Published online by Cambridge University Press: 27 January 2009
Citizens do not choose sides on issues like busing or abortion whimsically. They have reasons for their preferences – certainly they can give reasons for them. But how is this possible? Citizens as a rule pay little attention to politics, indeed take only a modest interest in it even during election campaigns when their interest in politics is at its height. And since they pay little attention to politics, it is hardly surprising that they know little about it. Many, in fact, are quite ignorant of basic facts of political life – such as the identity of the party that controls Congress or indeed the name of the congressman who represents them. Which, of course, raises a question of some interest: how do citizens figure out what they think about political issues, given how little they commonly know about them?
1 Elkins, David J., ‘The Nature of Belief Systems in Issue Publics’, paper presented at the annual meeting of the Canadian Political Science Association, Ottawa, Canada, 1982.Google Scholar
2 Campbell, Angus, Converse, Philip E., Miller, Warren E. and Stokes, Donald E., The American Voter (New York: Wiley, 1960).Google Scholar
3 Miller, Warren E. and Stokes, Donald E., ‘Constituency Influence in Congress’, American Political Science Review, LVII (1963), 45–57CrossRefGoogle Scholar; Erikson, Robert S., Luttbeg, Norman R. and Tedin, Kent L., American Public Opinion: Its Origins, Content and Impact (New York: Wiley, 1980).Google Scholar
4 Simon, Herbert, ‘A Behavioral Model of Rational Choice’, Quarterly Journal of Economics, LXIX (1955), 99–108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
5 Zaller, John, ‘Toward a Theory of the Survey Response’, paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Political Science Association, Washington, D.C., 1984.Google Scholar
6 For a thoughtful and nuanced layout of a hierarchical model of information processing, see Peffley, Mark A. and Hurwitz, Jon, American Journal of Political Science, XXIX (1985), 871–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
7 Converse, Philip E., ‘The Nature of Belief Systems in Mass Publics’, in Apter, David E., ed., Ideology and Discontent (Glencoe, Ill.: Free Press, 1964), pp. 206–61.Google Scholar
8 Weiner, Bernard, ‘A Cognitive (Attribution)–Emotion–Action Model of Motivated Behavior: An Analysis of Judgments of Help-giving’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, XXXIX (1980), 186–200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
9 The data were made available by the Inter-University Consortium for Political and Social Research. The Center for Political Studies of the Institute for Social Research at the University of Michigan collected the data for the 1972 National Election Study under a grant from the National Science Foundation. Neither the consortium nor the original collectors of the data bear any responsibility for the analyses reported here.
10 We rely upon data from 1972, rather than from a more recent National Election Study, because subsequent NES surveys have not included all of the items required for our analysis.
11 For each item used to assess preferences on racial policy, respondents who said that they were not interested in the issue or who said ‘don't know’ were rescored to the centre of the scale for that item.
12 Brady, Henry E., ‘Factor Analysis for Ordinal Data which are not Interpersonally Comparable’ (Working Paper, Department of Government, Harvard University, 1986).Google Scholar
13 Duncan, Otis Dudley, Notes on Social Measurement (New York: Russell Sage Foundation, 1984).Google Scholar
14 Brady, , ‘Factor Analysis for Ordinal Data which are not Interpersonally Comparable’.Google Scholar
15 The analysis produced a second component with an eigenvalue slightly greater than the conventional critical value of 1·0. The eigenvalue of the first component, however, was more than twice that of the second, while the eigenvalue of the second component was only slightly larger than that of the third. The eigenvalues decreased gradually from that point. Moreover, the second component extracted was substantively indistinct from the first. For both of these reasons, the second component was discarded and only the first was employed in our analysis. A principal factors solution yielded almost identical results.
16 For another treatment of these items, different in methodology and in focus, see Sniderman, Paul M. with Hagen, Michael Gray, Race and Inequality (Chatham, N.J.: Chatham House, 1985).Google Scholar
17 People who said ‘don't know’ in response to a question were placed at the centre of the scale for that item.
18 This analysis also produced a second component with an eigenvalue slightly greater than the conventional critical value of 1·0. The eigenvalue of the first component, however, was substantially larger than that of the second, while the eigenvalue of the second component was only slightly larger than those of the third and fourth. Again, the eigenvalues declined gradually from there. Only the first component was employed in our analysis. A principal factors solution yielded almost identical results.
19 Sears, David O., ‘The Person-Positivity Bias’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, XLIV (1982), 233–50.Google Scholar
20 People who responded ‘don't know’ or ‘haven't thought much about it’ were rescored to place them at the centre of the scale.
