408. doi:10.1192/bjp.179.5.403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davies, C., Segre, G., Estrade, A., Radua, J., De Micheli, A., Provenzani, U., … Fusar-Poli, P. (2020). Prenatal and perinatal risk and protective factors for psychosis: A systematic review and meta-analysis. The Lancet. Psychiatry, 7(5), 399410. doi:10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30057-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Boer, J. N., Brederoo, S. G., Voppel, A. E., & Sommer, I. E. C. (2020). Anomalies in language as a biomarker for schizophrenia. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 33(3), 212218. doi:10.1097/YCO.0000000000000595.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeLisi, L. E. (2001). Speech disorder in schizophrenia: Review of the literature and exploration of its relation to the uniquely human capacity for language. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 27(3), 481496. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a006889.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ducsay, C. A., Goyal, R., Pearce, W. J., Wilson, S., Hu, X. Q., & Zhang, L. (2018). Gestational hypoxia and developmental plasticity. Physiological Reviews, 98(3), 12411334. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00043.2017.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischl, B., Salat, D. H., Busa, E., Albert, M., Dieterich, M., Haselgrove, C., … Dale, A. M. (2002). Whole brain segmentation: Automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron, 33(3), 341355. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11832223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fraser, W. I., King, K. M., Thomas, P., & Kendell, R. E. (1986). The diagnosis of schizophrenia by language analysis. British Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 275278. doi:10.1192/bjp.148.3.275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gignac, G. E., & Bates, T. C. (2017). Brain volume and intelligence: The moderating role of intelligence measurement quality. Intelligence, 64, 1829. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2017.06.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilmore, J. H., Knickmeyer, R. C., & Gao, W. (2018). Imaging structural and functional brain development in early childhood. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 19(3), 123137. doi:10.1038/nrn.2018.1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gluckman, P. D., Wyatt, J. S., Azzopardi, D., Ballard, R., Edwards, A. D., Ferriero, D. M., … Gunn, A. J. (2005). Selective head cooling with mild systemic hypothermia after neonatal encephalopathy: Multicentre randomised trial. Lancet (London, England), 365(9460), 663670. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(05)17946-X.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gurholt, T. P., Lonning, V., Nerland, S., Jorgensen, K. N., Haukvik, U. K., Alloza, C., … Group, E.-E. W. (2020). Intracranial and subcortical volumes in adolescents with early-onset psychosis: A multisite mega-analysis from the ENIGMA consortium. Human Brain Mapping, 112. doi:10.1002/hbm.25212.Google ScholarPubMed
Hahn, C., Neuhaus, A. H., Pogun, S., Dettling, M., Kotz, S. A., Hahn, E., … Gunturkun, O. (2011). Smoking reduces language lateralization: A dichotic listening study with control participants and schizophrenia patients. Brain and Cognition, 76(2), 300309. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2011.03.015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haijma, S. V., Van Haren, N., Cahn, W., Koolschijn, P. C., Hulshoff Pol, H. E., & Kahn, R. S. (2013). Brain volumes in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis in over 18 000 subjects. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 39(5), 11291138. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbs118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harvey, P. D., Koren, D., Reichenberg, A., & Bowie, C. R. (2006). Negative symptoms and cognitive deficits: What is the nature of their relationship? Schizophrenia Bulletin, 32(2), 250258. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbj011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hshieh, T. T., Fox, M. L., Kosar, C. M., Cavallari, M., Guttmann, C. R., Alsop, D., … Inouye, S. K. (2016). Head circumference as a useful surrogate for intracranial volume in older adults. International Psychogeriatrics, 28(1), 157162. doi:10.1017/S104161021500037X.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hulshoff Pol, H. E., van Baal, G. C., Schnack, H. G., Brans, R. G., van der Schot, A. C., Brouwer, R. M., … Kahn, R. S. (2012). Overlapping and segregating structural brain abnormalities in twins with schizophrenia or bipolar disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 69(4), 349359. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.1615.Google ScholarPubMed
