Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-f46jp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-01-07T08:54:27.787Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Viral social media videos can raise pro-social behaviours when an epidemic arises

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2025

Yiting Guo
Affiliation:
Economics and Management School, Wuhan University, Wuhan, HUB 430072, China
Jason Shachat*
Affiliation:
Economics and Management School, Wuhan University, Wuhan, HUB 430072, China Department of Economics and Finance, Durham University Business School, DH1 3LB Durham, UK
Matthew J. Walker
Affiliation:
Newcastle University Business School, NE1 4SE Newcastle Upon Tyne, UK
Lijia Wei
Affiliation:
Economics and Management School, Wuhan University, Wuhan, HUB 430072, China
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Access to information via social media is one of the biggest differentiators of public health crises today. During the early stages of the Covid-19 outbreak in January 2020, we conducted an experiment in Wuhan, China to assess the impact of viral social media content on pro-social and trust behaviours and preferences towards risk taking with known and unknown probabilities. Prior to the experiment, participants viewed one of two videos that had been widely and anonymously shared on Chinese social media: a central government leader visiting a local hospital and supermarket, or health care volunteers transiting to Wuhan. In a control condition, participants watched a Neutral video, unrelated to the crisis. Viewing one of the leadership or volunteer videos leads to higher levels of pro-sociality and lesser willingness to take risks in an ambiguous situation relative to the control condition. The leadership video, however, induces lower levels of trust. We provide evidence from two post-experiment surveys that the video’s impact on pro-sociality is modulated by influencing the viewer’s affective emotional state.

Type
Original Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.
Copyright
Copyright © 2021 The Author(s)

1 Introduction

On January 23, 2020, local authorities in China imposed a full lockdown of Wuhan city in response to the emergence of a novel coronavirus and associated disease, Covid-19. This event was followed shortly after by the lockdown of other cities across Hubei province. With the movements of over 50 million people in the centre of China being closely monitored, this represented one of the largest forced quarantines in human history.Footnote 1 The lockdown of Wuhan city was the first, and one of the most stringent, to be implemented globally. During those early days of uncertainty, with scant reporting in official media outlets, social media became a key source of information about the virus for ordinary Chinese citizens. The currency of social media are viral videos and associated messages; during this time, many videos were circulated in private chat groups, showing evocative scenes of containment and relief efforts.Footnote 2

Access to information via social media is one of the biggest differentiators of pandemics today from the past (Balinska & Rizzo, Reference Balinska and Rizzo2009). Social media has become an important medium that individuals turn to for information in public emergencies (Reuter & Kaufhold, Reference Reuter and Kaufhold2018). Since information systems, rather than personal experience, are the most likely source of information during a crisis, social media is an important agent of risk amplification (Kasperson & Kasperson, Reference Kasperson and Kasperson1996; Pidgeon et al., Reference Pidgeon, Kasperson and Slovic2003). Recent research recognises the potential of viral social media content to promote collective action in moments of crisis, alongside possible negative effects for trust and risk perception (Alexander, Reference Alexander2014; Haushofer & Metcalf, Reference Haushofer and Metcalf2020; Taylor et al., Reference Taylor, Wells, Howell and Raphael2012). During a public health crisis, the coordination of individual efforts towards collective demands underpins the success of mitigating measures (Van Bavel et al., Reference Van Bavel, Baicker, Boggio, Capraro, Cichocka and Cikara2020). Trust and risk attitudes are associated with the adoption of health behaviours and so indirectly for controlling the rate of disease transmission (Chuang et al., Reference Chuang, Huang, Tseng, Yen and Yang2015). It is, therefore, important to understand how social media content influences pro-social, trust and risk-related preferences.

This study examines the influences of social media content on such preferences via the modulation of a viewer’s affective emotional state. A long literature in psychology emphasises that affective states influence normative judgements and decision-making processes (Forgas, Reference Forgas1995; Loewenstein & Lerner, Reference Loewenstein, Lerner, Davidson, Scherer and Goldsmith2003; Pham, Reference Pham2007).Footnote 3 Online media content that arouses emotions is more likely to go “viral” in the first place (for an example of this in the Chinese social media context, see Fan et al., Reference Fan, Zhao, Chen and Xu2014). Thus, we conjecture that social media videos of the type used in this study may act as an incidental (that is, not normatively relevant for the judgement at hand) influence on individuals’ emotions and mood, which in turn affects their decisions.Footnote 4

An extensive experimental economics literature explores the ability of induced emotions to affect strategic behaviour and individual preferences. Capra (Reference Capra2004) finds that induced good mood leads to more altruistic giving. Drouvelis and Grosskopf (Reference Drouvelis and Grosskopf2016) and Bartke et al. (Reference Bartke, Bosworth, Snower and Chierchia2019) observe that induced positive (negative) affective states increase (diminish) pro-social behaviour in a public goods game, while findings for cooperation are mixed (Hertel & Fiedler, Reference Hertel and Fiedler1994; Hertel et al., Reference Hertel, Neuhof, Theuer and Kerr2000). Dunn and Schweitzer (Reference Dunn and Schweitzer2005) suggest that incidental anger reduces trust. In a gift-exchange game, Kirchsteiger et al. (Reference Kirchsteiger, Rigotti and Rustichini2006) observe that those individuals who watched a funny movie clip are more generous, while those who watched a sad clip are more reciprocal. Kugler et al. (Reference Kugler, Connolly and Ordóñez2012) show that induced mood can have a systematic impact on individual risk preferences: in their study, fearful participants make more risk-averse lottery choices than angry participants.

Our study consists of an economic experiment that measures the behavioural impact of watching viral social media videos, and two follow-up surveys that assess the emotional states induced by the content of those same videos. In the experiment, Wuhan-based students complete a panel of decision tasks to measure the effects of crisis-related social media video stimuli on their pro-social, cooperative and trusting behaviour, as well as their preferences towards risk taking with known and unknown probabilities. The experiment was implemented in late January 2020, the time of greatest uncertainty about the coronavirus in China. All experimental tasks are incentive compatible: all choices have monetary rewards proportional to the good outcomes of the tasks.Footnote 5

The experiment comprises two treatment conditions and a control condition, using a between-subjects design. In the treatment conditions, participants are primed with one of two videos, each of which shows evocative content related to the Covid-19 crisis. The first of these videos shows a senior central government official’s visit to a local hospital and a supermarket (henceforth “Leadership video”). The second of these videos shows health care volunteers from other provinces in transit to Wuhan (henceforth “Volunteer video”). Both videos were circulating widely and anonymously among chat groups on the Chinese social media application WeChat at the time of the experiment. In the control condition, participants watch a neutral product advertisement video, unrelated to the crisis (henceforth “Neutral video”).

