No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
The function and process of perception
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 04 February 2010
Abstract
An abstract is not available for this content so a preview has been provided. Please use the Get access link above for information on how to access this content.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1980
References
Attneave, F. (1972) Representation of physical space. In: Coding processes in human memory, ed. Melton, A. W. & Martin, E., Washington, D.C.: V.H.Winston & Sons. [SU]Google Scholar
Attneave, F & Frost, R. (1969) The determination of perceived tridimensional orientation by minimum criteria. Perception and Psychophysics 6(6B):391–96. [SU]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barlow, H. B. (1972) Single units and sensation: A neuron doctrine for perceptual psychology. Perception 1:371–94. [OJB]Google Scholar
Barrow, H. G. & Tenenbaum, J. M. (1978) Recovering intrinsic scene characteristics from images. In: Computer vision systems, ed. Hanson, A. R. & Riseman, E. M.. New York: Academic Press. [GEH]Google Scholar
Bentley, D., & Hoy, R. R. (1974) The neurobiology of cricket song. Scientific American 231:34–44, 08 [SU]Google Scholar
Bernstein, N. (1967) The co-ordination and regulation of movements. New York: Pergamon Press. [WMM]Google Scholar
Blaivas, A. S. (1975) Visual analysis: theory of lie group representations. Mathematical Biosciences 28:45–67. [JWG]Google Scholar
Block, B. (1950) Studies in colloquial Japanese IV: phonemics. Language 26:86–125. [SJK]Google Scholar
Braunstein, M. L. (1962) Depth perception in rotating dot patterns: effects of numerosity and perspective. Journal of Experimental Psychology 64(4):415–70. [SU]Google Scholar
Breitmeyer, B. G. & Ganz, L. (1976) Implications of sustained and transient channels for theories of visual pattern masking, saccadic suppression and information processing. Psychological Review 83:1–36. [GRL]Google Scholar
Bridgeman, B.; Hendry, D.; & Stark, L. (1975) Failure to detect displacement of the visual world during saccadic eye movements. Vision Research 15:719–22. [BB]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, J. F. (1931) The visual perception of velocity. Psychologische Forschung 14:199–232. [IR]Google Scholar
Caelli, T. M., & Julesz, B. (1978) On perceptual analyzers underlying visual texture discrimination: part I. Biological Cybernetics 28:167–75. [SU]Google Scholar
Campbell, F. W. & Robson, J. G. (1968) Application of Fourier analysis to the visibility of gratings. Journal of Physiology – London 197:551–66. [SU]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1957) Review of R. Jakobson and M. Halle's Fundamentals of Language. International Journal of American Linguistics 23:234–41. [SJK]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1959) A review of B. F. Skinner's Verbal Behavior. In: The structure of language, ed. Fodor, J. A. & Karz, J. J.. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall (1964). [SU]Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1964) Current issues in linguistic theory. In: The structure of language: Readings in the Philosophy of Language, ed. Fodor, J. A. and Katz, J.J.; Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall. [SJK]Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. & Halle, M. (1968) The sound pattern of English. New York: Harper and Row. [SJK]Google Scholar
Coren, S.; Ward, L.; Porac, C.; & Fraser, R. (1978) The effect of optical blur on visual-geometric illusions. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 11(6):390–92. [SWZ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dember, W. N. & Purcell, D. G. (1967) Recovery of masked visual targets by inhibition of the masking stimulus. Science 157:1335–36. [GRL]Google Scholar
Dodwell, P. C. (1970) Visual pattern recognition. New York: Holt, Rinhart & Winston. [KP]Google Scholar
Eden, M. (1962) A three-dimensional optical illusion. Quarterly Progress Report no. 64 M.I.T.R.L.E. 267–74. [SU]Google Scholar
Epstein, W. (1973) the process of “taking-into-account” in visual perception. Perception 2:267–85. [WE]Google Scholar
Epstein, W. (ed.) (1977a) Stability and constancy in visual perception: mechanisms and processes. New York: Wiley-Interscience. [WE]Google Scholar
Epstein, W. (ed.) (1977b) What are the prospects for a higher-order stimulus theory of perception? Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 18:164–71. [WE, SU]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epstein, W. (ed.) (1979) In the eye of the beholder. Psychological Studies 24:82–97. [WE]Google Scholar
Epstein, W. (ed.) (In prep.) The relationship between texture gradient and perceived slant-in-depth. [WE]Google Scholar
Epstein, W. & Hatfield, G. (1978) The locus of masking shape-at-a-slant. Perception and Psychophysics 24(6):501–4. [WE]Google Scholar
Epstein, W.; Hatfield, G.; & Muise, G. (1977) Perceived shape at a slant as a function of processing time and processing load. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 3:473–83. [WE]Google Scholar
Epstein, W. & Park, J. (1963) Shape constancy: functional relationships and theoretical formulations. Psychological Bulletin 60:265–88. [WE]Google Scholar
Epstein, W. & Park, J. (1964) Examination of Gibson's psychophysical hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin 62:180–96. [SU]Google Scholar
Eriksson, E. S. (1974) A theory of veridical space perception. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 15:225–35. [SU]Google Scholar
Evarts, E. V. (1971) Feedback as corollary discharge: a merging of the concepts. Neurosciences Research Program Bulletin 9(1):86–112. [JWG]Google Scholar
Farber, J. M. & McConkie, A. B. (1979) Optical motion as information for unssigned depth. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Psychophysics 5(3):494–500. [SU]Google Scholar
Fehrer, E. & Raab, D. (1962) Reaction time to stimuli masked by metacontrast. Journal of Experimental Psychology 63:143–47. [GRL]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibson, E. J. (1969) Perceptual learning and development. New York: Appleton-Gentury-Crofts. [IR]Google Scholar
Gibson, E. J. (1977) How perception really develops: a view from outside the network. In: Basic processess in reading, ed. LaBerge, D. & Samuels, S. J.. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum. [WE]Google Scholar
Gibson, E. J.; Gibson, J. J.; Smith, O. W.; & Flock, H. (1959) Motion parallax as a determinant of perceived depth. Journal of Experimental Psychology 8(1):40–51. [SU]Google Scholar
Gibson, E. J.; Owsley, C. J.; & Johnston, J. (1978) Perception of invariants by five-month-old infants: differentiation of two types of motion. Developmental Psychology 14(4):407–15. [SU]Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1950) The perception of the visual world. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. [JR. SU, KvF, CBZ]Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1954) The visual perception of objective motion and subjective movement. Psychological Review 61(5):304–14. [SU]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibson, J. J. (1957) Optical motions and transformations as stimuli for visual perception. Psychological Review 64(5):288–95. [SU]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibson, J. J. (1958) Visually controlled locomotion and visual orientation in animals. British Journal of Psychology 49:182–94. [WMM]Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1959) Perception as a function of stimulation. In: Psychology: a study of a science, vol. 1., ed. Koch, S., New York, Toronto, London: McGraw-Hill. [ESR, RS, SU]Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1960) The concept of the stimulus in psychology. American Psychologist 15:694–703. [SU]Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1963) The useful dimensions of sensitivity. American Psychologist 18:1–15. [ESR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1965) Research on the visual perception of motion and change. In: Spigel, , I. 125–46. [SU]Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1966) The senses considered as perceptual systems. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. [BB, WE, KvF, WMM, ESR, IR, SU]Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1968) What gives rise to the perception of motion? Psychological Review 75(4):335–46. [ESR, SU]Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1968–1969) Are there sensory qualities of objects? Synthese 19:408–9. [SU]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1970) On theories for visual space perception: a reply to Johansson. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 11:73–79. [ESR]Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1972) A theory of direct visual perception. In: The psychology of knowing, ed. Royce, J. R. & Rozeboom, W. W.. New York, Paris, London: Gordon & Breach. [GRL, SU]Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1976) The myth of passive perception: a reply to Richards. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 37:234–38. [RJK, ESR]Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1979) The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. [SG, RKJ, WMM, AKM, SSP, ESR, IR, RS, SU, CBZ]Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. & Gibson, E. J. (1957) Continuous perspective tranformations and the perception of rigid motion. Journal of Experimental Psychology 54(2):129–38. [SU]Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J.; Kaplan, G.; Reynolds, H. N.; & Wheeler, K. (1969) The change from visible to invisible: a study of optical transitions. Perception and Psychophysics 5:113–16. [WMM]Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J.; Olum, P.; & Rosenblatt, F. (1955) Parallax and perspective during aircraft landings. American Journal of Psychology 68:372–85. [WMM]Google Scholar
Gilchrist, A. (1977) Perceived lightness depends on perceived spatial arrangement. Science 195:185–87. [SU]Google Scholar
Ginzburg, A. (1971) Psychological correlates of a model of the human visual system. Proceedings of the National Electronics Conference, IEEE:283–90. [SWZ]Google Scholar
Gregory, R. (1974) Choosing a paradigm for perception. In: Handbook of perception. Vol. 1, Historical and philosophical roots of perception, ed. Carterette, E. C. and Friedman, M. P.. New York: Academic Press. [WE]Google Scholar
Gregory, R. (1979) Perceptual hypotheses. In: The psychology of vision, ed. Longuet Higgins, H. C. & Sutherland, N. S.Royal Society of London symposium on vision (in press). [KP]Google Scholar
Griffin, D. R. (1978) Prospects for a cognitive ethology. Brain and Behavioral Sciences 1(4):527–38. [SU]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. (1976a) Adaptive pattern classification and universal recoding, I: parallel development and coding of neural feature detectors. Biological Cybernetics 23:121–34. [SG]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. (1976b) Adaptive pattern classification and universal recoding, II: feedback, expectation, olfaction, and illusions. Biological Cybernetics 23:187–202. [SG]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. (1978) A theory of human memory: self-organization and perfomance of sensory-motor codes, maps, and plans. In: Progress in theoretical biology, vol. 5, ed. Rosen, R. & Snell, F.. New York: Academic Press. [SG]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. (1980) How does a brain build a cognitive code? Psychological Review 87:1–51. [SG]Google Scholar
Gyr, J. W. (1972a) Is a theory of direct visual perception adequate? Psychological Bulletin 77:246–61. [SU]Google Scholar
Gyr, J. W. (1972b) Comments on Gibson's paper. In: The psychology of knowing, ed. Royce, J. R. & Rozeboom, W. W.. New York, Paris, London: Gordon & Breach. [SU]Google Scholar
Gyr, J.; Willey, R.; & Henry, A. (1979) Motor-sensory feedback and geometry of visual space: a replication. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2:59–64. [JWG, SU]Google Scholar
Hay, C. J. (1966) Optical motions and space perception – and extension of Gibson's analysis. Psychological Review 73:550–65. [SU]Google Scholar
Hayek, F. A. (1969) The primary of the abstract. In: Beyond reductionism, ed. Koestler, A. & Smythies, J. R., pp. 309–33. New York: Macmillan. [WBW]Google Scholar
Hayes-Roth, F. (1977) Critique of Turvey's “Contrasting orientations to the theory of visual information processing.” Psychological Review 531–35. [FHR]Google Scholar
Hecht, S. (1934) Vision II. the nature of the photoreceptor process. In: A handbook of general experimental psychology, ed. Murchinson, C.. Worcester: Clark University Press. [GRL]Google Scholar
Helmholtz, H. von, (1963) Treatise on physiological optics, ed. South, J. P. C.-all. New York: Dover. [BB, WE, SWZ]Google Scholar
Henle, M. (1974) On naive realism. In: Perception: essays in honor of James J. Gibson, ed. Macleod, R. B. & Pick, H. L. Jr. Ithaca and London: Cornell University Press. [SU]Google Scholar