Inder, T. E., & Volpe, J. J. (2018). Hypoxic-ischemic injury in the term infant: Clinical-neurological features, diagnosis, imaging, prognosis, therapy. In Volpe's neurology of the newborn (pp. 510563. e515). Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jensen, M. H., Bak, N., Rostrup, E., Nielsen, M. O., Pantelis, C., Glenthoj, B. Y., … Fagerlund, B. (2019). The impact of schizophrenia and intelligence on the relationship between age and brain volume. Schizophrenia Research: Cognition, 15, 16. doi:10.1016/j.scog.2018.09.002.Google ScholarPubMed
Jiang, C., Sun, J., Dai, Y., Cao, P., Zhang, L., Peng, S., … Xiang, J. (2015). HIF-1A and C/EBPs transcriptionally regulate adipogenic differentiation of bone marrow-derived MSCs in hypoxia. Stem Cell Research & Therapy, 6, 21. doi:10.1186/s13287-015-0014-4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kay, S. R., Fiszbein, A., & Opler, L. A. (1987). The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 13, 261276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, K., Fraser, W. I., Thomas, P., & Kendell, R. E. (1990). Re-examination of the language of psychotic subjects. British Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 211215. doi:10.1192/bjp.156.2.211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuperberg, G. R. (2010a). Language in schizophrenia part 1: An introduction. Language and Linguistics Compass, 4(8), 576589. doi:10.1111/j.1749-818X.2010.00216.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuperberg, G. R. (2010b). Language in schizophrenia part 2: What can psycholinguistics bring to the study of schizophrenia…and vice versa? Language and Linguistics Compass, 4(8), 590604. doi:10.1111/j.1749-818X.2010.00217.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kurinczuk, J. J., White-Koning, M., & Badawi, N. (2010). Epidemiology of neonatal encephalopathy and hypoxic-ischaemic encephalopathy. Early Human Development, 86(6), 329338. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2010.05.010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loberg, E. M., Hugdahl, K., & Green, M. F. (1999). Hemispheric asymmetry in schizophrenia: A “dual deficits” model. Biological Psychiatry, 45(1), 7681. doi:10.1016/s0006-3223(98)00219-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ma, D., Popuri, K., Bhalla, M., Sangha, O., Lu, D., Cao, J., … Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging, I. (2019). Quantitative assessment of field strength, total intracranial volume, sex, and age effects on the goodness of harmonization for volumetric analysis on the ADNI database. Human Brain Mapping, 40(5), 15071527. doi:10.1002/hbm.24463.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCrimmon, A. W., & Smith, A. D. (2013). Review of the Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence, second edition (WASI-II). Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 31(3), 337341. doi:10.1177/0734282912467756.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mills, K. L., Goddings, A. L., Herting, M. M., Meuwese, R., Blakemore, S. J., Crone, E. A., … Tamnes, C. K. (2016). Structural brain development between childhood and adulthood: Convergence across four longitudinal samples. Neuroimage, 141, 273281. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.07.044.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morice, R. D., & Ingram, J. C. (1982). Language analysis in schizophrenia: Diagnostic implications. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 16(2), 1121. doi: 10.3109/00048678209161186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, R. M., Bhavsar, V., Tripoli, G., & Howes, O. (2017). 30 Years on: How the neurodevelopmental hypothesis of schizophrenia morphed into the developmental risk factor model of psychosis. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 43(6), 11901196. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbx121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nicodemus, K. K., Marenco, S., Batten, A. J., Vakkalanka, R., Egan, M. F., Straub, R. E., & Weinberger, D. R. (2008). Serious obstetric complications interact with hypoxia-regulated/vascular-expression genes to influence schizophrenia risk. Molecular Psychiatry, 13(9), 873877. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4002153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
