Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-fbnjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T12:45:25.314Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Priming and adaptation in native speakers and second-language learners

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 February 2017

EDITH KAAN*
Affiliation:
University of Florida
EUNJIN CHUN
Affiliation:
University of Florida
*
Address for correspondence: Edith Kaan, University of Florida, Department of Linguistics, Box 115454, Gainesville, Fl 32611, USA[email protected]

Abstract

Native speakers show rapid adjustment of their processing strategies and preferences on the basis of the structures they have recently encountered. The present study investigated the nature of priming and adaptation in second-language (L2) speakers and, more specifically, whether similar mechanisms underlie L2 and native language adaptation. Native English speakers and Korean L2 learners of English completed a written priming study probing the use of double object and prepositional phrase datives. Both groups showed cumulative adaptation effects for both types of dative, which was stronger for the structure that was initially less frequent to them (prepositional phrase datives for the native English speakers, and double object datives for the L2 learners). This supports models of priming that incorporate frequency-based modulation of long-lasting activation of structures. L2 learners and native speakers use similar processing mechanisms; differences in adaptation can be accounted for by differences in the relative frequency of structures.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*The authors would like to thank Martha Hinrichs and Aleuna Lee for their assistance with data collection and data analysis, Giboom Kim for creating line drawings, and Nalanda Chakraborty for picture editing.

Supplementary material can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728916001231

References

Arai, M., van Gompel, R. P. G., & Scheepers, C. (2007). Priming ditransitive structures in comprehension. Cognitive Psychology, 54, 218250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arnon, I., & Ramscar, M. (2012). Granularity and the acquisition of grammatical gender: How order-of-acquisition affects what gets learned. Cognition, 122, 292305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory & Language, 68, 255278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernolet, S., & Hartsuiker, R. J. (2010). Does verb bias modulate syntactic priming? Cognition, 114, 455461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernolet, S., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Pickering, M. J. (2013). From language-specific to shared syntactic representations: The influence of second language proficiency on syntactic sharing in bilinguals. Cognition, 127, 287306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bock, K. (1986). Syntactic persistence in language production. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 355387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bock, K. (1989). Closed-class immanence in sentence production. Cognition, 31, 163186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bock, K., & Griffin, Z. M. (2000). The persistence of structural priming: Transient activation or implicit learning? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 177192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bock, K., & Loebell, H. (1990). Framing sentences. Cognition, 35, 139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bock, K., Loebell, H., & Morey, R. (1992). From conceptual roles to structural relations: Bridging the syntactic cleft. Psychological Review, 99, 150171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brennan, S. E., & Clark, H. H. (1996). Conceptual pacts and lexical choice in conversation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 14821493.Google ScholarPubMed
Brennan, S. E., & Hanna, J. E. (2009). Partner-specific adaptation in dialog. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1, 274291.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chang, F., Dell, G. S., & Bock, K. (2006). Becoming syntactic. Psychological Review, 113, 234272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cleland, A. A., & Pickering, M. J. (2003). The use of lexical and syntactic information in language production: Evidence from the priming of noun-phrase structure. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 214230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dell, G. S., & Chang, F. (2014). The P-chain: relating sentence production and its disorders to comprehension and acquisition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 369.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dussias, P. E., Marful, A., Gerfen, C., & Bajo, M. T. (2010). Usage frequencies of complement-taking verbs in Spanish and English: Data from Spanish monolinguals and Spanish-English bilinguals. Behavior Research Methods 42, 10041011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farmer, T. A., Fine, A. B., Yan, S., Cheimariou, S., & Jaeger, T. F. (2014). Error-driven learning of higher-level expectancies during reading. In Bello, P., Guarini, M., McShane, M. & Scassellati, B. (eds.), Proceedings of the 36th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 21812186). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Ferreira, V. S., & Bock, K. (2006). The functions of structural priming. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21, 10111029.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fine, A. B., & Jaeger, T. F. (2013). Evidence for implicit learning in syntactic comprehension. Cognitive Science, 37, 578591.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fine, A. B., & Jaeger, T. F. (2016). The role of verb repetition in cumulative structural priming in comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition.Google Scholar
Fine, A. B., Jaeger, T. F., Farmer, T. A., & Qian, T. (2013). Rapid expectation adaptation during syntactic comprehension. PLoS ONE, 8, 118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flett, S., Branigan, H. P., & Pickering, M. J. (2013). Are non-native structural preferences affected by native language preferences? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, FirstView, 1–10.Google Scholar
Foucart, A., Martin, C. D., Moreno, E. M., & Costa, A. (2014). Can bilinguals see it coming? Word anticipation in L2 sentence reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 4161–1469.Google ScholarPubMed
Gries, S. T. (2005). Syntactic priming: A corpus-based approach. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 34, 365399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gries, S. T., & Wulff, S. (2005). Do foreign language learners also have constructions? Evidence from priming, sorting, and corpora. Annual Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 3, 182200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hartsuiker, R. J., Bernolet, S., Schoonbaert, S., Speybroeck, S., & Vanderelst, D. (2008). Syntactic priming persists while the lexical boost decays: Evidence from written and spoken dialogue. Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 214238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hartsuiker, R. J., Kolk, H. H. J., & Huiskamp, P. (1999). Priming word order in sentence production. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, 52, 129147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hartsuiker, R. J., & Westenberg, C. (2000). Word order priming in written and spoken sentence production. Cognition, 75, B27B39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hawkins, R. (1987). Markedness and the acquisition of the English dative alternation by L2 speakers. Second language Research, 3, 2055.Google Scholar
Hinrichs, M. (2015). Learner preferences and performance: Korean learners of English and the ditransitive alternation. Unpublished BA thesis, University of Florida.Google Scholar
Hopp, H. (2013). Grammatical gender in adult L2 acquisition: Relations between lexical and syntactic variability. Second Language Research, 29, 3356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopp, H. (2015). Semantics and morphosyntax in predictive L2 sentence processing. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 53, 277306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jaeger, T. F., & Snider, N. E. (2008). Implicit learning and syntactic persistence: Surprisal and cumulativity, Proceedings of the 29th annual Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1061–1066). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Jaeger, T. F., & Snider, N. E. (2013). Alignment as a consequence of expectation adaptation: Syntactic priming is affected by the prime's prediction error given both prior and recent experience. Cognition, 127, 5783.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaan, E. (2014). Predictive sentence processing in L2 and L1: What is different? Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 4, 257282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaschak, M. P. (2007). Long-term structural priming affects subsequent patterns of language production. Memory & Cognition, 35, 925937.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaschak, M. P., & Borreggine, K. L. (2008). Is long-term structural priming affected by patterns of experience with individual verbs? Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 862878.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaschak, M. P., Kutta, T. J., & Jones, J. L. (2011). Structural priming as implicit learning: Cumulative priming effects and individual differences. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 11331139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaschak, M. P., Loney, R. A., & Borreggine, K. L. (2006). Recent experience affects the strength of structural priming. Cognition, 99, B73B82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, S. (2010). Syntactic representations of English in second language learners: An investigation of the process of English sentence production by bilingual speakers using a within-language (L2) structural priming paradigm. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, State University of New York at Buffalo, United States – New York.Google Scholar
Kim, Y., & McDonough, K. (2008). Learners' production of passives during syntactic priming activities. Applied Linguistics, 29, 149154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kraljic, T., Samuel, A. G., & Brennan, S. E. (2008). First impressions and last resorts: How listeners adjust to speaker variability. Psychological Science, 19, 332338.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ledoux, K., Traxler, M. J., & Swaab, T. Y. (2007). Syntactic priming in comprehension: Evidence from event-related potentials. Psychological Science, 18, 135143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, E.-K., Lu, D. H.-Y., & Garnsey, S. M. (2013). L1 word order and sensitivity to verb bias in L2 processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16, 761775.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matsumoto, M., & Yamashita, H. (2006). Structural priming within a foreign language. Paper presented at the 19th annual CUNY conference on Human Sentence Processing.Google Scholar
McDonough, K. (2006). Interaction and syntactic priming: English L2 speakers' production of dative constructions. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 28, 179207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Messenger, K., Branigan, H. P., & McLean, J. F. (2011). Evidence for (shared) abstract structure underlying children's short and full passives. Cognition, 121, 268274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Park, B.-J. (2007). Syntactic persistence within and across languages in English and Korean L1 and L2 speakers. University of Arizona.Google Scholar
Peter, M., Chang, F., Pine, J. M., Blything, R., & Rowland, C. F. (2015). When and how do children develop knowledge of verb argument structure? Evidence from verb bias effects in a structural priming task. Journal of Memory and Language, 81, 115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pickering, M. J., & Branigan, H. P. (1998). The representation of verbs: Evidence from syntactic priming in language production. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 633651.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Core Team, R. (2015). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.Google Scholar
Reitter, D., Keller, F., & Moore, J. D. (2011). A computational cognitive model of syntactic priming. Cognitive Science, 35, 587637.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rhee, S.-C., & Jung, C. K. (2014). Compilation of the Yonsei English Learner Corpus (YELC) 2011 and its use for understanding current usage of English by Korean pre-university Students. The Journal of the Korea Contents Association, 14, 10191029.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rowland, C. F., Chang, F., Ambridge, B., Pine, J. M., & Lieven, E. V. M. (2012). The development of abstract syntax: Evidence from structural priming and the lexical boost. Cognition, 125, 4963.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savage, C., Lieven, E. V. M., Theakston, A., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Structural priming as implicit learning in language acquisition: The persistence of lexical and structural priming in 4-year-olds. Language learning and development, 2, 27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scheepers, C. (2003). Syntactic priming of relative clause attachments: persistence of structural configuration in sentence production. Cognition, 89, 179205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schoonbaert, S., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Pickering, M. J. (2007). The representation of lexical and syntactic information in bilinguals: Evidence from syntactic priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 56, 153171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shin, J.-A., & Christianson, K. (2009). Syntactic processing in Korean-English bilingual production: Evidence from cross-linguistic structural priming. Cognition, 112, 175180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shin, J.-A., & Christianson, K. (2012). Structural priming and second language learning. Language Learning, 62, 931964.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2000). The item-based nature of children's early syntactic development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 156163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tooley, K. M., & Bock, K. (2014). On the parity of structural persistence in language production and comprehension. Cognition, 132, 101136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tooley, K. M., & Traxler, M. J. (2010). Syntactic Priming Effects in Comprehension: A Critical Review. Language and Linguistics Compass, 4, 925937.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Kaan and Chun supplementary material

Kaan and Chun supplementary material 1

Download Kaan and Chun supplementary material(File)
File 45.6 KB