We find that priming participants with either the Leadership or Volunteer video results in significantly more pro-social behaviour relative to the Neutral video, as measured by participants’ levels of altruism and expectations of reciprocity in the decision tasks. Participants’ also display lesser willingness to take risks in an ambiguous situation in the treatment conditions, although there is no systematic effect of the viral videos on risk preferences in unambiguous situations. The Leadership video alone induces a significant fall in trust, although we caution on drawing generalized conclusions from this result, because we test only one video which involves just one (prominent) government figure.

In the two follow-up surveys, we find evidence to support the argument that the videos impact behaviour through the manipulation of affective emotional states. In the first survey, we ask respondents to select five events, from a list of fifteen, which acted as psychologically positive motivating factors during the early stages of the Covid-19 crisis. The two most selected events are health care teams volunteering to assist in Hubei province and national leaders countering the epidemic. In the second survey, we evaluate the impact of viewing the videos in our experiment through a set of questionnaires that assess an inventory of emotional measurements. We find that the Leadership and Volunteer videos each induce a significant rise in positive affect relative to the Neutral video, with underlying increases in joviality and self-assuredness. Those individuals who watch the Volunteer video also report greater attentiveness. There is some evidence that viewers of the Leadership and Volunteer videos feel a comparatively greater sense of guilt.

The paper continues in Sect. 2 with a description of the experimental design and procedures. In Sect. 3, we present the results of the economic experiment. In Sect. 4, we describe the two post-experiment surveys we undertook to assess the video-induced changes in emotional states. In Sect. 5, we conclude with a discussion of how to interpret the results and outline some limitations of our study.

2 Experimental design

A timeline of our economic experiment in context of the Covid-19 outbreak in Hubei province is displayed in Fig. 1. We recruited 240 participants at random from an online database of over 9000 Wuhan University students. Our study has three experimental conditions based upon the nature of a priming social media video. We randomly assigned 80 participants to each condition. The experiment consisted of twelve sessions designed for twenty participants each. Three sessions, one for each experimental condition, were run concurrently in the morning and afternoon on January 28 and 30, 2020. Morning and afternoon sessions differed by which subset of tasks we administered. The final day of data collection coincided with the World Health Organization’s declaration of the virus as a Public Health Emergency of International Concern (WHO, Reference Organization2020).

Fig. 1 Timeline of Covid-19 events in Hubei province, 2019–2020. Figure shows the dates of experiment data collection in context of wider events relating to the public emergency

Invitations to participate in a session were sent directly to participants’ WeChat accounts, which is the most popular instant messaging app and the largest social media platform in China. At the time of the experiment, participants had already left the university for the semester break, which coincides with the annual spring festival holiday. Thus, participants were residing in twenty-nine different provinces. Forty participants were residing in Hubei province, including seventeen from Wuhan. On average, each session lasted forty-five minutes, and participants earned 63.79 RMB (about 9.5 US dollars), including a participation fee of 10 RMB. Participation in the experiment was completed using a mobile device and we transferred payment to their WeChat account immediately after completion of their experimental session.

Four canonical games, multi-persons decision problems, are considered: a dictator game (DG), an ultimatum game (UG), a trust game (TG), and a prisoner’s dilemma game (PD).Footnote 6 In the DG, players are matched into pairs and assigned to the role of player 1 or player 2. Player 1 is allotted a real sum of money (the stake) and decides how to allocate the stake between the two. Higher amounts offered to player 2 reflect greater pro-sociality on the part of player 1. The UG is the same as the DG, except that player 2 can choose to accept or reject—which results in both receiving zero—the proposed allocation. In the UG, higher amounts offered by player 1 may reflect a combination of pro-sociality and expectations of reciprocity; player 2’s decision reflects actual reciprocity. The stake used in the DG is 5 RMB, and in the UG is 8 RMB. We use different stakes to reduce the likelihood of participants anchoring on their first response when making their second.

In the TG, players are again matched into pairs, player 1 is allotted a stake and decides how much of that stake to transfer to player 2. The amount transferred is tripled, which player 1 is aware of, before reaching player 2. After player 2 receives the multiplied transfer, he or she decides how much of it to return to player 1. In the TG, the amount that player 1 sends is a measure of trust and the amount that player 2 returns reflects trustworthiness. The stake used for this game is 8 RMB.

The PD is a normal-form game in which each player chooses to either Cooperate or Defect. Choosing Defect yields a player a higher payoff than choosing Cooperate against each of the opponent’s possible choices. However, the pair’s total payoff is highest when both choose to Cooperate. The payoff to mutual cooperation was 6 RMB, to mutual defection 3 RMB, to unilateral cooperation 0 RMB and to unilateral defection 9 RMB.

We also include tasks designed to elicit preferences towards risk taking with known and unknown probabilities.Footnote 7 The risk preference elicitation task with known probabilities involves a series of nine pairwise choices between a lottery (option A) and a sure amount of money (option B). The lottery remains fixed across all choices: a 50% chance of receiving 9 RMB, and a 50% chance of receiving 3 RMB. The sure amount increases evenly with each choice from 3 RMB up to 9 RMB. The task to elicit preferences over risk taking in an ambiguous situation is identical except that the lottery is unknown. Participants are informed that if they choose option A, a ball is randomly drawn from an opaque urn. The urn contains both red and blue balls, but the number of each colour is unknown. If the draw is red, they earn 9 RMB. If the draw is blue, they earn 3 RMB. One choice from each risk/ambiguity elicitation task is randomly drawn for payment.

Before completing the decision tasks, participants watch a once repeated video of approximately two minutes in length.Footnote 8 To mitigate the possibility of experimenter demand effects, we did not provide any cover story or description to accompany the videos. In the control condition, participants are shown a product advertisement for the sculpting of a plastic bottle, unrelated to the Covid-19 crisis (the Neutral video). By contrast, the two treatment conditions each feature a video that had been circulating widely and anonymously on Chinese social media around the time of the experiment.

In the Leadership video treatment, participants are shown excerpts from the visit of a senior central government official to Wuhan Jinyintan Hospital—the first hospital designated to treat patients with Covid-19—and to a local supermarket, on January 27, 2020. In this video, the senior central government official uses morale-boosting words and phrases such as “protect”, “save lives” and “empowered”, to which the doctors and customers alike respond enthusiastically. In the Volunteer video treatment, participants are shown a video of health care volunteers from other provinces in transit to Wuhan. The video pans to teams from various hospitals and acknowledges their place of origin and number, in almost regimental style. Exclamations are heard over the public tannoy such as “go Wuhan!” and “we will carry the burden with you”.

We summarize our economic experiment and the data collection process in Table 1. The decision-making tasks were completed sequentially. In the morning sessions we excluded the UG task, and in the afternoon sessions we excluded the TG task. We did this to rule out the possibility of a behavioural spillover effect; specifically, the second-movers conditioning their actions on the first movers’ actions in both the UG and TG games. We only informed participants of their respective task outcomes and earnings after all decision tasks were completed. No individual participated in more than one session and all sessions consisted of 20 participants, except one session in which there wereonly 16 participants due to participant no-shows. To ensure consistency of the interface, four participants are excluded from the sample for using a computer rather than mobile device to complete the experiment.

Table 1 Experimental design and session information

Treatment

Jan 28 morning

Jan 28 afternoon

Jan 30 morning

Jan 30 afternoon

Neutral video

20 participants

20 participantsa

20 participants

20 participants

Volunteer video

16 participants

20 participants

20 participants

20 participants

Leadership video

20 participantsb

20 participants

20 participants

20 participants

Excluded task

Ultimatum game

Trust game

Ultimatum game

Trust game

aID 5 & 20 used a computer and are excluded from the analysis

bID 12 & 18 used a computer and are excluded from the analysis

3 Experimental results

The aggregate outcomes of the lab experiment are presented in Table 2. The Leadership (L) and Volunteer (V) videos have marginally significant positive effects on pro-sociality in the experiment relative to the Neutral (N) video. In the DG, both videos increase the average amount sent by dictators (p value = 0.08 and p value = 0.07, respectively, n L = n V = 38 and n N = 39). There is also evidence that participants offer higher amounts in the UG. While this increase is not significant at conventional thresholds for the Leadership video versus the Neutral video, it is for the Volunteer video (p value = 0.12 and p value = 0.03, respectively, nL = 20, n V = 20 and n N = 19). The corresponding difference between Leadership and Volunteer video conditions, however, is not significant (p value = 0.54). UG acceptance rates are lower in the Neutral video condition in response to the reduced offers, although higher variability means that the pairwise differences are not significant (p value = 0.15 and p value = 0.15, respectively, n L = 20, n V = 20 and n N = 19).

Table 2 Key outcome measures in the experimental treatments

Task (response range)

N c

Definitions: higher value implies

Neutral video

Leadership video

Volunteer video

DG amount senta (0–5)

39/38/38

Pro-sociality

1.37 (1.01)

1.76* (0.99)

1.84* (1.03)

UG offera (0–8)

19/20/20

Pro-sociality or expectations of reciprocity norms

2.58 (1.43)

3.23 (1.24)

3.55** (0.83)

UG acceptance rateb (accept = 1, reject = 0)

19/20/20

Actual reciprocity norms

0.79 (0.42)

0.95 (0.22)

0.95 (0.22)

TG amount senta (0–8)

20/18/18

Trust

3.20 (2.28)

2.06* (1.98)

3.11 (2.78)

TG returna (0– 24)

20/20/18

Trustworthiness

3.05 (3.78)

2.1 (3.38)

3.21 (4.38)

PD cooperation rateb (cooperate = 1, defect = 0)

78/78/76

Cooperation

0.40 (0.49)

0.46 (0.50)

0.45 (0.50)

Risk preferencea,d (0–9)

78/77/75

Willingness to take risks

4.78 (1.54)

4.75 (1.46)

4.79 (1.48)

Ambiguity preferencea,d (0–9)

77/74/75

Willingness to take risks under ambiguity

4.53 (1.51)

4.14** (1.44)

3.96** (1.33)

*p < 0.1; **p < 0.05; ***p < 0.01. Mean (SD) values are presented in the table

aTwo-tailed Wilcoxon rank-sum test

bTwo-proportions z test

cThe convention is number of observations by Neutral/Leadership/Volunteer video treatment

dFirst row in which the certain option was chosen. We excluded responses from participants exhibiting inconsistent preference, defined as switching between the lottery and the sure amount of money options more than once (no participant switched only once in the “wrong direction”, i.e. from a low certain amount to the lottery). For the Leadership video, this required excluding one response for risk and four for ambiguity; for the Volunteer video, this required excluding one response for risk and one for ambiguity; and for the Neutral video, this required excluding no responses for risk and one for ambiguity

Interestingly, the Leadership video appears to undermine trust. Amounts sent by first movers in the TG are lower in the Leadership video treatment than in the Neutral video control, although this difference is only marginally significant (p value = 0.09, n L = 18 and n N = 20). We find no significant negative effect of the Volunteer video on trust (p value = 0.82, n V = 18 and n N = 20) and no significant difference in the raw TG amount sent between the Leadership and Volunteer video (p value = 0.26). Reciprocity, in terms of amounts returned, adjusts proportionally in each experimental condition.Footnote 9 Consistent with earlier TG experiments, a trustor’s decision to transfer money is on average a breakeven strategy (Berg et al., Reference Berg, Dickhaut and McCabe1995). There are no statistically significant pairwise differences in average PD cooperation rates in the respective treatment conditions relative to the control condition (p value = 0.52 and p value = 0.64, respectively, n L = n N = 78 and n V = 76).

An ancillary question is how the treatment condition videos influence participants’ preferences towards risk taking with known and unknown probabilities. To examine this question, we use the first row in which the certain option was chosen in the risk and ambiguity preference elicitation tasks. For each task, there were nine rows; risk neutrality corresponds to choosing the certain amount for the first time in row five. While neither the Leadership nor Volunteer video significantly influences risk aversion with known probabilities (p value = 0.70 and p value = 0.85, respectively, n L = 77 n V = 75 and n N = 78), both videos reduce participants’ willingness to take risks in an ambiguous situation (p value = 0.03 and p value = 0.02, respectively, n L = 74, n V = 75 and n N = 77). We advise against reading into the absolute levels recorded in the ambiguity preference elicitation task, because our implementation did not permit participants to choose the payoff-relevant colour and so this task may also pick up trust in the experimenter.Footnote 10

That participants’ reactions to the treatment condition videos differ across risk and ambiguity domains is not unusual (see also Cavatorta & Groom, Reference Cavatorta and Groom2020). Whereas risk involves known probabilities, ambiguity describes situations in which probabilities are vague. If the treatment videos heightened feelings of uncertainty about the situation in Wuhan in those early days of the Covid-19 outbreak, individuals may have been prone to act more cautiously when the likelihoods of outcomes associated with their actions are unknown. It does not necessarily follow that they would also prefer to take fewer risks. The probabilities of bad outcomes in an emerging public health crisis are far from well-defined.

Due in part to features of our design (e.g. no role uncertainty, alternation of UG and TG tasks across sessions to preclude second-mover learning), our non-parametric analyses above rest on small sample sizes per condition and so lack statistical power to detect effects. To address this issue, we conduct an individual-level regression analysis of our key outcome measures on indicator variables for the two treatment condition videos, while controlling for other demographic and local factors that might be influencing behaviour. We include as covariates the aggregate number of diagnosed virus cases at provincial level, participant gender, cell phone operating system and screen size.

The results of the regression analysis are presented in Table 3.Footnote 11 We find support for the positive impact of the Leadership and Volunteer videos on first-mover offers in the UG, suggesting that the content of these videos increased expectations of reciprocity norms relative to the Neutral video. We also find some support for our observation that the Volunteer video induces a significant increase in giving by dictators in the DG. While the coefficient estimate on the Leadership video indicator is positive, it is not significant at conventional levels. Since we are primarily interested in the effect of the Leadership and Volunteer videos on “pro-sociality” broadly defined, rather than behaviour in a specific task, we pool standardized first-mover data from the DG and UG and control for task indicators in a random-effects model. There is strong statistical evidence that both treatment condition videos cause an increase in pro-sociality relative to the Neutral video.Footnote 12

Table 3 Regression analysis of covariance

Dependent variable

DG sent

UG offer

Pro-socialityd

Ambiguity preference

TG sent

TG sent

OLS

OLS

Panel

OLS

OLS

OLS

(1)

(2)

(3)

(4)

(5)

(6)

Diagnosed casesa

0.031 (0.062)

− 0.125 (0.098)

− 0.013 (0.013)

0.023 (0.054)

− 0.363* (0.209)

− 0.460* (0.272)

Volunteer videob

0.414* (0.242)

1.200*** (0.387)

0.149*** (0.053)

− 0.578** (0.225)

− 0.503 (0.813)

− 0.409 (0.869)

Leadership video

0.334 (0.237)

0.857** (0.435)

0.111** (0.052)

− 0.334 (0.239)

− 1.496** (0.716)

− 1.467* (0.799)

Risk preference

− 0.315 (0.337)

Ambiguity preference

0.097 (0.269)

PD cooperation

0.016 (0.680)

Constant

2.522* (1.042)

3.804 (3.736)

0.654 (0.423)

3.185** (1.564)

6.646 (6.461)

7.891 (5.787)

Wald test stat. for diff in treatment videosc

0.11

0.79

0.60

1.04

1.59

1.48

Control variables

Yes

Yes

Yes

Yes

Yes

Yes

Random effects

No

No

Yes

No

No

No

Observations

114

59

118

224

55

54

R-squared

0.043

0.158

0.098

0.052

0.153

0.165

*p < 0.1; **p < 0.05; ***p < 0.01. Robust standard errors are shown in the parentheses, calculated using the Huber/White sandwich estimator of variance. Control variables include gender, cell phone operating system and screen size. In regression models (1) and (5), one subject is dropped due to a missing value for the screen size variable; in models (4) and (6), two subjects are dropped for this reason. In model (3), random effects are included at the individual-level

aLog transformation of the aggregate number of diagnosed virus cases at the provincial level by the midnight of the previous day, based on data from CDC China

bThe reference video category is the Neutral video

cLinear hypothesis test: coefficient of Volunteer video is equal to coefficient of Leadership video

dStandardized outcome data on DG amount sent and UG offer; DG task indicator included as control variable

In Table 3, we further examine the impact of the Volunteer and Leadership video on ambiguity preferences. The Volunteer video has a robust negative effect on willingness to take risks in an ambiguous situation which is significant at the 5% level; the coefficient estimate on the Leadership video is negative but not significant at the 10% level.

To estimate the impact of the treatment videos on amounts sent by first movers in the TG, we first conduct a straightforward OLS regression. The Leadership video has a robust negative effect on trust which is significant at the 5% level. The Volunteer video has no significant effect on trust. We then control for subjects’ decisions in the risk and ambiguity tasks, and in the PD game, to isolate the effect of the videos on trust net of pro-sociality, risk and ambiguity aversion.Footnote 13 The negative impact of the Leadership video on trust remains significant at the 10% level. Scholars in the experimental economics literature have long debated whether risk preferences are a component of trusting behaviour (e.g. Ben-Ner & Halldorsson, Reference Ben-Ner and Halldorsson2010; Eckel & Wilson, Reference Eckel and Wilson2004). The decision to take a “social risk” on the trustworthiness of a stranger in the one-shot TG might be viewed as conceptually closer to a preference over ambiguity (Li et al., Reference Li, Turmunkh and Wakker2019). Our results are consistent with the interpretation of the Leadership video—but not the Volunteer video—undermining the component of trust which is unrelated to preferences over risk, ambiguity and pro-sociality. There is, however, no significant difference between the Leadership and Volunteer video coefficient estimates. We will return to discuss these differential effects further in Sect. 5.

4 Two survey studies on the emotional states induced by the social media videos

To gain insight into the channels mediating the differential effects observed in our economic experiment, we provide two post-experiment assessments on how the content of the social media videos are perceived. The first assessment is a follow-up survey we administered from March 12 to 20, 2020 to 5,686 non-student individuals around China. Among other questions, we asked respondents to select five events, from a list of fifteen, which acted as psychologically positive motivating factors in the early stages of the Covid-19 crisis (see Fig. 2).Footnote 14 The two most selected events were health care teams volunteer to assist in Hubei province and national leaders countering the epidemic (77.67% and 66.43%, respectively).

Fig. 2 Positive motivating factors during the Covid-19 outbreak. Figure presents the results of a survey question administered from March 12–20, 2020 to 5686 non-student individuals around China. The survey question asked respondents to consider the development of the epidemic and select five factors that had provided them with positive psychological motivation

In the second assessment, we evaluate the impact of video viewing on a more extensive inventory of emotional measurements.Footnote 15 We recruited 241 new participants between September 11 and 18, 2020, from the same population of Wuhan-based students as our original experiment. As before, we randomly assigned 80 participants to each experimental condition (81 to the Leadership video condition) and used WeChat for the experimental implementation. At the start of the survey each participant watched their randomly assigned video twice. Participants were told only that the video they were about to watch was recorded and edited within a week of Wuhan’s January lockdown.

After watching their randomly assigned video, participants were asked to complete three psychological questionnaires. These were the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS, Watson et al., Reference Watson, Clark and Tellegen1988), Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI, Davis, Reference Davis1983) and Sense of Control (SOC, Lachman & Weaver, Reference Lachman and Weaver1998).Footnote 16 The PANAS contains two higher-order scales measuring positive and negative affect, which can be decomposed into sub-scales measuring specific positive emotions (e.g. fear, hostility, guilt) and negative emotions (e.g. joviality, self-assurance, attentiveness). The IRI is often used in the social sciences to measure pro-social behaviours along four dimensions: empathy, perspective taking, personal distress and fantasy (e.g. Büchner et al., Reference Büchner, Coricelli and Greiner2007; Carlo et al., Reference Carlo, Allen and Buhman1999). The SOC provides a generalized measure of competence and contingency along two dimensions: personal mastery and perceived constraints. All responses are measured on Likert scales, with a higher number on the scale indicating greater agreement with the described feeling or emotion at the present moment. The sequence in which the three questionnaires were presented to participants in the experiment was randomized, to control for possible order effects. On completion of the psychological questionnaires, participants were asked to fill in demographic information.Footnote 17 They received a show-up fee of 5 RMB, and a payment of 8 RMB for each questionnaire completed. The total completion time was less than 30 min.

We find significant increases in positive affect induced by the Leadership and Volunteer videos relative to the Neutral video (Table 4). Participants in both treatment conditions describe feeling relatively more self-assured and jovial (all pairwise comparison p values < 0.01). There is some evidence that participants who watched the Volunteer video also feel more attentive than those who watched the Neutral video (p value = 0.08), and more active, inspired and interested than those who watched the Neutral video (p value < 0.01) or Leadership video (p value = 0.06).Footnote 18

Table 4 Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) outcomes

Item

Emotions

Neutral video (N = 80)

Leadership video (N = 81)

Volunteer video (N = 80)

Higher-order scales

Positive affect (10–50)

Active, alert, attentive, determined, enthusiastic, excited, inspired, interested, proud, strong

29.01 (8.48)

32.38*** (6.88)

33.82*** (7.24)

Negative affect (10–50)

Afraid, scared, nervous, jittery, irritable, hostile, guilty, ashamed, upset, distressed

19.69 (7.99)

20.68 (7.61)

20.88 (8.15)

Positive emotion scales

Self-assurance

Proud, strong

5.38 (2.05)

6.58*** (1.87)

7.01*** (1.95)

Attentiveness

Alert, attentive, determined

8.78 (2.67)

9.37 (2.09)

9.55* (2.38)

Joviality

Enthusiastic, excited

5.95 (1.94)

6.69*** (1.73)

6.73*** (1.86)

Other

Active, inspired, interested

8.91 (2.87)

9.74* (2.47)

10.53***,# (2.39)

Negative emotion scales

Fear

Afraid, scared, nervous, jittery

7.99 (3.77)

8.37 (3.74)

8.63 (3.70)

Hostility

Hostile, irritable

3.38 (1.54)

3.14 (1.39)

3.19 (1.50)

Guilt

Guilty, ashamed

3.82 (1.79)

4.28* (1.97)

4.34* (1.77)

Other

Upset, distressed

4.51 (2.19)

4.89 (2.01)

4.73 (2.28)

*[#] p < 0.1; **p < 0.05; ***p < 0.01; based on two-tailed Wilcoxon rank-sum test versus the Neutral [Leadership] video. Mean (SD) values are presented in the table

By contrast, we find no significant difference in negative affect for either the Leadership video (p value = 0.42) or Volunteer video (p value = 0.35), and little underlying variability in the negative emotions of fear or hostility. An interesting exception is that participants in both the Leadership and Volunteer video conditions describe feeling a greater sense of guilt than their counterparts in the Neutral video condition (respectively, p value = 0.07 and p value = 0.08). We observe no systematic differences in responses to the IRI or SOC questionnaires.Footnote 19

The implied positive relationship between positive affect and pro-sociality in the economic experiment is consistent with prior studies (Bartke et al., Reference Bartke, Bosworth, Snower and Chierchia2019; Capra, Reference Capra2004; Drouvelis & Grosskopf, Reference Drouvelis and Grosskopf2016). The relative effect sizes also support this interpretation. Compared to the Neutral video, the Volunteer video induced a larger emotional response across all four positive emotion sub-scales than the Leadership video (see second panel of Table 4), which translated into consistently larger behavioural estimates of the video’s impact on pro-sociality (see Table 2 and columns 1–3 of Table 3).

Neither treatment condition video significantly affected fear, which Kugler et al. (Reference Kugler, Connolly and Ordóñez2012) observed to increase risk aversion, and we find no significant difference in risk preferences versus the control condition. While it is plausible that heightened feelings of guilt are behind the fall in willingness to take risks in an ambiguous situation, we do not know of a precedent for this channel and cannot disentangle it from the corresponding increase in positive affect. The elicited emotions are also unable to explain the fall in trust induced by the Leadership video alone, as we discuss below.

5 Discussion and conclusion

The results of our economic experiment suggest that viral social media videos, a distinctive feature of modern crises, can influence behaviour and preferences. We find that, in the early days of the Covid-19 outbreak in China, videos showing either a prominent government leader’s visit to Wuhan or the inward transit of health care volunteers induce greater pro-sociality, relative to a neutral video unrelated to the crisis. In the field, this could translate to a rise in donations, assistance and willingness to comply with mandated health behaviours. On the other hand, the Leadership video used in our study had the unintended consequence of decreasing individuals’ levels of trust. This may undermine the authorities’ effectiveness in crisis response efforts.

It is important to keep in mind that these videos are context-specific and not representative of the macrocosm of social media videos circulating on Chinese social media platforms during January 2020. We cannot rule out that participants’ perceptions of the Leadership video are biased by their preconceptions about the particular leader involved. One should remain wary of extrapolating our findings as to the effect of viral media content on behaviour to other populations.

We also uncover evidence that the social media videos used in our study had an incidental influence on individuals’ emotions. Specifically, the Leadership and Volunteer videos induced significant increases in positive affect and mood, which may plausibly have driven the increase in pro-social behaviours. This complements evidence from hypothetical survey response data that emotional responses to a pandemic correlate with intended compliant behaviours (Rubin et al., Reference Rubin, Amlôt, Rogers, Hall, Leach, Simpson and Wessely2010; Sadique et al., Reference Sadique, Edmunds, Smith, Meerding, De Zwart, Brug and Beutels2007). Two caveats should be noted here.

First, our measurements of emotional responses to the video stimuli were elicited nearly 8 months after the behavioural measures obtained in the economic experiment. Thus, it is possible that the videos would have induced different emotional responses between the two time periods. In this regard, we take re-assurance from the consistency of our psychological response data with the representative follow-up survey conducted in early March 2020, which indicate that the events broadcast in the two social media videos were perceived among ordinary Chinese citizens as positive motivating factors.

Second, we observe few systematic differences in emotional responses between the Leadership and Volunteer videos themselves, and so emotions cannot well explain the differential impact of the Leadership video on trust. An alternative explanation for this finding is that the content of the two videos had differential cognitive (rather than affective) influences. Li et al. (Reference Li, Turmunkh and Wakker2019) show that participants’ inability to discriminate between events with different likelihoods dampens their tendency to act based on beliefs; conditional on equally optimistic beliefs about the other’s trustworthiness, the more insensitive individuals are less inclined to trust. If, as implied by our behavioural measurements, the Leadership and Volunteer videos increased participants’ pro-sociality, but decreased their willingness to take risks under ambiguity, then the net outcome for trust may be a null effect (as observed in the Volunteer video). If the Leadership video further reduced the ability of participants to discriminate between different likelihood levels, then a negative impact on trust would result in this condition.

A further possibility is that the source of the video influences behaviour. The direction of communication in the Leadership video is from leader to citizen, whereas the direction of communication in the Volunteer video is citizen to citizen. Leadership studies have traditionally suggested a top–down communication style is most effective in establishing trust and conformity (the centralization thesis, e.g. 't Hart et al., Reference ‘t Hart, Rosenthal and Kouzmin1993; McEntire & Dawson, Reference McEntire, Dawson, Waugh and Tierney2007). Other literature has challenged the veracity of the centralization thesis in times of crisis, postulating that bottom–up communication styles are more effective (Boin and 't Hart, Reference Boin and ‘t Hart2003; Demiroz & Kapucu, Reference Demiroz and Kapucu2012). On this interpretation, our experimental findings on trust would come out in favour of the critics.

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at https://doi.org/10.1007/s40881-021-00104-w.

Funding

The authors gratefully acknowledge Natural Science Foundation of China under Grant number 72173093 and by the ESRC/FNR under Grant number ES/P011829/1.

Availability of data and material

Data files are available from the authors on request. Experimental instructions and further details are contained in the Supplementary Material.

Code availability

Computing scripts to replicate the data analyses are available from the authors on request.

Declarations

Conflict of interest

We declare that we have no competing interests.

Institutional review board

This research was approved by the Academy of Humanities and Social Sciences of Wuhan University.

Footnotes

3 Various experimental economics studies also find a correlation between emotions and behaviour (Bolle et al., Reference Bolle, Tan and Zizzo2014; Charness & Grosskopf, Reference Charness and Grosskopf2001; Hopfensitz & Reuben, Reference Hopfensitz and Reuben2009; Joffily et al., Reference Joffily, Masclet, Noussair and Villeval2014; Meshulam et al., Reference Meshulam, Winter, Ben-Shakhar and Aharon2012; Sanfey et al., Reference Sanfey, Rilling, Aronson, Nystrom and Cohen2003; Schniter & Sheremeta Reference Schniter and Sheremeta2014; Xiao & Houser, Reference Xiao and Houser2005).

4 Psychologists distinguish between emotions (intense and short-lived feelings) and mood (diffuse feelings that tend to persist). For a discussion, see Lerner et al. (Reference Lerner, Li, Valdesolo and Kassam2015). We do not pursue this distinction further here.

5 This contrasts with psychometric-style questionnaires conducted during previous public health emergencies (Bults et al., Reference Bults, Beaujean, de Zwart, Kok, van Empelen and van Steenbergen2011; Jones & Salathé, Reference Jones and Salathé2009; Leung et al., Reference Leung, Ho, Chan, Ho, Bacon-Shone and Choy2005; Prati et al., Reference Prati, Pietrantoni and Zani2011; van der Weerd et al., Reference van der Weerd, Timmermans, Beaujean, Oudhoff and van Steenbergen2011).

6 An additional two games were implemented to elicit measures of cognitive reasoning and perceptions of strategic uncertainty. Since these games are not directly related to pro-sociality, we do not report on them here. Details of these games are available in the Supplementary Material.

7 An additional four tasks were implemented to measure preferences for truth-telling, time preferences and aversion to losses. Details are contained in the Supplementary Material; for a discussion of some related results using a different dataset, see Shachat et al. (Reference Shachat, Walker and Wei2021).

8 Specifically, the Leadership video lasts 2 min and 3 s, the Volunteer video lasts 1 min and 47 s, and the Neutral video lasts 1 min and 54 s. The videos used in this study (subtitled in English) and corresponding transcripts are available from the authors on request.

9 We also exclude observations in which first movers sent zerorns and calculate a measure of trustworthiness based on returns relative to the amount sent; there is no significant pairwise difference in this measure between any two of the three conditions.

10 Since we focus on relative comparisons, this is a relevant confound only if the videos had differential impacts on participants’ trust in the experimenter. While we cannot rule this out, we observe a (marginally significant) fall in generalized trust in the Leadership video condition, but not the Volunteer video condition, and so if this were a confound, we might expect to observe a larger negative effect on risk taking under ambiguity in the Leadership video condition. Yet we observe comparable effects of the two videos.

11 In the Supplementary Material, we also present regression analyses for second-mover behaviour in the TG and UG, respectively (Table S2). Amounts returned/acceptance rates are significantly increasing in the first-mover amount sent/offered, but there is no differential impact by treatment video. In a further set of regressions, we find no significant impact of the Leadership or Volunteer video on risk preferences and PD cooperation rates (Table S3), nor on pooled risk and ambiguity decisions in a random effects model (Table S4).

12 We are grateful to the editors for this suggestion. Random effects are included at the individual-level. Since the regression results reported in column 3 of Table 3 are based only on those subjects who played both the DG and the UG, the change in statistical significance on the Leadership video coefficient may be driven by a change in sample rather than by a treatment effect. To check this possibility, we compare giving in our sample between participants in the morning and afternoon sessions; there is no significant difference in mean DG amount sent between the morning subsample (1.53) and afternoon subsample (1.78) based on a two-tailed Wilcoxon rank-sum test (p value = 0.28).

13 In the Supplementary Material, we also present the results from a two-step regression to proxy for aspects of trust not directly captured by the tasks implemented (Table S5). The results are qualitatively unchanged. A limitation of the regression approach to decomposing trust is that measurement error may be a non-trivial confound (Gillen et al., Reference Gillen, Snowberg and Yariv2018). Further drawbacks are the restrictive assumption of a linear relationship and potential omitted variable bias. An alternative, non-parametric approach is to use exogenous treatment variation (Engelmann et al., Reference Engelmann, Friedrichsen, Veldhuizen, Vorjohann and Winter2021).

14 Full details of the survey administration are included in the Supplementary Material.

15 We thank an anonymous reviewer for this suggestion.

16 The complete questionnaires are contained in the Supplementary Material.

17 Demographic characteristics of this follow-up sample are comparable to the main experiment (Tables S1 and S6).

18 We observe no other significant difference in emotional response between the Leadership and Volunteer video treatments.

19 Results of the IRI and SOC can be found in the Supplementary Material (Table S7).

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

Alexander, D. E. (2014). Social media in disaster risk reduction and crisis management. Science and Engineering Ethics, 20(3), 717733. 10.1007/s11948-013-9502-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Balinska, M., Rizzo, C. (2009). Behavioural responses to influenza pandemics: What do we know? PLoS Currents Influenza, 1, RRN1037.Google ScholarPubMed
Bartke, S., Bosworth, S. J., Snower, D. J., Chierchia, G. (2019). Motives and comprehension in a public goods game with induced emotions. Theory and Decision, 86(2), 205238. 10.1007/s11238-018-9677-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ben-Ner, A., Halldorsson, F. (2010). Trusting and trustworthiness: What are they, how to measure them, and what affects them. Journal of Economic Psychology, 31(1), 6479. 10.1016/j.joep.2009.10.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berg, J., Dickhaut, J., McCabe, K. (1995). Trust, reciprocity, and social history. Games and Economic Behavior, 10(1), 122142. 10.1006/game.1995.1027CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boin, A., ‘t Hart, P. (2003). Public leadership in times of crisis: Mission impossible? Public Administration Review, 63(5), 544553. 10.1111/1540-6210.00318CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolle, F., Tan, J. H. W., Zizzo, D. J. (2014). Vendettas. American Economic Journal: Microeconomics, 6(2), 93130.Google Scholar
Büchner, S., Coricelli, G., Greiner, B. (2007). Self-centered and other-regarding behavior in the solidarity game. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 62(2), 293303. 10.1016/j.jebo.2004.12.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bults, M., Beaujean, D. J., de Zwart, O., Kok, G., van Empelen, P., van Steenbergen, J. E. et al., (2011). Perceived risk, anxiety, and behavioural responses of the general public during the early phase of the Influenza A (H1N1) pandemic in the Netherlands: Results of three consecutive online surveys. BMC Public Health, 11(1),2. 10.1186/1471-2458-11-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Capra, M. C. (2004). Mood-driven behavior in strategic interactions. American Economic Review, 94(2), 367372. 10.1257/0002828041301885CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlo, G., Allen, J. B., Buhman, D. C. (1999). Facilitating and disinhibiting prosocial behaviors: The nonlinear interaction of trait perspective taking and trait personal distress on volunteering. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 21(3), 189197. 10.1207/S15324834BASP2103_3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cavatorta, E., Groom, B. (2020). Does deterrence change preferences? Evidence from a natural experiment. European Economic Review, 127, 103456. 10.1016/j.euroecorev.2020.103456CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charness, G., Grosskopf, B. (2001). Relative payoffs and happiness: An experimental study. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 45(3), 301328. 10.1016/S0167-2681(01)00148-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chuang, Y.-C., Huang, Y.-L., Tseng, K.-C., Yen, C.-H., Yang, L.-H. (2015). Social capital and health-protective behavior intentions in an influenza pandemic. PLoS ONE, 10(4),e0122970. 10.1371/journal.pone.0122970CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44(1), 113126. 10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Demiroz, F., & Kapucu, N. (2012). The role of leadership in managing emergencies and disasters. European Journal of Economic & Political Studies, 5(1), 91101.Google Scholar
Drouvelis, M., Grosskopf, B. (2016). The effects of induced emotions on pro-social behaviour. Journal of Public Economics, 134, 18. 10.1016/j.jpubeco.2015.12.012CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, J. R., Schweitzer, M. E. (2005). Feeling and believing: The influence of emotion on trust. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(5), 736748. 10.1037/0022-3514.88.5.736CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eckel, C. C., Wilson, R. K. (2004). Is trust a risky decision? Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 55(4), 447465. 10.1016/j.jebo.2003.11.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engelmann, D., Friedrichsen, J., Veldhuizen, R. V., Vorjohann, P., & Winter, J. (2021). Decomposing trust (manuscript in preparation).Google Scholar
Fan, R., Zhao, J., Chen, Y., Xu, K. (2014). Anger is more influential than joy: Sentiment correlation in Weibo. PLoS ONE, 9(10),e110184. 10.1371/journal.pone.0110184CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forgas, J. P. (1995). Mood and judgment: The affect infusion model (AIM). Psychological Bulletin, 117(1), 3966. 10.1037/0033-2909.117.1.39CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gillen, B., Snowberg, E., Yariv, L. (2018). Experimenting with measurement error: Techniques with applications to the Caltech cohort study. Journal of Political Economy, 127(4), 18261863. 10.1086/701681CrossRefGoogle Scholar
‘t Hart, P., Rosenthal, U., Kouzmin, A. (1993). Crisis decision making: The centralization thesis revisited. Administration & Society, 25(1), 1245. 10.1177/009539979302500102CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haushofer, J., & Metcalf, J. C. E. (2020). Combining behavioral economics and infectious disease epidemiology to mitigate the COVID-19 outbreak. Princeton University. Retrieved March 6.Google Scholar
Hertel, G., Fiedler, K. (1994). Affective and cognitive influences in social dilemma game. European Journal of Social Psychology, 24(1), 131145. 10.1002/ejsp.2420240110CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hertel, G., Neuhof, J., Theuer, T., Kerr, N. L. (2000). Mood effects on cooperation in small groups: Does positive mood simply lead to more cooperation? Cognition and Emotion, 14(4), 441472. 10.1080/026999300402754CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopfensitz, A., Reuben, E. (2009). The importance of emotions for the effectiveness of social punishment. Economic Journal, 119(540), 15341559. 10.1111/j.1468-0297.2009.02288.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joffily, M., Masclet, D., Noussair, C. N., Villeval, M. C. (2014). Emotions, sanctions, and cooperation. Southern Economic Journal, 80(4), 10021027. 10.4284/0038-4038-2012.067CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, J. H., Salathé, M. (2009). Early assessment of anxiety and behavioral response to novel swine-origin influenza A (H1N1). PLoS ONE, 4(12),e8032. 10.1371/journal.pone.0008032CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kasperson, R. E., Kasperson, J. X. (1996). The social amplification and attenuation of risk. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 545(1), 95105. 10.1177/0002716296545001010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirchsteiger, G., Rigotti, L., Rustichini, A. (2006). Your morals might be your moods. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 59(2), 155172. 10.1016/j.jebo.2004.07.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kugler, T., Connolly, T., Ordóñez, L. D. (2012). Emotion, decision, and risk: Betting on gambles versus betting on people. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 25(2), 123134. 10.1002/bdm.724CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lachman, M. E., Weaver, S. L. (1998). The sense of control as a moderator of social class differences in health and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(3), 763773. 10.1037/0022-3514.74.3.763CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lerner, J. S., Li, Y., Valdesolo, P., Kassam, K. S. (2015). Emotion and decision making. Annual Review of Psychology, 66(1), 799823. 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115043CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leung, G. M., Ho, L.-M., Chan, S. K., Ho, S.-Y., Bacon-Shone, J., Choy, R. Y. et al., (2005). Longitudinal assessment of community psychobehavioral responses during and after the 2003 outbreak of severe acute respiratory syndrome in Hong Kong. Clinical Infectious Diseases, 40(12), 17131720. 10.1086/429923CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, C., Turmunkh, U., Wakker, P. P. (2019). Trust as a decision under ambiguity. Experimental Economics, 22(1), 5175. 10.1007/s10683-018-9582-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loewenstein, G., Lerner, J. S., & Davidson, R. J., Scherer, K. R., Goldsmith, K. R. (2003). The role of affect in decision making Handbook of affective science, Oxford University Press 619642.Google Scholar
McEntire, D., Dawson, G., & Waugh, W. L. J., Tierney, K. (2007). The Intergovernmental Context Emergency management: Principles and practice for local government, 2ICMA 5770.Google Scholar
Meshulam, M., Winter, E., Ben-Shakhar, G., Aharon, I. (2012). Rational emotions. Social Neuroscience, 7(1), 1117. 10.1080/17470919.2011.559124CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pham, M. T. (2007). Emotion and rationality: A critical review and interpretation of empirical evidence. Review of General Psychology, 11(2), 155178. 10.1037/1089-2680.11.2.155CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pidgeon, N., Kasperson, R. E., Slovic, P. (2003). The social amplification of risk, Cambridge University Press 10.1017/CBO9780511550461CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prati, G., Pietrantoni, L., Zani, B. (2011). Compliance with recommendations for pandemic influenza H1N1 2009: The role of trust and personal beliefs. Health Education Research, 26(5), 761769. 10.1093/her/cyr035CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reuter, C., Kaufhold, M. A. (2018). Fifteen years of social media in emergencies: A retrospective review and future directions for crisis informatics. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management, 26(1), 4157. 10.1111/1468-5973.12196CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rubin, G. J., Amlôt, R., Rogers, M. B., Hall, I., Leach, S., Simpson, J., Wessely, S. (2010). Perceptions and reactions with regard to pneumonic plague. Emerging Infectious Diseases, 16(1),120. 10.3201/eid1601.081604CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sadique, M. Z., Edmunds, W. J., Smith, R. D., Meerding, W. J., De Zwart, O., Brug, J., Beutels, P. (2007). Precautionary behavior in response to perceived threat of pandemic influenza. Emerging Infectious Diseases, 13(9),1307. 10.3201/eid1309.070372CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanfey, A. G., Rilling, J. K., Aronson, J. A., Nystrom, L. E., Cohen, J. D. (2003). The neural basis of economic decision-making in the ultimatum game. Science, 300(5626),1755. 10.1126/science.1082976CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schniter, E., & Sheremeta, R. M. (2014). Predictable and predictive emotions: explaining cheap signals and trust re-extension. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8(401), 401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shachat, J., Walker, M. J., Wei, L. (2021). The impact of an epidemic: Experimental evidence on preference stability from Wuhan. AEA Papers and Proceedings, 111, 302–306. 10.1257/pandp.20211002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, M., Wells, G., Howell, G., Raphael, B. (2012). The role of social media as psychological first aid as a support to community resilience building. Australian Journal of Emergency Management, the, 27(1),20.Google Scholar
Van Bavel, J. J., Baicker, K., Boggio, P. S., Capraro, V., Cichocka, A., Cikara, M. et al., (2020). Using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response. Nature Human Behaviour, 4(5), 460471. 10.1038/s41562-020-0884-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
van der Weerd, W., Timmermans, D. R., Beaujean, D. J., Oudhoff, J., van Steenbergen, J. E. (2011). Monitoring the level of government trust, risk perception and intention of the general public to adopt protective measures during the influenza A (H1N1) pandemic in the Netherlands. BMC Public Health, 11(1),575. 10.1186/1471-2458-11-575CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 10631070. 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Organization, World Health. (2020). Novel Coronavirus (2019-nCoV): situation report, 11. World Health Organization. Retrieved August 24, 2021, from https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/330776.Google Scholar
Xiao, E., Houser, D. (2005). Emotion expression in human punishment behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102(20),7398. 10.1073/pnas.0502399102CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Timeline of Covid-19 events in Hubei province, 2019–2020. Figure shows the dates of experiment data collection in context of wider events relating to the public emergency

Figure 1

Table 1 Experimental design and session information

Figure 2

Table 2 Key outcome measures in the experimental treatments

Figure 3

Table 3 Regression analysis of covariance

Figure 4

Fig. 2 Positive motivating factors during the Covid-19 outbreak. Figure presents the results of a survey question administered from March 12–20, 2020 to 5686 non-student individuals around China. The survey question asked respondents to consider the development of the epidemic and select five factors that had provided them with positive psychological motivation

Figure 5

Table 4 Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) outcomes

Supplementary material: File

Guo et al. supplementary material

Guo et al. supplementary material
Download Guo et al. supplementary material(File)
File 690.9 KB