NIMH (2018). Schizophrenia. Bethesda, MD: Author. Retrieved from https://www.nimh.nih.gov/health/statistics/schizophrenia.shtml.Google Scholar
Ocklenburg, S., Westerhausen, R., Hirnstein, M., & Hugdahl, K. (2013). Auditory hallucinations and reduced language lateralization in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis of dichotic listening studies. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 19(4), 410418. doi: 10.1017/S1355617712001476.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Okada, N., Fukunaga, M., Yamashita, F., Koshiyama, D., Yamamori, H., Ohi, K., … Hashimoto, R. (2016). Abnormal asymmetries in subcortical brain volume in schizophrenia. Molecular Psychiatry, 21(10), 14601466. doi:10.1038/mp.2015.209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pedersen, G., Hagtvet, K. A., & Karterud, S. (2007). Generalizability studies of the global assessment of functioning-split version. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 48, 8894.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Radua, J., Ramella-Cravaro, V., Ioannidis, J. P. A., Reichenberg, A., Phiphopthatsanee, N., Amir, T., … Fusar-Poli, P. (2018). What causes psychosis? An umbrella review of risk and protective factors. World Psychiatry, 17(1), 4966. doi:10.1002/wps.20490.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rais, M., Cahn, W., Schnack, H. G., Hulshoff Pol, H. E., Kahn, R. S., & van Haren, N. E. (2012). Brain volume reductions in medication-naive patients with schizophrenia in relation to intelligence quotient. Psychological Medicine, 42(9), 18471856. doi:10.1017/S0033291712000098.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rossell, S. L., & Boundy, C. L. (2005). Are auditory-verbal hallucinations associated with auditory affective processing deficits? Schizophrenia Research, 78(1), 95106. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.06.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saunders, J. B., Aasland, O. G., Babor, T. F., de la Fuente, J. R., & Grant, M. (1993). Development of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): WHO collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption – II. Addiction, 88(6), 791804. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1993.tb02093.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt-Kastner, R., van Os, J., Esquivel, G., Steinbusch, H. W., & Rutten, B. P. (2012). An environmental analysis of genes associated with schizophrenia: Hypoxia and vascular factors as interacting elements in the neurodevelopmental model. Molecular Psychiatry, 17(12), 11941205. doi:10.1038/mp.2011.183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sgouros, S., Goldin, J. H., Hockley, A. D., Wake, M. J., & Natarajan, K. (1999). Intracranial volume change in childhood. Journal of Neurosurgery, 91(4), 610616. doi: 10.3171/jns.1999.91.4.0610.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smeland, O. B., Bahrami, S., Frei, O., Shadrin, A., O'Connell, K., Savage, J., … Andreassen, O. A. (2019). Genome-wide analysis reveals extensive genetic overlap between schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and intelligence. Molecular Psychiatry, 25(4), 844853. doi:10.1038/s41380-018-0332-x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smeland, O. B., Wang, Y., Frei, O., Li, W., Hibar, D. P., Franke, B., … Andreassen, O. A. (2018). Genetic overlap between schizophrenia and volumes of Hippocampus, Putamen, and Intracranial volume indicates shared molecular genetic mechanisms. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 44(4), 854864. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbx148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spitzer, R. L., Williams, J. B., Gibbon, M., & First, M. B. (1988). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R-Patient Version (SCID-P). New York, NY: Biometrics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute.Google Scholar
