No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Discarding locality assumptions: Problems and prospects
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 04 February 2010
Abstract
An abstract is not available for this content so a preview has been provided. Please use the Get access link above for information on how to access this content.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1994
References
Allport, D. A. (1985) Distributed memory, modular subsystems, and dysphasia. In: Current perspectives in dysphasia, ed. Newman, S. K. & Epstein, R.. Churchill Livingstone. [aMJF]Google Scholar
Allport, D. A. (1989) Visual attention. In: Foundations of cognitive science, ed. Posner, M. I.. MIT Press. [aMJF]Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R. (1978) Arguments concerning representation for mental imagery. Psychological Review 85:249–77. [aMJF]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ansell, B. & Flower, C. (1982) Aphasic adults' use of heuristic and structural linguistic cues for analysis. Brain and Language 16:62–72. [EZ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Auerbach, S. H. (1986) Neuroanatomical correlates of attention and memory disorders in traumatic brain injury: An application of neurobehavioral subtypes. Journal of Head Trauma and Rehabilitation 1:1–12. [SLS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Basso, A., Capitani, E. & Laiacona, M. (1988) Progressive language impairment without dementia: A case with isolated category specific semantic defect. Journal of Neurology, Ncurosurgery and Psychiatry 51:1201–7. [aMJF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bauer, R. M. (1984) Autonomic recognition of names and faces in prosopagnosia: A neurophysiological application of the Guilty Knowledge Test. Neuropsychologia 22:457–69. [JDa]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Behrmann, M. & Bub, D. (1992) Surface dyslexia and dysgraphia: Dual routes, a single lexicon. Cognitive Neuropsychology 9(3):209–58. [DCP]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benson, D. F. & Geschwind, N. (1989) The aphasias and related disturbances. In: Clinical neurology, ed. Joynt, R.. J. B. Lippincott. [SLS]Google Scholar
Bever, T. G., Fodor, J. A. & Carrett, M. (1968) A formal limitation of associationism. In: Verbal behavior and general behavior theory, ed. Dixon, T. R. & Morton, D. L.. Prentice-Hall. [JMC]Google Scholar
Biederman, I. (1990) Higher-level vision. In: Visual cognition and action, ed. Osherson, D. N., Kosslyn, S. M. & Hollerbach, J. M.. MIT Press. [aMJF]Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M. (1992) The underlying nature of specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 33:3–66. [RC]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boring, E. G. (1942) Sensation and perception in the history of experimental psychology. Appleton-Century-Crofts. [RS]Google Scholar
Broca, P. P. (1861) Nouvelle observation d'aphémie produite par une lésion de la partie postérieure des douxième et troisième circonvolutioiis frontales. Bulletin Société Anatomié (Paris) 6:398–407. [SLS]Google Scholar
Bruce, V., Burton, A. M. & Craw, I. (1992) Modelling face recognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society 335:121–28. [AMB]Google ScholarPubMed
Bruce, V. & Young, A. W. (1986) Understanding face recognition. British Journal of Psychology 77:305–27. [AMB, JDa, RAM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bullinaria, J. A. (1993) Neural network models of reading without Wickelfeatures. (Submitted.) [JAB]Google Scholar
Bullinaria, J. A. & Chater, N. (1993) Double dissociation in artificial neural networks: Implications for neuropsychology. Proceedings of the Fifteenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society.Erlbaum. [JAB, NC]Google Scholar
Burton, A. M., Bruce, V. & Johnston, R. A. (1990) Understanding face recognition with an interactive activation model. British Journal of Psychology 81:361–80. [AMB]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burton, A. M., Young, A. W., Bruce, V., Johnston, R. A. & Ellis, A. W. (1991) Understanding covert recognition. Cognition 39:129–66. [rMJF, AMB, RC, AWY]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, J. I. D. (1992) In defense of the encoding-complex approach: Reply to McCloskey, Macaruso & Whetstone. In: The nature and origins of mathematical skills, ed. Campbell, J. I. D.. Elsevier. [JIDC]Google Scholar
Campbell, J. I. D. (1993) The architecture of numerical cognition: Modular or integrated mechanisms? International Journal of Psychology (in press). [JIDC]Google Scholar
Campbell, J. I. D. & Clark, J. M. (1988) An encoding-complex view of cognitive number processing: Comment on McCloskey, Sokol & Goodman (1986). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Central 117:204–14. [JIDC, JMC]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, J. I. D. & Clark, J. M. (1992) Cognitive number processing: An encoding-complex perspective. In: The Nature and origin of mathematical skills, ed. Campbell, J. I. D.. Elsevier. [JIDC, JMC]Google Scholar
Campbell, J. I. D. & Oliphant, M. (1992) Representation and retrieval of arithmetic facts: A network-interference model and simulation. In: The nature and origin of mathematical skills, ed. Campbell, J. I. D.. Elsevier. [JMC]Google Scholar
Campbell, R. (1992) Speech in the head? Rhyme skill, reading and immediate memory in the deaf. In: Auditory imagery, ed. Reisberg, D.. Erlbaum. [RC]Google Scholar
Caplan, D. (1981) On the cerebral localization of linguistic functions: Logical and empirical issues surrounding deficit analysis and functional localization. Brain and Language 14:120–37. [aMJF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caplan, D. & Futter, C. (1986) Assignment of thematic roles by an agrammatic aphasic patient. Brain and Language 27:117–35. [EZ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caramazza, A. (1984) The logic of neuropsychological research and the problem of patient classification in aphasia. Brain and Language 21:9–20. [aMJF, CS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caramazza, A. (1986) On drawing inferences about the structure of normal cognitive systems from the analysis of patterns of impaired performance: The case for single-patient studies. Brain and Cognition 5:41–66. [aMJF, BB, DCP]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caramazza, A. (1992) Is cognitive neuropsychology possible? Journal of Cognitive Neuroscicncc 4:80–95. [aMJF, DCP, CS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caramazza, A. & Hillis, A. E. (1990) Spatial representation of words in the brain implied by studies of a unilateral neglect patient. Nature 346:267–69. [CU]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caramazza, A., Hillis, A. E., Rapp, B. C. & Romani, C. (1990) The multiple semantics hypothesis: Multiple confusions? Cognitive Neuropsychology 7:161–90. [aMJF, RAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caramazza, A. & Zurif, E. B. (1976) Dissociation of algorithmic and heuristicprocesses in language comprehension: Evidence from aphasia. Brain and Language 3:572–82. [EZ]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charcot, J. M. (1883) Un cas de suppression brusque et isolée de la vision mentale des signes et objets (formes et couleurs). Progres Medical 11:569–71. [JDa]Google Scholar
Chater, N. & Oaksford, M. (1990) Autonomy, implementation and cognitive architecture: A reply to Fodor and Pylyshyn. Cognition 34:93–107. [MO]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, J. M. & Campbell, J. I. D. (1991) Integrated versus modular theories of number skills and acalculia. Brain and Cognition 17:204–39. [JIDC, JMC]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, J. D., Romero, R. D. & Farah, M. J. (in press) Disengaging from the disengage function: The relation of macrostructure to microstructure in parietal attentional deficits. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. [arMJF, GWH, DCP, CU]Google Scholar
Cohen, L. & Dehaene, S. (1991) Neglect dyslexia for numbers? A case report. Cognitive Neuropsychology 8:39–58. [CU]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coltheart, M. (1985) Cognitive ncunipsychology and the study of reading. In: Attention and performance XI, ed. Posner, M. I. & Marin, O. S. M.. Erlbanm. [aMJF]Google Scholar
Corbetta, M., Miezin, F. M., Shulman, G. L. & Peterson, S. E. (1993) A PET study of visuospatial attention. Journal of Neuroscience 13:1202–26. [rMJF, MIP]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowey, A. (1985) Aspects of cortical organization related to selective attention and selective impairments of visual perception. In: Attention and performance XI, ed. Posner, M. I. & Marin, O. S. M.. Erlbaum. [GWH]Google Scholar
Crick, F. (1989) The recent excitement about neural networks. Nature 337:129–32. [DPC]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daffner, K., Ahern, G., Weintranb, S. & Mesulam, M.-M. (1990 Dissociated neglect behavior following sequential strokes to the right hemisphere. Annals of Neurology 28:97–101. [M-MM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Damasio, A. R. & van Hoesen, G. W. (1985) The limbic system and the localization of herpes simplex encephalitis. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery. and Psychiatry 48:297–301. [SLS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Damasio, H. & Damasio, A. R. (1989) Lesion analysis in nenropsychology. Oxford University Press. [SLS]Google Scholar
Davidoff, J. & Landis, T. (1990) Recognition of unfamiliar faces in prosopagnosia. Neuropsychologia 28:1143–61. [JDa]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, S. N., Lester, R. A., Reymann, K. G. & Collingridge, G. L. (1989) Temporarily distinct pre- and post-synaptic mechanisms maintain longterm potentiation. Nature 338:283–321. [RvH]Google Scholar
de Haan, E. H. F., Bauer, R. M. & Greve, K. W. (1992) Behavioural and physiological evidence for covert face recognition in a prosopagnosic patient. Cortex 28:77–95. [arMJF, JDa, DCP, RVG, RvH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Haan, E. H. F., Young, A. & Newcombe, F. (1987a) Faces interfere with name classification in a prosopagnosic patient. Cortex 23:309–16. [aMJF]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Haan, E. H. F., Young, A. & Newcombe, F. (1987b) Face recognition without awareness. Cognitive Neuropsychology 4:385–416. [aMFJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dennett, D. C. & Kinsbourne, M. (1992) Time and the observer: The where and when of consciousness in the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15:183–247. [RVG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeRenzi, E. (1986) Current issues in prosopagnosia. In: Aspects of face processing, ed. Ellis, H. D., Jeeves, M. A., Newcombe, F. & Young, A.. Martinus Nijhoff. [aMJF]Google Scholar
DeRenzi, E., Gentilini, M. & Barbieri, C. (1989) Auditory neglect. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 52:613–17. [CU]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dodd, B. & Murphy, J. (1992) Language without sound: Prelingual deafness in development. In: Mental lives: Case studies in cognition, ed. Campbell, R.. Blackwell. [RC]Google Scholar
Downing, C. J. & Pinker, S. (1985) The spatial structure of visual attention. In: Attention and performance XI, ed. Posner, M. I. & Marin, O. S. M.. Erlbaum. [CU]Google Scholar
Driver, J. & Halligan, P. W. (1991) Can visual neglect operate in object-centered co-ordinates? An affirmative single-case study. Cognitive Neuropsychology 8:475–96. [CU]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, J. C. & Kirsner, K. (1988) Discovering functionally independent mental processes: The principle of reversed association. Psychological Review 95:91–101. [JAB]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eagleson, H. & Carey, D. P. (1992) Connectionist networks do not model brain function. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15:734–35. [DPC]Google Scholar
Farah, M. J. (1990) Visual agnosia: Disorders of object recognition and what they tell us about normal vision. Bradford Books/MIT Press. [rMJF]Google Scholar