Hinton, G. (1979) Some demonstration of the effects of structural descriptions in mental imagery. Cognitive Science 3:231–50. [SU]Google Scholar
Hochberg, J. (1974) Higher-order stimuli and inter-response coupling in the perception of the visual world. In: Perception: essays in honor of James J. Gibson, ed. Macleod, R. B. & Pick, H.L. Jr. Ithaca and London: Cornell University Press. [SU]Google Scholar
Hochberg, J. & McAlister, A. (1953) A quantitative approach to figural “goodness.” Journal of Experimental Psychology 46:361–64. [SU]Google Scholar
Hoffman, W. C. (1966) The lie-algebra of visual perception. Journal of Mathematical Psychology 3:65–98. [J WG]Google Scholar
Hoffman, W. C. (1977) An informal, historical description (with bibliography) of the “L.T.G./N.P.” Cahiers de Psychologie 20:135–74. [KvF]Google Scholar
Jansson, G. & Johansson, G. (1973) Visual perception of bending motion. Perception 2:321–26. [SU]Google Scholar
Johannson, G. (1964) Perception of motion and changing form. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 5:181–208. [SU, KvF]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johannson, G. (1970) On theories for visual space perception – a letter to Gibson. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 11:67–74. [SU, KvF]Google Scholar
Johansson, G.; Von Hofsten, C.; & Jansson, G. (1980) Event perception. Annual Review of Psychology 31:27–63. [GJ]Google Scholar
Julesz, B. (1971) Foundations of Cyclopean perception. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [SU]Google Scholar
Julesz, B. & Caelli, T. (1979) On the limits of fourier decomposition in visual texture perception. Perception 8:69–73. [SU]Google Scholar
Julesz, B. & Miller, J. (1975) Independent spatial-frequency-tuned channels in binocular fusion and rivalry. Perception 4:125–43. [SU]Google Scholar
Kahneman, D. (1967) An onset-onset law for one case of apparent motion and metacontrast. Perception and Psychophysics 2:577–84. [GRL]Google Scholar
Kaila, Eino (1979) Reality and experience. In: Four philosophical essays ed., Cohen, R. S.. Dordrecht, Boston, London: Reidel. [KvF]Google Scholar
Kalil, R. E. & Freedman, S. J. (1966) Persistence of ocular rotation following compensation for displaced vision. Perceptual & Motor Skills 22:135–39. [IR]Google Scholar
Kanizsa, G. (1955) Margini quasi-percettivi in campi con stimolazione omogenea. Rivista di Psicologia 49:7–30. [IR]Google Scholar
Kant, Immanuel (1781) Critique of Pure Reason, trans. Smith, N. K. (1929). London: Macmillan. [AS]Google Scholar
Kantor, J. R. (1920) Suggestions toward a scientific interpretation of perception. Psychological Review 27:191–216. [SSR]Google Scholar
Kaplan, G. A. (1969) Kinetic disruption of optical texture: the perception of depth at an edge. Perception & Psychophysics 6:193–98. [IR]Google Scholar
Kelly, D. H. (1979a) Motion and vision. 1. Stabilized images of stationary gratings. Journal of the Opticals sociaty America 69(9):1266–74. [KP]Google Scholar
Kelly, D. H. (1979b) Motion and vision. 2. Stabilized spatio-temporal threshold surface. Journal of the Optical Society of America 69(10):1340–49. [KP]Google Scholar
Kling, J. W. & Riggs, L. A. (1972) Experimental Psychology. New York: Holt, inehart & Winston. [GRL]Google Scholar
Koenderink, J. J. & van Doom, A. J. (1975) Invariant properties of the motion parallax field due to the movement of rigid bodies relative to an observer. Optica Acta 22(9):773–91. [KP]Google Scholar
Koenderink, J. J. & van Doom, A. J. (1976) Local structure of movement parallax of the plane. Journal of the Optical Society of America 66(7):717–23. [KP]Google Scholar
Koenderink, J. J. & van Doom, A. J. (1977) How an ambulant observer can construct a model of the environment from the geometrical structure of the visual inflow. Kybernetik 77:224–47. [KP]Google Scholar
Koenderink, J. J. & van Doom, A. J. (1979) The internal representation of solid shape with respect to vision. Biological Cybernetics. 32:211–16. [KP]Google Scholar
Koffka, K. (1935) Principles of Gestalt psychology. New York: Hareourt, Brace, and World. [IR, SU]Google Scholar
Kosslyn, S. M. (in press) Image and Mind. Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press. [SU]Google Scholar