Spitzer, R. L., Williams, J. B., Kroenke, K., Linzer, M., deGruy III, F. V., Hahn, S. R., Brody, D., & Johnson, J. G. (1994). Utility of a new procedure for diagnosing mental disorders in primary care. The PRIME-MD 1000 study. Journal of the American Medical Association, 272, 17491756.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stilo, S. A., & Murray, R. M. (2019). Non-Genetic factors in schizophrenia. Current Psychiatry Reports, 21(10), 100. doi:10.1007/s11920-019-1091-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thoresen, M., Hobbs, C. E., Wood, T., Chakkarapani, E., & Dingley, J. (2009). Cooling combined with immediate or delayed xenon inhalation provides equivalent long-term neuroprotection after neonatal hypoxia-ischemia. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow & Metabolism, 29(4), 707714. doi: 10.1038/jcbfm.2008.163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thornberg, E., Thiringer, K., Odeback, A., & Milsom, I. (1995). Birth asphyxia: Incidence, clinical course and outcome in a Swedish population. Acta Paediatrica, 84(8), 927932. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.1995.tb13794.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ursini, G., Punzi, G., Chen, Q., Marenco, S., Robinson, J. F., Porcelli, A., … Weinberger, D. R. (2018). Convergence of placenta biology and genetic risk for schizophrenia. Nature Medicine, 24(6), 792801. doi:10.1038/s41591-018-0021-y.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Erp, T. G., Hibar, D. P., Rasmussen, J. M., Glahn, D. C., Pearlson, G. D., Andreassen, O. A., … Turner, J. A. (2016). Subcortical brain volume abnormalities in 2028 individuals with schizophrenia and 2540 healthy controls via the ENIGMA consortium. Molecular Psychiatry, 21(4), 585. doi: 10.1038/mp.2015.118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Haren, N. E. M., Setiaman, N., Koevoets, M., Baalbergen, H., Kahn, R. S., & Hillegers, M. H. J. (2020). Brain structure, IQ, and psychopathology in young offspring of patients with schizophrenia or bipolar disorder. European Psychiatry, 63(1), e5. doi:10.1192/j.eurpsy.2019.19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vieta, E., Berk, M., Schulze, T. G., Carvalho, A. F., Suppes, T., Calabrese, J. R., … Grande, I. (2018). Bipolar disorders. Nature Reviews Disease Primers, 4, 18008. doi:10.1038/nrdp.2018.8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Volpe, J. J. (2012). Neonatal encephalopathy: An inadequate term for hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy. Annals of Neurology, 72(2), 156166. doi:10.1002/ana.23647.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (1999). Manual for the Wechsler abbreviated intelligence scale (WASI). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (2007). Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence (WASI). Norwegian manual supplement. Stockholm, Sweden: Pearson.Google Scholar
Wells, R., Swaminathan, V., Sundram, S., Weinberg, D., Bruggemann, J., Jacomb, I., … Weickert, T. W. (2015). The impact of premorbid and current intellect in schizophrenia: Cognitive, symptom, and functional outcomes. Nature Partner Journal Schizophrenia, 1, 15043. doi: 10.1038/npjschz.2015.43.Google ScholarPubMed
Woods, B. T. (1998). Is schizophrenia a progressive neurodevelopmental disorder? Toward a unitary pathogenetic mechanism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 155(12), 16611670. doi: 10.1176/ajp.155.12.1661.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wortinger, L., Engen, K., Barth, C., Andreassen, O., Jørgensen, K., & Agartz, I. (2020). Asphyxia at birth affects brain structure in patients on the schizophrenia- bipolar disorder spectrum and healthy participants. Psychological Medicine, 110. doi: 10.1017/S0033291720002779.Google ScholarPubMed
Wortinger, L., Engen, K., Barth, C., Lonning, V., Jorgensen, K., Andreassen, O., … Agartz, I. (2019). Obstetric complications and intelligence in patients on the schizophrenia-bipolar spectrum and healthy participants. Psychological Medicine, 19. doi:10.1017/S0033291719002046.Google ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. ASP, demographics, ICV and IQ between groups

Figure 1

Table 2. Clinical information between patient groups

Figure 2

Fig. 1. In the SZ group, the relationship between ICV and verbal IQ was significantly greater in the presence of ASP compared to when ASP was not present, which was not found in the other groups. SZ (schizophrenia spectrum); IQ (intelligence quotient); ICV (intracranial volume); ASP (birth asphyxia).

Figure 3

Table 3. Multiple linear regression analyses by the level of IQ performance

Supplementary material: File

Wortinger et al. supplementary material

Wortinger et al. supplementary material

Download Wortinger et al. supplementary material(File)
File 35.4 KB