Farah, M. J. (1991) Patterns of co-occurrence among the associative agnosias: Implications for visual object representation. Cognitive Neuropsychology 8:1–19. [RC]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farah, M. J. (in press) Visual perception and visual awareness after brain damage: A tutorial overview. In: Conscious and unconscious infomiation processing: Attention and performance XIV, ed. M. Moscovitch & C. Umiltá. MIT Press. [rMJF]Google Scholar
Farah, M. J. & McClelland, J. L. (1991) A computational model of semantic memory impairment: Modality-specificity and emergent category-specificity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 120(4):339–57. [arMJF, BB, CWH, RAM, DCP, RvH]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farah, M. J., McMullen, P. A. & Meyer, M. M. (1991) Can recognition of living things be selectively impaired? Neuropsychologia 29:185–93. [arMJF, RvH]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farah, M. J., O'Reilly, R. C. & Vecera, S. P. (1993) Dissociated overt and covert recognition as an emergent property of lesioned neural networks. Psychological Review 100:571–88. [arMJF, RC, GWH, DCP, RvH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feldman, J. A. & Ballard, D. (1982) Connectionist models and their properties. Cognitive Science 6:205–54. [SLS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feldman, J. A., Fanty, M. A. & Goddard, N. (1988) Computing with structured neural networks. IEEE Computer 21(3):91–103. [JDi]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Felleman, D. J. & Van Essen, D. C. (1991) Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. Cerebral Cortex 1:1–47. [DPC, JDi, RS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fiorelli, M., Blin, J., Bakchine, S., Laplane, D. & Baron, J. C. (1991) PET studies of cortical diaschisis in patients with motor hemi-neglect. Journal of Neurological Science 104:135–42. [M-MM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flourens, P. (1846) Phrenology examined, 2nd ed. (trans. De Lucena Meigs, Charles). Hogan & Thompson. [TvG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1983) The modularity of mind. Bradford Books/MIT Press. [arMJF, NC, RAM, DCP, CU, TvG, RvH, EZ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. A., Garrett, M. & Swinney, D. (1992) A modular effect in parsing. Unpublished manuscript, Cognitive Science Department, Rutgers University. [EZ]Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. & Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1988) Connectionism and cognitive architecture: A critical analysis. Cognition 28:3–71. [DPC, MO]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forster, K. (1979) Levels of processing and the structure of the language processor. In: Sentence processing: Psycholinguistic studies presented to Merrill Garrett, ed. Cooper, W. & Walker, E.. Erlbaum. [EZ]Google Scholar
Fuhrer, M. J. & Eriksen, C. W. (1960) The unconscious perception of the meaning of verbal stimuli. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 61:432–39. [JDa]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gainotti, G., Messerli, P. & Tissot, R. (1972) Quantitative analysis of unilateral spatial neglect in relation to lateralisation of cerebral lesions. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 35:545–50, [CU]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garrett, M. (1981) Objects of psycholinguistic inquiry. Cognition 10:97–101. [EZ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geschwind, N. (1985) Mechanisms of change after brain lesions. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 457:1–11. ]JAB]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cluck, M. A. & Rumelhart, D. E. (1990) Neuroscience and Connectionist theory. Erlbaum. [PS]Google Scholar
Goldberg, M. E. & Segraves, M. A. (1987) Visuospatial and motor attention in the monkey. Neuropsychologia 25:107–18. [M-MM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldman-Rakie, P. S. (1987) Circuitry of primate prefrontal cortex and regulation of behavior by representational memory. In: Handbook of physiology: Nervous system. Vol. 5: Higher functions of the brain, ed. Plum, F.. American Psychological Society. [rMJF]Google Scholar
Goodale, M. A. & Milner, A. D. (1992) Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends in Neuroscience 15:20–25. [DPC]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodale, M. A., Milner, A. D., Jakobson, L. S. & Carey, D. P. (1991) A neurological dissociation between perceiving objects and grasping them. Nature 349:154–56. [rMJF, DPC]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, J., Morris, J. C., Sandson, J., McKeel, D. W. & Miller, J. W. (1990) Progressive aphasia: A precursor of global dementia? Neurology 40:423–29. [SLS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gregory, R. L. (1961) The brain as an engineering problem. In: Current problems in animal behaviour, ed. Thorpe, W. H. & Zangwill, O. L.. Cambridge University Press. [RAM]Google Scholar
Grodzinsky, Y. (1986) Language deficits and the theory of syntax. Brain and Language 27:135–59. [EZ]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grodzinsky, Y. (1990) Theoretical perspectives on language deficits. MIT Press. [YG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grossberg, S. (1987) Competitive learning: From interactive activation to adaptive resonance. Cognitive Science 11:23–63. [RvH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guariglia, C. & Antonucci, G. (1992) Personal and extrapersonal space: A case of neglect dissociation. Neuropsychologia 30:1001–9. [CU]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halligan, P. W. & Marshall, J. C. (1991) Left neglect for near but not for space in man. Nature 350:498–500. [CU]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halligan, P. W. & Marshall, J. C. (1992) Left visuo-spatial neglect: A meaningless entity? Cortex 28:525–35. [CU]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hart, G. & Gordon, B. (1992) Neural subsystems for object knowledge. Nature 359:60–64. [RAM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hart, J., Berndt, R. S. & Caramazza, A. (1985) Category specific naming deficit following cerebral infarction. Nature (London) 316:439–40. [BB]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hatfield, F. M., Barber, J., Jones, C. & Morton, J. (1977) Object naming in aphasics: The lack of an effect of context or realism. Neuropsychologia 15:717–27. [RAM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hay, D. C. & Young, A. W. (1982) The human face. In: Normality and pathology in cognitive functions, ed. Ellis, A. W.. Academic Press. [AMB]Google Scholar
Heilman, K. M., Watson, R. T. & Valenstein, E. (1985) Neglect and related disorders. In: Clinical neuropsychology, 2nd ed., ed. Heilman, K. M. & Valenstein, E.. Oxford University Press. [rMJF]Google Scholar
Henderson, J. M. & Macquistan, A. D. (1993) The spatial distribution of attention following an exogenous cue. Perception & Psychophysics 53:221–30. [CU]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hildreth, E. C. & Ullman, S. (1989) The computational study of vision. In: Foundations of cognitive science, ed. Posner, M. I.. MIT Press. [aMJF]Google Scholar
Hillis, A. E. & Caramazza, A. (1991) Category-specific naming and comprehension impairment: A double dissociation. Brain 114:2081–94. [aMJF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hinton, G. E. (1992) How neural networks learn from experience. Scientific American 267:105–9. [DPC]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hinton, G. E., McClelland, J. L. & Rumelhart, D. E. (1986) Distributed representations. In: Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition, ed. Rnmelhart, D. E. & McClelland, J. L.. MIT Press. [arMJF]Google Scholar
Hinton, G. E. & Shallice, T. (1991) Lesioning an attractor network: Investigations of acquired dyslexia. Psychological Review 98(1):74–95. [aMJF, AMB, NC, DCP]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Homa, D., Haver, B. & Schwartz, T. (1976) Perceptibility of schematic face stimuli: Evidence for a perceptual Gestalt. Memory and Cognition 4:176–85. [JDa]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Humphreys, G. W. & Bruce, V. (1989) Visual cognition: Computational, experimental and neuropsychological perspectives. Erlbaum. [aMJF]Google Scholar
Humphreys, G. W. & Evett, L. J. (1985) Are there independent lexical and nonlexical routes in word processing? An evaluation of dual-route theory of reading. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8:689–740. [JAB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humphreys, G. W., Freeman, T. & Muller, H. J. (1992) Lesioning a connectionist model of visual search: Selective effects of distractor grouping. Canadian Journal of Psychology 46:417–60. [aMJF, AMB]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Humphreys, G. W. & Riddoch, M. J. (1987) Visual object processing: A cognitive neuropsychological approach. Erlbaum. [aMJF]Google Scholar
Humphreys, G. W. & Riddoch, M. J. (1993) Interactions between object- and space-vision revealed through neuropsychology. In: Attention and performance XIV, ed. Meyer, D. E. & Kornblum, S.. MIT Press. [aMJF, GWH]Google Scholar
Jackendoff, J. A. (1987) Consciousness and the computational mind. Cambridge University Press. [RvH]Google Scholar
Jackson, J. H. (1873) On the anatomical and physiological localization of movements in the brain. Lancet 1:84–85, 162–64, 232–34. [aMJF]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson, J. H. (1878) On affections of speech from diseases of the brain. Brain 1:304–30. [SLS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobs, R. A. & Jordan, M. I. (1992) Computational consequences of a bias toward short connections. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 4:323–36. [DPC, GWH]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jakobson, L. S., Archibald, Y. M., Carey, D. P. & Goodale, M. A. (1991) A kinematic analysis of reaching and grasping movements in a patient recovering from optic ataxia. Neuropsychologia 29:803–9. [DPC]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jared, D., McRae, K. & Seidenberg, M. (1990) The basis of consistency effects in word naming. Journal of Memory & Language 29:687–715. [rMJF]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelley, H. H. (1992) Common-sense psychology and scientific psychology. Annual Review of Psychology 43:1–25. [RS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kettner, R., Marcario, J. & Port, N. (1993) A neural network model of cortical activity during reaching. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 5:14–33. [DPC]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kimberg, D. Y. & Farah, M. J. (in press) A unified account of cognitive impairments following frontal lobe damage: The role of working memory in complex, organized behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. [aMJF]Google Scholar
Kinsbourne, M. (1970) A model for the mechanism of unilateral neglect of space. Transactions of the American Neurological Association 95:143–46. [MK]Google Scholar
Kinsbourne, M. (1971) Cognitive deficit: Experimental analysis. In: Psychobiology, ed. McGaugh, J. L.. Academic Press. [aMJF]Google Scholar
Kinsbourne, M. (1977) Hemi-neglect and hemispheric rivalry. In: Advances in neurology, ed. Weinstein, E. A. & Friedland, R. P.. Raven Press. [arMJF]Google Scholar
Kinsbourne, M. (1987) Mechanisms of unilateral neglect. In: Neurophysiological and neuropsychological aspects of spatial neglect, ed. Jeannerod, M.. Elsevier. [MK]Google Scholar
Kinsbourne, M. (1988) Integrated field theory of consciousness: In: Concept of consciousness in contemporary science, ed. Marcel, A. J. & Bisiach, E.. Oxford University Press. [rMJF, MK, RVG]Google Scholar
Kinsbourne, M. (1993) Integrated cortical field model of consciousness. In: Ciba Foundation Symposium 174: Experimental and Theoretical Studies of Consciousness, ed. Bock, G. R. & Marsh, J.. Wiley. [rMJF, RVG]Google Scholar
Kinsbourne, M. (in press) Orientational bias model of unilateral neglect: Evidence from attentional gradients within hemispace. In: Unilateral neglect: Clinical and experimental studies, ed. I. H. Robertson & J. C. Marshall. Erlbaum. [MK]Google Scholar
Klahr, D., Langley, P. & Neches, R. (1987). Production system models of learning and development, MIT Press. [aMJF]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kosslyn, S. M., Flynn, R. A., Amsterdam, J. B. & Wang, G. (1990) Components of high-level vision: A cognitive neuroscience analysis and accounts of neurological syndromes. Cognition 32:203–77. [aMJF]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kosslyn, S. M. & Van Kleek, M. (1990) Broken brains and normal minds: Why Humpty Dumpty needs a skeleton. In: Computational neuroscience, ed. Schwartz, E.. MIT Press. [aMJF]Google Scholar
Kunst-Wilson, W. R. & Zajonc, R. B. (1980) Affective discrimination of stimuli that cannot be recognized. Science 207:557–58. [RS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LaBerge, D. (1990) Thalamic and cortical mechanisms of attention suggested by recent positron emission tomographic experiments. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2:358–72. [MIP]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lakatos, I. (1970) Falsification and the methodology of scientific research programmes. In: Criticism and the growth of knowledge, ed. Lakatos, I. & Musgrave, A.. Cambridge University Press. [RvH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lashley, K. S. (1950) In search of the engram. In: Symposia for the Society for Experimental Biology, Number 4. Cambridge University Press. [SLS]Google Scholar
Levin, B. & Pinker, S., eds. (1991) Lexical and conceptual semantics. Cognition 41: Special issue. [EZ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luck, S. J., Hillyard, S. A., Mangun, G. R. & Gazzaniga, M. S. (1989) Independent hemispheric attentional systems mediate visual search in split-brain patients. Nature 342:543–45. [MIP]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lynch, J. C., Mountcastle, V. B., Talbot, W. H., & Yin, T. C. T. (1977) Parietal lobe mechanisms for directed visual attention. Journal of Neurophysiology 40:362–89. [M-MM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mandler, G., Nakamura, Y. & Shebo van Zandt, B. J. (1987) Nonspecific effects of exposure on stimuli that cannot be recognized. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 13:646–48. [RS]Google Scholar
Marie, P. (1906) The third left frontal convolution plays no special role in the function of language. In: Pierre Marie's papers on speech disorders, trans. Cole & Cole (1971). Hafner Press. [SLS]Google Scholar
Marin, O. S., Saffran, E. M. & Schwartz, M. (1976) Dissociation of language in aphasia: Implications for normal functions. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 280:868–84. [CS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mark, V. W., Kooistra, C. A. & Heilman, K. M. (1988) Hemispatial neglect is affected by non-neglected stimuli. Neurology 38:1207–11. [CU]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshall, J. C. & Newcombe, F. (1966) Syntactic and semantic errors in paralexia. Neuropsychologia 4:169–76. [AWY]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Massaro, D. (1988) Some criticisms of connexionist models of human performance. Journal of Memory and Language 27:213–34. [RC]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mazzoni, P., Anderson, R. A. & Jordan, M. I. (1991a) A more biologically plausible learning rule for neural networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 88:4433–37. [DPC]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mazzoni, P., Anderson, R. A. & Jordan, M. I. (1991b) A more biologically plausible learning rule than backpropagation applied to a network model of cortical area 7a. Cerebral Cortex 1:293–307. [DPC]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCarthy, R. A. & Warrington, E. K. (1986) Phonological reading: Problems and paradoxes. Cortex 22:359–80. [RAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCarthy, R. A. & Warrington, E. K. (1990) Cognitive neuropsychology: A clinical introduction. Academic Press. [RAM, AWY]Google Scholar
McClelland, J. L. (1981) Retrieving general and specific information from stored knowledge of specifics. Proceedings of the Third Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society:170–72. [AMB]Google Scholar
McClelland, J. L. (1991) Toward a theory of information processing in graded random, interactive networks. Technical Report PDP.CNS.91.1, Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University. [NC]Google Scholar
McClelland, J. L. & Elman, J. L. (1986) Interactive processes in speech perception: The Trace model. In: Parallel distributed processing, vol. 2, ed. Rumelhart, D. E. & McClelland, J. L.. MIT Press. [NC]Google Scholar
McClelland, J. L. & Humelhart, D. E. (1981) An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 1. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review 88:375–407. [rMJF]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, J. L. & Humelhart, D. E. (1982) An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 2. The contextual enhancement effect and some tests and extensions of the model. Psychological Review 89:60–94. [rMJF]Google Scholar
McCloskey, M. (1991) Networks and theories: The place of connectionism in cognitive science. Psychological Science 2:387–95. [rMJF]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCloskey, M. (1992) Cognitive mechanisms in numerical processing: Evidence from acquired dyscalculia. Cognition 44:107–57. [JIDC]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCloskey, M., Caramazza, A. & Basili, A. G. (1985) Cognitive mechanisms in number processing and calculation: Evidence from dyscalculia. Brain anil Cognition 4:171–96. [JIDC]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCloskey, M., Macaruso, P. & Whetstone, T. (1992) The functional architecture of numerical processing mechanisms: Defending the modular model. In: The nature and origins of mathematical skills, ed. Campbell, J. I. D.. Elsevier. [JIDC, JMC]Google Scholar
McCloskey, M., Sokol, S. M. & Goodman, R. A. (1986) Cognitive processes in verbal-number production: Inferences from the performance of braindamaged subjects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 115:307–30. [JIDC, JMC]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNeil, J. E. & Warrington, E. K. (1991) Prosopagnosia: A reclassification. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 43A:267–88. [rMJF, GWH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merigan, W. H. & Maunsell, J. H. R. (1993) How parallel are the primate visual pathways? Annual Review of Neuroscience 16:396–402. [DPC]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mesulam, M.-M. (1981) A cortical network for directed attention and unilateral neglect. Annals of Neurology 10:309–25. [M-MM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mesulam, M.-M. (1990) Large-scale neurocognitive networks and distributed processing for attention, language and memory. Annals of Neurology 28:597–613. [M-MM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, G. & Fellbaum, C. (1991) Semantic networks of English. Cognition 41:197–229. [EZ]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milner, A. D. & Goodale, M. A. (1993) Visual pathways to perception and action. In: The visually responsive neuron: From basic neurophysiology to behavior (Progress in Brain Research, vol. 95), ed. Hicks, T. P., Molotehuikoff, S. & Quo, T.. Elsevier. [DPC]Google Scholar
Milner, A. D., Perrett, D. I., Johnston, R. S., Benson, P. J., Jordan, T. R., Heeley, D. W., Bettucci, D., Mortara, F., Mutani, R., Terazzi, E. & Davidson, D. L. W. (1991) Perception and action in visual form agnosia. Brain 114:405–28. [DPC]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morecraft, R. J., Geula, C. & Mesulam, M.-M. (1993) Architecture of connectivity within a cingulo-fronto-parietal neurocognitive network for directed attention. Archives of Neurology 50:279–84. [M-MM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moscovitch, M. & Umiltà, C. (1990) Modularity and neuropsychology: Modules and central processes in attention and memory. In: Modular deficits in Alzheimer-type dementia, ed. Schwartz, M. F.. MIT Press. [aMJF, CU]Google Scholar
Moscovitch, M. & Umiltà, C. (1991) Conscious and nonconscious aspects of memory: A neuropsychological framework of modules and central systems. In: Perspectives on cognitive neuroscience, ed. Lister, R. G. & Weingartner, H. J.. Oxford University Press. [CU]Google Scholar
Movellan, J. R. (1990) Contrastive Hebbian learning in the continuous Hopfield model. In: Proceedings of the 1989 Connectionist Models Summer School, ed. Touretzky, D. S., Hinton, G. E. & Sejnowski, T. J.. Morgan Kaufmann. [aMJF]Google Scholar
Mozer, M. C. & Behrmann, M. (1990) On the interaction of selective attention and lexical knowledge: A connectionist account of neglect dyslexia. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2(2):96–123. [aMJF, DCP]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nebes, R. D. (1989) Semantic memory in Alzheimer's disease. Psychological Bulletin 106:377–94. [aMJF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newcombe, F., Mehta, Z. & de Haan, E. F. (in press) Category-specificity in visual recognition. In: The neural bases of high-level vision: Collected tutorial essays, ed. M. J. Farah & G. Ratcliff. Erlbaum. [aMJF]Google Scholar
Norris, D. G., (1990) A dynamic-net model of human speech recognition. In: Cognitive models of speech processing: Psycholinguistic and cognitive perspectives, ed. Altmann, G.. MIT Press. [NC]Google Scholar
Oaksford, M. & Chater, N. (1991) Against logical cognitive science. Mind & Language 6:1–38. [MO]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ogden, J. A. (1987) The “neglected” left hemisphere and its contribution to visuospatial neglect. In: Neurophysiological and neuropsychological aspects of spatial neglect, ed. Jeannerod, M.. North-Holland. [CU]Google Scholar
Paivio, A. (1986) Mental representations: A dual-coding approach. Oxford University Press. [JMC]Google Scholar
Patterson, K. E., Seidenberg, M. S. & McClelland, J. L. (1989) Connections and disconnections: Acquired dyslexia in a computational model of reading processes. In: Parallel distributed processing: Implications for psychology and neurobiology, ed. Morris, R. G. M.. Oxford University Press. [aMJF, AMB, NC, RAM, DCP]Google Scholar
Perenin, M.-T. & Vighetto, A. (1988) Optic ataxia: A specific disruption in visuomotor mechanisms. I. Different aspects of the deficit in reaching for objects. Brain 111:643–74. [DPC]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pietrini, V., Nertimpi, T., Vaglia, A., Revello, M. G., Pinna, V. & Ferro-Milone, F. (1988) Recovery from herpes simplex encephalitis: Selective impairment of specific semantic categories with neuroradiological correlation. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgcry, and Psychiatry 51:1284–93. [aMJF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinker, S. (1985) Visual cognition: An introduction. In: Visual cognition, ed. Pinker, S.. MIT Press. [aMJF]Google Scholar
Plaut, D. C. & Shallice, T. (1993) Perseverative and semantic influences on visual object naming errors in optic aphasia: A connectionist account. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 5(1):89–117. [DPC, DCP]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plaut, D. C. & Shallice, T. (in press) Deep dyslexia: A case study of connectionist neuropsychology. Cognitive Neuropsychology. [DCP]Google Scholar
Popper, K. R. (1963) Conjectures and refutations: The growth of scientific knowledge. Routledge & Kegan Paul. [RvH]Google Scholar
Posner, M. I. (1980) Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 32:3–25. [CU]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Posner, M. I. (1988) Structures and functions of selective attention. In: Master lectures in clinical neuropsychology and brain function, research, measurement, and practice, ed. Boll, T. & Bryant, B.. American Psychological Association. [MIP]Google Scholar
Posner, M. I. & Rothbart, M. K. (in press) Constructing neuronal theories of mind. In: High level neuronal theories of the brain, ed. C. Koch & J. Davis. MIT Press. [MIP]Google Scholar
Posner, M. I., Walker, J. A., Friedrich, F. J. & Rafal, K. D. (1984) Effects of parietal lobe injury on covert orienting of visual attention. Journal of Neuroscience 4:1863–74. [aMJF, DPC, MK, CU]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Posner, M. I., Walker, J. A., Friedrich, F. J. & Rafal, K. D. (1987) How do the parietal lobes direct covert attention? Neuropsychologia 25:135–45. [DPC]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prather, P., Shapiro, L., Zurif, E. & Swinney, D. (1991) Real-time examination of lexical processing in aphasics. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research (Special issue on sentence processing) 20:271–81. [EZ]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Price, R. W. (1986) Neurobiology of human herpes virus infections. CRC Critical Reviews in Clinical Neurobiology 2:61–123. [SLS]Google Scholar
Raichle, M. E. (1989) Developing a functional anatomy of the human brain with positron emission tomography. Current Neurology 9:161–78. [SLS]Google Scholar
Reeke, G. N. Jr., & Sporns, O. (1993) Behaviorally based modelling and computational approaches to neuroscience. Annual Review of Neuroscience 16:507–623. [DPC]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Renault, B., Signoret, J. L., Debruille, B., Breton, F. & Bolgert, F. (1989) Brain potentials reveal covert face recognition in prosopagnosia. Neuropsychologia 27:905–12. [JDa]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riddoch, M. J. & Humphreys, G. W. (1987) Visual object processing in optic aphasia: A case of semantic access agnosia. Cognitive Neuropsychology 4:131–85. [GWH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riddoch, M. J. & Humphreys, G. W. (1992) The smiling giraffe: An illustration of a visual memory disorder. In: Mental lives, ed. Campbell, R.. Blackwell. [GWH]Google Scholar
Robinson, D. L., Bowman, E. M. & Kertzman, C. (1991) Covert orienting of attention in macaque II: A signal in parietal cortex to disengage attention. Society of Neuroscience Abstracts 17:442. [rMJF, MIP]Google Scholar
Rosenberg, C. R. & Sejnowski, T. K. (1986) NETtalk: A parallel network that learns to read aloud. EE & CS Technical Report #JHU-EECS-86/01. Johns Hopkins University Press. [aMJF]Google Scholar
Rumelhart, D. E., Hinton, G. E. & McClelland, J. L. (1986) A general framework for parallel distributed processing. In: Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition, ed. Rumelhart, D. E. & McClelland, J. L.. MIT Press. [aMJF]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rumelhart, D. E. & McClelland, J. L. (1982) An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 2. The contextual enhancement effect and some tests and extensions of the model. Psychological Review 89:60–94. [rMJF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rumelhart, D. E. & McClelland, J. L. (1985) Levels indeed! A response to Broadbent. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 114:193–97. [MO]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rumelhart, D. E. & McClelland, J. L. (1986) Parallel Distributed Processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition. Vol. 1: Foundations. MIT Press. [aMJF, PS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saffran, E. M. (1982) Neuropsychological approaches to the study of language. British Journal of Psychology 73:317–37. [CS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sartori, G. & Job, R. (1988) The oyster with four legs: A neuropsychological study on the interaction of visual and semantic information. Cognitive Neuropsychology 5:105–32. [aMJF]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schott, B., Jeannerod, M. & Zahin, M. Z. (1966) L'agnosie spatiale unilatérale: Perturbation en secteur des mécanismes d'exploration et de fixation du regard. Journal Médical de Lyon 47:169–95. [CU]Google Scholar
Schweieh, M. & Bruyer, R. (in press) Heterogeneity in the cognitive manifestations of prosopagnosia: The study of a group of single cases. Cognitive Neuropsychology. [JDa]Google Scholar
Searle, J. R. (1984) Minds, brains and science: The 1984 Reith lectures. British Broadcasting Corporation. [AWY]Google Scholar
Seidenberg, M. S. (1988) Cognitive neuropsychology and language: The state of the art. Cognitive Neuropsychology 5:403–26. [GWH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seidenberg, M. S. & McClelland, J. (1989) A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review 96:323–68. [rMJF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Semenza, C. (1993) Methodological issues. In: A dictionary of neuropsychology, ed. Beaumont, G. & Sergent, J.. Basil Blackwell. [CS]Google Scholar
Semenza, C., Bisiacchi, P. S. & Rosenthal, V. (1988) A function for cognitive neuropsychology. In: Perspectives on cognitive neuropsychology, ed. Denes, G., Semenza, C. & Bisiacchi, P. S.. Erlbaum. [CS]Google Scholar
Sergent, J. (1987) Information processing and laterality effects for object and face perception. In: Visual object processing: A cognitive neuropsychological approach, ed. Humphreys, G. W. & Riddoch, M. J.. Erlbaum. [GWH]Google Scholar
Shallice, T. (1988) From neuropsychology to mental structure. Cambridge University Press. [arMJF, BB, JAB, GWH, RAM, AWY, RVG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shallice, T. (1991) Précis of From neuropsychology to mental structure. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14:429–69. [CS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shallice, T. & Saffran, S. (1986) Lexical processing in the absence of explicit word identification: Evidence from a letter-by-letter reader. Cognitive Neuropsychology 3:429–58. [GWH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shankweiler, D. & Grain, S. (1986) Language mechanisms and reading disorders: A modular approach. Cognition 24:121–37. [YG]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shepherd, G. M. (1990) The significance of real neuron architectures for neural network simulations. In: Computational neuroscience, ed. Schwartz, E. L.. MIT Press. [JDi]Google Scholar
Sheridan, J. & Humphreys, G. W. (1993) A verbal-semantic category-specific deficit. Cognitive Neuropsychology 10:143–84. [GWH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shillcock, R., Lindsey, G., Levy, J. & Chafer, N. (1992) A phonologically motivated input representation for the modelling of auditory word perception in continuous speech. Proceedings of the fourteenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Erlbaum. [NC]Google Scholar
Silveri, M. C. & Gianotti, G. (1988) Interaction between vision and language in category-specific semantic impairment. Cognitive Neuropsychology 5:677–709. [aMJF]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, N. & Tsimpli, I.-M. (1991) Linguistic modularity? A case of a savant-linguist. Lingua 84:315–51. [RC]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sokol, S. M., Goodman-Schulman, R. & McCloskey, M. (1989) In defense of a modular architecture for the number processing system: Reply to Campbell & Clark. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 118 (1):105–10. [JMC]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sokol, S. M., McCloskey, M., Cohen, N. J. & Aliminosa, D. (1991) Cognitive representations and processes in arithmetic: Inferences from the performance of brain-damaged patients. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 17:355–76. [JIDC]Google Scholar
Sparks, D. L., Lee, C. & Rohrer, W. H. (1990) Population coding of the direction, amplitude, and velocity of saccadic eye movements by neurons in the superior colliculus. Cold Spring Harbor Symposia on Quantitative Biology 55:805–11. [rMJF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spurzheim, J. G. (1815) The physiognomical system of Drs. Gall and Spurzheim. Baldwin, Craddock & Joy. [TvG]Google Scholar
Squire, L. R. (1992) Memory and the hippocampus: A synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychological Review 99:195–231. [aMJF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swinney, D. (1979) Lexical access during sentence comprehension: (Re)consideration of context effects. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 18:645–59. [EZ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swinney, D. & Fodor, J. D., eds. (1989) Journal of Psycholinguistic Research (Special issue on sentence processing) 18(1). [EZ]Google Scholar
Swinney, D. & Osterhourt, L. (1990) Inference generation during auditory language comprehension. In: Inferences and text comprehension, ed. Graesser, A. & Bower, G.. Academic Press. [EZ]Google Scholar
Swinney, D., Zurif, E. B. & Nicol, J. (1989) The effects of focal brain damage on sentence processing: An examination of the neurological organization of a mental module. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 1:25–37. [EZ]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tegner, R. & Levander, M. (1991) Through a looking glass. A new technique to demonstrate directional hypokinesia in unilateral neglect. Brain 114:1943–51. [CU]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tippett, L. J. & Farah, M. J. (1992) A model of naming in Alzheimer's disease. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 30:444. [RAM]Google Scholar
Tippett, L. J. & Farah, M. J. (in press) A computational model of naming in Alzheimer's disease: Semantic, visual, and lexical factors. Neuropsychology. [aMJF]Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (1972) Episodic and semantic memory. In: Organization of memory, ed. Tulving, E. & Donaldson, W.. Academic Press. [aMJF]Google Scholar
Umiltà, C. (1988) Orienting of attention. In: Handbook of neuropsychology, vol. 1, ed. Boiler, F. & Grafman, J.. Elsevier. [CU]Google Scholar
Verfaellie, M., Rapcsak, S. Z. & Heilman, K. M. (1990) Impaired shifting of attention in Balint's syndrome. Brain and Cognition 12:195–204. [aMJF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
von Klein, B. E. (1977) Inferring functional localization from neurological evidence. In: Explorations in the biology of language, ed. Walker, E.. Bradford Books/MIT Press. [aMJF]Google Scholar
Warrington, E. K. (1975) The selective impairment of semantic memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 27:635–57. [BB]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warrington, E. K. (1981) Neuropsychological studies of verbal semantic systems. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B 295:411–23. [BB]Google Scholar
Warrington, E. K. (1985) Agnosia: The impairment of object recognition. In: Handbook of clinical neurology, ed. Vinken, P. J., Bruyn, G. W. & Klawans, H. L.. Elsevier. [aMJF]Google Scholar
Warrington, E. K. & McCarthy, R. (1983) Category specific access dysphasia. Brain 106:859–78. [aMJF, EZ]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warrington, E. K. & McCarthy, R. (1987) Categories of knowledge: Further fractionation and an attempted integration. Brain 110:1273–96. [arMJF, BB, EZ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Warrington, E. K. & Shallice, T. (1969) The selective impairment of auditory verbal short-term memory. Brain 92:885–96. [AWY, EZ]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warrington, E. K. & Shallice, T. (1984) Category specific semantic impairments. Brain 107:829–54. [aMJF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wasserman, G. S. (1992) Isomorphism, task dependence, and the multiple meaning theory of neural coding. Biological Signals 1:117–42. [GSW]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiskrantz, L. (1968) Some traps and pontifications. In: Analysis of behavioural change, ed. Weiskrantz, L.. Harper & Row. [RAM]Google Scholar
Yantis, S. & Jonides, J. (1990) Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: Voluntary vs. automatic allocation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 16:121–34. [CU]Google Scholar
Young, A. W., de Haan, E. H., Newcombe, F. & Hay, D. C. (1990) Facial neglect. Neuropsychologia 28:391–415. [CU]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, A. W., Newcombe, F., Hellawell, D. & de Haan, E. H. F. (1989) Implicit access to semantic information. Brain and Cognition 11:186–209. [RvH]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, M. P. & Yamane, S. (1992) Sparse population coding of faces in the inferotemporal cortex. Science 256:1327–31. [rMJF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zurif, E. B. (1980) Language mechanisms: A neuropsycholinguistic perspective. American Scientist 68:305–34. [aMJF]Google Scholar
Zurif, E. B. & Swinney, D. (in press) The neuropsychology of language. In: Handbook of psycholinguistics, ed. M. Gernsbacher. Academic Press. [EZ]Google Scholar
Zurif, E. B., Swinney, D., Prather, P., Solomon, J. & Bushell, C. (in press) An on-line analysis of syntactic processing in Broca's and Wernicke's aphasia. Brain and Language. [EZ]Google Scholar