Kugler, P. N.; Turvey, M. T.; & Shaw, R. (forthcoming) Is the “cognitive penetrability” criterion invalidated by contemporary physics? Behavioral and Brain Sciences. [SSP]Google Scholar
Kuhn, T. (1962) The structure of scientific revolutions, Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [SWZ]Google Scholar
Lee, D. N. (1974) Visual information during locomotion. In: Perception: essays in honor of James J. Gibson, ed. MacLeod, R. B. & Pick, H. L.. Ithaca and London: Cornell University Press. [RS]Google Scholar
Lee, D. N. (1976) A theory of visual control of braking based on information about time-to-collision. Perception 5:437–56. [SU]Google Scholar
Lee, D. N. (1980) Visuo-motor coordination in space-time. In: Tutorials and motor behavior, ed. Stelmach, G. & Requin, J.. Amsterdam: North-Holland Publishing. [RJK, SSP]Google Scholar
Lee, D. N. (in press) The optic flow field: The foundation of vision. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. [RKJ]Google Scholar
Liberman, A. M.; Cooper, F. S.; Shankweiler, D. P.; & Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1967) Perception of the speech code. Psychological Review 74:431–61. [SJK]Google Scholar
Lindsay, P. H. & Norman, D. A. (1972) Human information processing. New York and London: Academic Press. [SU]Google Scholar
Longuet-Higgins, H. C. & Prazdny, K. (in press) The interpretation of a moving retinal image. Proceddings of the Royal Society – London. [SU]Google Scholar
Mace, W. M. (1977) James Gibson's strategy for perceiving: ask not what's in your head, but what your head is inside of. In: Perceiving, acting and knowing: towards an ecological psychology, ed. Shaw, R. & Bransford, J.. Hillsdale, N. J.: Erlbaum. [ESR]Google Scholar
Mack, A. (1970) An investigation of the relationship between eye and retinal image movement in the perception of movement. Perception and Psychophysics 8:291–98. [BB]Google Scholar
Mack, A. (1979) Nonvisual determinants of perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2(1):75. [SU]Google Scholar
Mackworth, A. K. (1976) Model-driven interpretation in intelligent vision systems. Perception 5:349–70. [AKM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marcel, A. (in press) Conscious and unconscious perception: visual masking, word recognition and an approach to consciousness. Cognitive Psychology. [GRL]Google Scholar
Marmolin, H. (1973) Visually perceived motion in depth resulting from proximal changes. Perception and Psychophysics 14(1):133–48. [SU]Google Scholar
Marr, D. (1976) Early processing of visual information. Philosphical Transactions of the Royal Society of London 275(942)483–534. [SU, SWZ]Google ScholarPubMed
Marr, D. (1977) Artificial intelligence – a personal view. Artificial Intelligence 9:37–48. [SU]Google Scholar
Marr, D. & Nishihara, K. (1978) Representation and recognition of the spatial organization of three-dimensional shapes. Proceedings of the Royal Society – London B 200:269–94. [SU]Google Scholar
Marr, D.; Palm, G.; & Poggio, T. (1978) Analysis of a cooperative stereo algorithm. Biological Cybernetics 28:223–39. [JFD]Google Scholar
Marr, D. & Poggio, T. (1976) Cooperative computation of stereo disparity. Science 194:283–87. [JFD, GEH]Google Scholar
Marr, D. & Poggio, T. (1977) From understanding computation to understanding neural circuitry. Neuroscience Research Program Bulletin 15(3):470–88. [SU]Google Scholar
Marr, D. & Poggio, T. (1979) A computational theory of human stereo vision. Proceedings of the Royal Society – London B 204:301–28. [SU]Google Scholar
Masterton, R. B. & Berkley, M. A. (1974) Brain function: changing ideas on the rolè of sensory, motor and association cortex in behavior. Annual Review of Psychology 25:277–312. [ESR]Google Scholar
Metzger, W. (1930) Optische untersuchungen im Ganzfeld II. Psychologische Forschung. 13:6–29. [RKJ]Google Scholar
Metzger, W. (1972) Critical remarks to J. J. Gibson's conception of “direct” visual perception, i.e., of revived prephysiological realism. In: The psychology of knowing, ed. Royce, J. R. & Rozeboom, W. W., New York, Paris, London: Gordon & Breach. [SU]Google Scholar
Meyer, D. E. & Schvaneveldt, R. W. (1971) Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations. Journal of Experimental Psychology 90:227–34. [GRL]Google Scholar
Michaels, C. F. & Carello, C. (in press) The theory of direct perception. New York: Prentice-Hall. [SSP]Google Scholar
Minsky, M. (1967) Computation: finite and infinite machines. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall. [RS]Google Scholar
Minsky, M. (1968) Matter, mind and models. In: Semantic information processing. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press. [AS]Google Scholar
Minsky, M. (1975) A framework for representing knowledge. In: The psychology of computer vision, ed. Winston, P. H.. New York: McGraw-Hill. [WE, KP]Google Scholar
Musatti, C. L. (1924) Sui fenomeni stereocinetici. Archivio Italiano Psicologia 3:105–20. [IR, CBZ]Google Scholar
Neff, W. S. (1936) A critical investigation of the visual apprehension of movement. American Journal of Psychology 48:1–42. [SU]Google Scholar
Neisser, U. (1976) Cognition and reality. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman and Company. [SG, SU, SWZ]Google Scholar
Oatley, K. (1978) Computational metaphors for perception. In: Perception (Part 2). Course in cognitive psychology. Milton Keynes: Open University. [KP]Google Scholar
Pattee, H. H. (1971) Physical theories of biological co-ordination. Quarterly Reviews of Biophysics 4:255–76. [WMM]Google Scholar
Pattee, H. H. (1974) Discrete and continuous processes in computers and brains. In: Lecture notes in bio-mathematics 4: physics and mathematics of the nervous system, ed. Conrad, M., Güttinger, W., & Cin, M. Dal. New York: Springer-Verlag. [WMM]Google Scholar
Pattee, H. H. (1977) Dynamic and linguistic modes of complex systems. International Journal of General Systems 8:259–66. [SSP]Google Scholar
Patten, B. C. (1979) Environs: relativistic elementary particles for ecology. Paper presented at Oak Ridge National Laboratory, Tenn. [SSP, RS]Google Scholar
Pittenger, J. B.; Shaw, R. E.; & Mark, L. S. (1979) Perceptual information for the age of faces as higher order invariances of growth. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Performance and Psychophysics 5(3):478–93. [SSP, SU]Google Scholar
Polyak, S. L. (1957) The vertebrate visual system. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [SU]Google Scholar
Prazdny, K. (1980) The information in optical flow. Artificial Intelligence Journal (submitted for publication). [KP]Google Scholar
Pylyshyn, Z. (1976) Imagery and artificial intelligence. In: Minnesota studies in the philosophy of science, vol. 9, ed. Savage, W.. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. [SU]Google Scholar
Pylyshyn, Z. (1978) Computational models and empirical constraints. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1(1):93–99. [SU]Google Scholar
Pylyshyn, Z. (1980) Computation and cognition: issues in the foundations of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3:111–69. [GEH, SSP]Google Scholar
Resile, F. (1979) Coding theory of the perception of motion configurations. Psychological Review 86(1):l–24. [SU]Google Scholar
Rock, I. (1977) In defense of unconscious inference. In: Stability and constancy in visual perception, ed. Epstein, W., pp. 321–73. New York: Wiley. [WE, GJ]Google Scholar
Rock, I. (in press) Alternative solutions to kinetic stimulus transformations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. [SU]Google Scholar
Rock, I. & Ebenholtz, S. (1959) The relational determination of perceived size. Psychological Review 66:387–401. [IR]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rock, I. & Gilchrist A. (1975) The conditions for the perception of the covering and uncovering of a line. American Journal of Psychology 88:571–82. [IR]Google Scholar
Rock, I.; Hill, A. L.; & Fineman, M. (1968) Speed constancy as a function of size constancy. Perception Psychophysics 4:37–40. [IR]Google Scholar
Rock, I.; Shallo, J.; & Schwartz, F. (1978) Pictorial depth and related constancy effects as a function of recognition. Perception 7:3–19. [CBZ]Google Scholar
Rock, I. & Sigman, E. (1973) Intelligence factors in the perception of form through a moving slit. Perception 2:357–69. [WE]Google Scholar
Runeson, S. (1977) On the possibility of “smart” perceptual mechanisms. Scan dinavian Journal of Psychology 18:172–79. [ESR, SR]Google Scholar
Schalz, C. (1954) The role of context in the perception of stops. Language 30:47–56. [SJK]Google Scholar
Sehiller, P. H. & Chorover, S. L. (1966) Metacontrasl: its relation to evoked potentials. Science 153:1398–1401. [GRL]Google Scholar
Schrödinger, E. (1967) Mind and mailer In: What is life? & mind and matter. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [SG, SU, WBW]Google Scholar
Shaw, R. & Bransford, J., eds. (1977a) Perceiving, acting, and knowing. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum. [KP]Google Scholar
Shaw, R. & Bransford, J., eds. (1977b) Psychological approaches to the problem of knowledge. In: Perceiving, acting, and knowing, ed. Shaw, R. & Bransford, J.. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum. [WMM, RS]Google Scholar
Shaw, R. & McIntyre, M. (1974) Algoristic foundations for cognitive psychology. In: Cognition and the symbolic processes, ed. Palermo, D. & Weimer, W.. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum. [SR]Google Scholar
Shaw, R.; Mclntyre, M.; & Mace, W. (1974) The role of symmetry in event perception. In: Perception: essays in honor of James J. Gibson, ed. Macleod, R. B. & Pick, H. L. Jr. Ithaca and London: Cornell University Press. [SU]Google Scholar
Shaw, R. & Turvey, M. (in press) Coalitions as models for ecosystems: a realist perspective on perceptual organization. In: Perceptual organization, ed. Kubovy, M. and Pomerantz, J., pp. 1–39. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum. [SSR, RS]Google Scholar
Shaw, R.; Turvey, M.; & Mace, W. (in press) Ecological psychology: the consequences of a commitment to realism. In Cognition and symbolic processes, vol. 2, ed. Weimer, W. & Palermo, D.. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum. [WMM, SSP, RS]Google Scholar
Shepard, R. N. (1975) Form, formation, and transformation of internal representations. In: Information processing and cognition: the Loyola symposium, ed. Solo, R.. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum. [SU]Google Scholar
Shepard, R. N. & Metzler, J. (1971) Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science 171:701–3. [SU]Google Scholar
Sigman, E. & Rock, I. (1974) Stroboscopic motion based on perceptual intelligence. Perception 3:9–28. [WE]Google Scholar
Simon, H. A. (1969) The sciences of the artificial. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. [GEH]Google Scholar
Sloman, A. (1978) The computer revolution in philosophy: philosophy science and models of mind. Sussex: Harvester Press and Humanities Press. [AS]Google Scholar
Sloman, A. & Owen, D. (1980) Why visual systems process sketches. In: Proceedings AISB Conference, ed. Hardy, S.. Amsterdam. [AS]Google Scholar
Smith, N. W. (1971) Aristotle's dynamic approach to sensing and some current implications. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences 7:375–77. [SSP]Google Scholar
Smith, N. W. (1974) The ancient background to Greek psychology and some implications for today. Psychological Record 24:309–24. [SSP]Google Scholar
Snellen, J. W. (1973) Set point and exercise. In: Essays on temperature regulation, ed. Bligh, J. & Moore, R. E.. Amsterdam: North-Holland Publishing. [SSP]Google Scholar
Sutherland, N. S. (1979) The representation of three-dimensional objects. Nature 278:395–98. [SU]Google Scholar
Taylor, J. G. (1962) The behavioral basis of perception. New Haven: Yale University Press. [SU]Google Scholar
Thatcher, R. W. & John, E. R. (1977) Foundations of cognitive processes. Hillsdale. N.J.: Erlbaum. [SU]Google Scholar
Tondeur, P. (1965) Introduction to the lie groups and transformation groups. Heidelberg: Springer. [KvF]Google Scholar
Turvey, M. T. (1976) On peripheral and central processes in vision: inferences from an information-processing analysis of masking with patterned stimuli. Psychological Review 80:1–52. [GRL]Google Scholar
Turvey, M. T. (1977) Contrasting orientations to the theory of visual information processing. Psychological Review 84(1):67–88. [WE, FHR, SU]Google Scholar
Turvey, M. T. (1979) The thesis of efference-mediation of vision cannot be rationalized. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2(1):81–83. [SU]Google Scholar
Turvey, M. T. & Shaw, R. (1979) The primacy of perceiving: An ecological reformulation for understanding memory. In: Perspectives on memory research: essays in honor of Uppsala University's 500th anniversary, ed. Nillson, L. G.. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum. [SSP, RS]Google Scholar
Turvey, M.; Shaw, R.; & Mace, W. (1978) Issues in the theory of action: degrees of freedom, coordinative structures and coalitions. In: Attention and performance VII, ed. Requin, J.. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum. [WMM, SSP, RS]Google Scholar
Ullman, S. (1978a) Artificial intelligence systems and human cognition: the missing link. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1(1):117–19. [SU]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ullman, S. (1978b) Mental representations and mental experiences. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1:605–6. [SU]Google Scholar
Ullman, S. (1979a) The interpretation of structure from motion. Proceedings of the Royal Society – London B 203:405–26. [SU]Google ScholarPubMed
Ullman, S. (1979b) The interpretation of visual motion. Cambridge and London: MIT Press. [RS. SU]Google Scholar
Ullman, S. (1979c) Relaxation and constrained optimization by local processes. Computer graphics and image processing 9(6):115–25. [SU]Google Scholar
von Fieandt, K. (1975) Experimentell analys av stereoeffekter med skuggfigurer i plan. Nordisk Psykologi 27. [KvF]Google Scholar
von Fieandt, K. & Gibson, J. J. (1959) The sensitivity of the eye to two kinds of continuous transformations of a shadow-pattern. Journal of Experimental Psychology 57:344–47. [KvF, SU]Google Scholar
von Fieandt, K. & Moustgaard, I. K. (1977) The perceptual world. London, New York, & San Francisco: Academic Press. [KvF]Google Scholar
Wall, P. D. (1970) The sensory and motor role of impulses travelling in the dorsal columns towards cerebral cortex. Brain 93:505–24. [ESR]Google Scholar
Wallach, H. (1948) Brightness, constancy and the nature of achromatic colors. Journal of Experimental Psychology 38:210–24. [IR]Google Scholar
Wallach, H. (in press) The perception of a stable environment. Scientific American. [CBZ]Google Scholar
Wallach, H. & O'Connell, D. N. (1953) The kinetic depth effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology 45(4):205–17. [SU]Google Scholar
Wallach, H.; O'Connell, D. N.; & Neisser, U. (1953) The memory effect of visual perception of three-dimensional form. Journal of Experimental Psychology 45:360–68. [IR]Google Scholar
Watson, B. A. & Nachmias, J. (1977) Patterns of temporal interaction in the detection of gratings. Vision Research 17:893–902. [SU]Google Scholar
Werner, J. (1977) Mathematical treatment of structure and function of the human thermoregulatory system. Biological Cybernetics 25:93–101. [SSP]Google Scholar
Wheatstone, C. (1838) Contributions to the physiology of vision. Part I. On some remarkable, and hitherto unobserved, phenomena of binocular vision. Royal Society of London Philosophical Transactions 128:371–94. [CBZ]Google Scholar
Wilson, H. R. (1978) Quantitative characterization of two types of line spread function near the fovea. Vision Research 18:493–96. [SU]Google Scholar
Wilson, H. R. & Bergen, J. R. (1979) A four mechanism model for spatial vision. Vision Research 19:19–32. [SU]Google Scholar
Wilkin, H. A. & Asch, S. E. (1948) Studies in space orientation, III. Perception of the upright in the absence of a visual field. Journal of Experimental Psychology 38:603–14. [IR]Google Scholar
Woodbridge, F. J. E. (1909) Consciousness, the sense organs, and the nervous system. Journal of Philosophy, Psychology, and Scientific Method 7:449–55. [SSP]Google Scholar
Yates, F. E. (1979) Physical biology: a basis for modeling living systems. Journal of Cybernetics and Information Science. [SSP]Google Scholar
Yates, F. E. (1980) Systems analysis of hormone action: principles and strategies. In: Biological regulation and development. Vol. 8: Hormone action, ed. Goldberger, R. F.. New York: Plenum. [SSP]Google Scholar
Yates, F. E.; Marsh, D. J.; & Iberall, A. S. (1973) Integration of the whole organism – a foundation for a theoretical biology. In: Challenging biological problems: directions toward their solution, ed. Behnke, J. A.. New York: Oxford University Press. [SSP]Google Scholar
Zaretsky, M. D. (1971) Patterned response to song in cricket central auditory neurone. Nature 229:195–96. [SU]Google Scholar
Zavelishin, N. V. & Tenenbaum, L. A. (1968) Control processes in the respiratory system. Automatikai Telemekhanika 9:106–22. [SSP]Google Scholar
Zucker, S. (1980) Motion and the Mueller-Lyer illusion, Technical Report 80–2R, Department of Electrical Engineering, McGill University, Montreal. [SWZ]Google Scholar