21 Brady, , ‘Factor Analysis for Ordinal Data which are not Interpersonally Comparable’.Google Scholar
22 Feldman, Stanley, ‘Economic Individualism and Mass Belief Systems’, paper prepared for delivery at the annual meeting of the Midwest Political Science Association, Cincinnati, Ohio, 1982Google Scholar; see also his ‘Economic Self-interest and Political Behavior’, American Journal of Political Science, XXVI (1982), 446–66.Google Scholar
23 People who said ‘don't know’ in response to a question were placed at the centre of the scale for that item. A principal factors solution yielded almost identical results.
24 SAS User's Guide, 1979 Edition (Raleigh, North Carolina: SAS Institute, 1979), p. 204.Google Scholar
25 These calculations are based upon information from Figure 1 and Table 4.
26 Note that our measures of racial policy preferences and of explanations for racial inequality are both computed from scores based on our principal components analyses. Thus the units of the two scales are similar and the unstandardized estimates of their effects on one another are comparable. Note as well that the two measures have standard deviations that are approximately equal.
27 Evidence of robustness can be found by comparing Figure 2 to the estimates for the well educated in Figure 3.
28 We should admit that we ourselves are surprised at this result for the well educated, for it is the well educated that we expected to find most prone to reason from general principle to specific policy preference. To discover why it is that even the well educated give so much weight to their racial policy preferences in arriving at their explanations for racial inequality is an important task for future research.
29 It is the lack of a connection between our instrument – ideology – and policy preferences among the poorly educated that is responsible for grossly inflating the standard error of the estimated effect of policy preferences on explanations for inequality.
30 Kinder, Donald R., ‘Diversity and Complexity in American Public Opinion’, in Finifter, Ada W., ed., Political Science: The State of the Discipline (Washington, D.C.: American Political Science Association, 1983), pp. 389–425Google Scholar; see also Levitin, Teresa E. and Miller, Warren E., ‘Ideological Interpretations of Presidential Elections’, American Political Science Review, LXXIII (1979), 751–71CrossRefGoogle Scholar; and Campbell, et al. , The American Voter.Google Scholar
31 Converse, , ‘The Nature of Belief Systems in Mass Publics’.Google Scholar
32 Converse, , ‘The Nature of Belief Systems in Mass Publics’.Google Scholar
33 For an extended discussion of possible limits on our ability to distinguish cognitive and affective explanations, see Tetlock, Philip E. and Levi, Ariel, ‘Attribution Bias: On the Inconclusiveness of the Cognition–Motivation Debate’, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, XIX (1982), 68–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
34 Converse, Philip E., ‘Public Opinion and Voting Behavior’, in Greenstein, Fred I. and Polsby, Nelson W., eds, Handbook of Political Science, Volume 4 (Reading, Mass.: Addison-Wesley, 1975), pp. 75–169Google Scholar; and Converse, , ‘The Nature of Belief Systems in Mass Publics’.Google Scholar
35 Converse, , ‘The Nature of Belief Systems in Mass Publics’Google Scholar; see also Achen, Christopher H., ‘Mass Political Attitudes and the Survey Response’, American Political Science Review, LXIX (1975), 1218–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
36 Feldman, Stanley, ‘The Reliability and Stability of Policy Positions: Evidence from a Five-Wave Panel Study’, paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Political Science Association, New Orleans, Louisiana, 1985.Google Scholar
37 Simon, Herbert A., ‘Human Nature in Politics: The Dialogue of Psychology with Political Science’, American Political Science Review, LXXIX (1985), 293–304, p. 301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
38 Zaller, , ‘Toward a Theory of The Survey Response’.Google Scholar
39 Harman, Gilbert, ‘Positive versus Negative Undermining in Belief Revision’, in Kornblith, Hilary, ed., Naturalizing Epistemology (Cambridge, Mass.: Bradford, 1985), pp. 231–48.Google Scholar
40 Brady, Henry E. and Sniderman, Paul M., ‘Attitude Attribution: A Group Basis for Political Reasoning’, American Political Science Review, LXXIX (1985), 1061–78CrossRefGoogle Scholar; Chubb, John E., Hagen, Michael G. and Sniderman, Paul M., ‘A Two-dimensional Theory of Ideology’ (Working Paper on Political Science, No, P-84–2, Hoover Institution, Stanford University, 1984).Google Scholar
41 Zajonc, R. B., ‘On the Primacy of Affect’, American Psychologist, XXXIX (1984), 117–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
42 For an analysis of some respects in which racial issues differ from many other issues in American politics, see Carmines, Edward G. and Stimson, James A., ‘The Two Faces of Issue Voting’, American Political Science Review, LXXIV (1980), 78–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar