Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T09:03:07.294Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The functionality of the study of language origin

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 February 2010

Antoni Gomila
Affiliation:
Philosophy Department, University of the Balearic Islands, 07071 Palma de Mallorca, Spain Electronic mall: [email protected]

Abstract

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Continuing Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1994

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bickerton, D. (1981) Roots of language. Karoma. [AG]Google Scholar
Hubel, D. & Wiesel, T. (1962) Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture. Journal of Physiology 160:106–54. [AG]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liberman, A. M. & Mattingly, I. G. (1985) The motor theory of speech perception revised. Cognition 21:136. [HMS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindblom, B. (1990) Adaptive complexity in sound patterns. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13(4):743–44. [HMS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matthews, R. (1989) Introduction. In: Learnability and linguistic theory, ed. Matthews, R. & Demopoulos, W.. Klnwer. [AG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S. & Bloom, P. (1990) Natural language and natural selection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13(4):707–84. [rSP, AG, HMS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ridley, M. (1990) Arbitrariness no argument against adaptation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13(4):756. [AG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sober, E. (1990) Anatomizing the rhinoceros. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13(4):764–65. [AG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sperber, D. (1990) The evolution of the language faculty: A paradox and its solution. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13(4):756–58. [AG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stabler, E. (1983) How are grammars represented? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6:391402. [AG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1990) This view of language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13(4):758–59. [HMS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suga, N., O'Neill, W. E., Kujirai, K. & Manabe, T. (1983) Specificity of combination-sensitive neurons for processing of complex biosonar signals in auditory cortex of the mustached bat. Neurophysiology 49:15731627. [HMS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sullivan, W. E. & Konishi, M. (1986) Neural map of interaural phase difference in the owl's brainstem. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, USA 83:84008404. [HMS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sussman, H. M. (1988) The neurogenesis of phonology. In: Phonological processes and brain mechanisms, ed. Whitaker, H.. Springer-Verlag. [HMS]Google Scholar
Sussman, H. M. (1989) Neural coding of relational invariance in speech: Human language analogs to the barn owl. Psychological Review 96:631–42. [HMS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, H., Takahashi, T. & Konishi, M. (1987) Representation of interaural time difference in the central nucleus of the barn owl's inferior colliculus. Journal of Neuroscience 7:3105–16. [HMS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilkins, W. & Dumford, J. (1990) In defense of exaptation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13(4):763–64. [AG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar