Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rcrh6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T13:05:00.912Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Animal affect, welfare and the Bayesian brain

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 October 2024

Benjamin Lecorps*
Affiliation:
Bristol Veterinary School, University of Bristol, Bristol BS40 5DU, UK
Daniel Weary*
Affiliation:
Animal Welfare Program, Faculty of Land and Food Systems, 2357 Main Mall, University of British Columbia, Vancouver BC V6T 1Z6, Canada
*
Corresponding authors: Benjamin Lecorps and Daniel Weary; Emails: [email protected]; [email protected]
Corresponding authors: Benjamin Lecorps and Daniel Weary; Emails: [email protected]; [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

According to the Bayesian brain hypothesis, the brain can be viewed as a predictive machine, such that predictions (or expectations) affect how sensory inputs are integrated. This means that in many cases, affective responses may depend more on the subject’s perception of the experience (driven by expectations built on past experiences) rather than on the situation itself. Little research to date has applied this concept to affective states in animals. The aim of this paper is to explore how the Bayesian brain hypothesis can be used to understand the affective experiences of animals and to develop a basis for novel predictions regarding animal welfare. Drawing from the literature illustrating how predictive processes are important to human well-being, and are often impaired in affective disorders, we explore whether the Bayesian brain theories may help understanding animals’ affective responses and whether deficits in predictive processes may lead to previously unconsidered welfare consequences. We conclude that considering animals as predictive entities can improve our understanding of their affective responses, with implications for basic research and for how to provide animals a better life.

Type
Horizon Topic
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Introduction

Emotions are often conceptualised as responses to a specific situation. Under this conception, it is often assumed that animals simply respond in accordance to a situation’s characteristics, similarly to how symptoms were once believed to directly reflect a physiological dysfunction (Ongaro & Kaptchuk Reference Ongaro and Kaptchuk2019). However, many studies have shown that the animal’s perception of the stressor can sometimes better explain their emotional response than can the objective characteristics of the situation (e.g. Weiss Reference Weiss1972).

The Bayesian brain hypothesis (also referred to as the predictive brain or predictive mind) states that an individual does not respond ‘naïvely’ to new information (arising from senses); rather, this new information is integrated within the context of expectations (termed ‘priors’) generated from past experiences (Box 1). This effect can be illustrated by a range of perceptual illusions, such as the size-weight illusion where a smaller object is rated as heavier than a larger one despite being of similar weight (Ellis & Lederman Reference Ellis and Lederman1993). Cognitive explanations for this illusion involve the expectation that the bigger object should be heavier than the smaller one; when this is not confirmed by sensory inputs people evaluate the smaller object as heavier. This type of illusion may come from innate predisposition, can involve the application of general rules of physics learnt through day-to-day interactions (e.g. bigger objects are typically heavier), or be affected by specific experiences. For instance, golfers who know practice balls are heavier than real golf balls will erroneously judge different types of balls (of equal weight) as different while people unaware of this difference will judge them to be similar (Ellis & Lederman Reference Ellis and Lederman1998). It is important to note that ‘expectations’ do not imply conscious, explicit knowledge but can also refer to implicit information (Box 2).

Box 1. The Bayesian brain hypothesis. The term ‘Bayesian brain’ is commonly used in cognitive sciences but may be misleading to some. As Friston (Reference Friston2012) explains, the idea is simply that “we are trying to infer the causes of our sensations based on a generative model of the world”. Predictive theories do not seek to replace a bottom-up approach with a top-down one, but rather provide an explanation for how the brain integrates information coming from senses (interoceptive and exteroceptive) in a context that is set by priors (largely determined by past experiences). These theories stem from Friston’s generative models specifying both the prior distribution of causes and the likelihood of input given the causes (Friston Reference Friston2010).

Box 2. Implicit vs explicit expectations. How perception is affected by implicit or explicit knowledge has been explored in a variety of contexts. People can use both explicit and implicit knowledge, but it appears that implicit knowledge, acquired through day-to-day interactions with the world, is often used to predict events encountered through life. For instance, most people have a limited understanding of physics when asked to express this verbally (Vicovaro Reference Vicovaro2021), but they can show an accurate implicit understanding (Kozhevnikov & Hegarty Reference Kozhevnikov and Hegarty2001); for example, an implicit understanding of motion allows people to determine the trajectory of moving objects and to intercept them. Illusions driven by implicit expectations are often ‘cognitively impenetrable’. For instance, the conscious knowledge that the two objects are of similar weight does not reduce the perceptual illusion that the smaller one seems heavier (Flournoy Reference Flournoy1894). ‘Prior beliefs’ in the context of Bayesian processing often can be assumed to be implicit (Van den Bergh et al. Reference Van den Bergh, Brosschot, Critchley, Thayer and Ottaviani2021) and can therefore be explored without implying conscious evaluations.

If expectations can trick us into believing that two objects are not of the same weight, can they also affect our emotions? Research on human pain illustrates how the shift towards a Bayesian perspective allows a better understanding of emotional responses (e.g. Hoskin et al. Reference Hoskin, Berzuini, Acosta-Kane, El-Deredy, Guo and Talmi2019). In this field, the role of expectations in modulating pain responses is well recognised. A variety of studies have shown that beliefs about pain affect the perception, response, and recovery from painful experiences, such that pain is now conceived as a combination of top-down and bottom-up mechanisms (Wiech & Shriver Reference Wiech and Shriver2018). In particular, pain experiences are seen to be affected by “the sufferer’s beliefs about pain” (Hoskin et al. Reference Hoskin, Berzuini, Acosta-Kane, El-Deredy, Guo and Talmi2019), as illustrated by placebo and nocebo effects (that either minimise or exaggerate pain expectations, respectively) (Stewart-Williams & Podd Reference Stewart-Williams and Podd2004).

Many empirical studies show that expectations can have a substantial effect on affective responses to pain. For example, a negative expectation can abolish the analgesic properties of a drug (e.g. Remifentanil), whereas a positive expectation can double its effect (Bingel et al. Reference Bingel, Wanigasekera, Wiech, Mhuircheartaigh, Lee, Ploner and Tracey2011). These findings are backed by neurobiological evidence showing that higher-order brain regions can affect lower-order ones to suppress or enhance the mismatch between expectations and perception (Schenk et al. Reference Schenk, Sprenger, Onat, Colloca and Büchel2017; Jepma et al. Reference Jepma, Koban, van Doorn, Jones and Wager2018).

Although expectations can bias perception, sensory inputs are still attended to. The Bayesian brain hypothesis does not take a purely top-down approach, rather it considers that sensory information is computed within the context of the prior knowledge, such that “a single highly flexible inferential process fluidly and constantly combines top-down predictions with bottom-up sensory information” (Nave et al. Reference Nave, Deane, Miller and Clark2020). Priors and sensory information can be viewed as probability density functions with margins of uncertainty (Figure 1). Expectations do not necessarily take precedence and may be discarded if they fail to predict current sensory experiences (see Figure 1C).

Figure 1. A graphical representation of how expectations resulting from prior experiences (also termed ‘priors’) are affected by new sensory experiences. Panel A illustrates how the new model (illustrated in green) is a result of animal’s current sensory experiences (shown in yellow) and its former expectations (or priors, shown in blue). The extent to which the new model is updated is affected by the confidence (also termed precision) in expectations (as illustrated in Panel B, where high confidence in expectations results in minor modification to the new model) and by precision of the current sensory experience (shown in Panel C), where high confidence in current sensory inputs results in substantial modification to the new model. Artwork by Ann Sanderson. Figure redrawn from Pezzulo et al. (Reference Pezzulo, Maisto, Barca and Van den Bergh2019).

The weight given to sensory inputs is affected by the strength of the individual’s belief. For example, in randomised controlled trials, people are typically told they have a 50% chance of receiving the drug. When patients are given a placebo but told that they were administrated an analgesic, their pain threshold is typically much higher than people who were told they had a 50% chance of receiving the analgesic (Rief & Glombiewski Reference Rief and Glombiewski2012). Expectations of receiving a treatment can be artificially strengthened (via sensory inputs) by pairing a placebo with an agent that can be directly sensed by participants (e.g. has a distinctive prickling sensation but no analgesic effect); these participants are more likely to respond as if they had received an analgesic compared to participants who were not provided the perceptual cue (Rief & Glombiewski Reference Rief and Glombiewski2012). These findings among others (Hoskin et al. Reference Hoskin, Berzuini, Acosta-Kane, El-Deredy, Guo and Talmi2019), suggest that the affective response to pain is affected by expectations and that the effect of expectations is dependent on the patient’s certainty. As a rule of thumb, the lower the certainty, the more weight is placed on sensory inputs (Tabor & Burr Reference Tabor and Burr2019).

Updating expectations is another important feature of the Bayesian brain theories. If expectations were fixed, individuals would not be able to fine tune their responses to a changing environment. Thus, individuals are thought to use new information to refine predictions for future experiences, so as to reduce discrepancies between expectations and experiences (termed ‘prediction errors’). For instance, subjecting a patient to a high-intensity stimulus when they expected to receive an analgesic may lead to the belief that the analgesic is ineffective (Büchel et al. Reference Büchel, Geuter, Sprenger and Eippert2014) and this new belief can then inform expectations for future experiences. The extent to which new information results in changing expectations is based on confidence in both the new data and the existing model.

As of now, we have mostly taken examples from the human pain literature because we believe it provides a relevant starting point. However, it should be noted that the Bayesian brain application is not limited to pain (Box 3), or to humans. In the sections that follow, we provide an overview of how the Bayesian brain hypothesis can help understanding affective experiences in animals. Expectations are too rarely (or not explicitly) considered when studying affective states in animals and we argue that the Bayesian brain hypothesis can help refine our understanding of these experiences. To illustrate the usefulness of this new perspective, we first explore how it has already been applied in the field of animal cognition, before exploring how it might now be applied to understanding animal affective responses. In the final section, we explore how a Bayesian view can also help understand animal welfare in a way that is consistent with perspectives on human well-being (Miller et al. Reference Miller, Kiverstein and Rietveld2022). For example, we discuss how mood-related disorders in humans often involve issues with updating processes (e.g. patients display persistent negative beliefs), and we call for research on dysfunctions in updating processes in animals, an issue we term ‘Bayesian blindness’.

Box 3. Other affective experiences. The Bayesian brain hypothesis can help explain complex integrations of interoceptive and exteroceptive signals for a range of feelings, for example, “why the sensation of a fast-beating heart feels so different when we suspect heart failure than it does after (say) a bout of vigorous exercise” (Nave et al. Reference Nave, Deane, Miller and Clark2020). Expectations alter results obtained via randomised controlled trials for a range of experiences (Finniss et al. Reference Finniss, Kaptchuk, Miller and Benedetti2010). For instance, participants told they had just received caffeine showed higher cardiovascular activity than those told they had a 50% chance of receiving the drug (Kirsch & Weixel Reference Kirsch and Weixel1988). Research has now started using predictive theories to better understand mechanisms driving complex social behaviours (Brown & Brüne Reference Brown and Brüne2012), arguing that to thrive in social environments one must be a good predictor of others’ actions and intentions. Predictive processes are likely to play a role in many affective experiences and likely other cognitive processes.

A Bayesian approach to learning and affect in animals

Understanding the affective experiences of animals remains a major challenge and an essential endeavour to improving the welfare of animals. Assessing expectations in non-verbal subjects can be challenging, but different experimental paradigms provide direct and indirect approaches, mainly by looking at how animals make decisions. Here, we will first explore the evidence suggesting that animals do behave (or at least learn) as Bayesian entities before exploring how this may alter their affective responses.

A Bayesian conception of animal learning

Animal learning theories have long adopted Bayesian models, progressively replacing traditional ‘associative learning models’ derived from the Rescorla-Wagner (Reference Rescorla, Wagner, Black and Prokasy1972) model. The main difference between traditional and Bayesian models is that the former assumes that, at any given time, an animal’s knowledge is represented by a single value (or associative weight) whereas the latter assumes animals will “entertain a spectrum of hypothetical values” (Kruschke Reference Kruschke2008). Thus, Bayesian models represent uncertainties in an animal’s belief (often represented as a distribution, with a greater spread reflecting lower certainty). Bayesian models of animal learning assume that when new information is processed, it will strengthen or weaken the individual’s belief in specific hypotheses. For instance, if human-animal interactions are increasingly negative, the belief that humans are ‘bad’ will strengthen and the belief that humans are good will shrink. The implication is that “Bayesian models generate probabilistic rather than deterministic anticipations” (Kruschke Reference Kruschke2008). This matters because it means the brain is often ready to see more than one hypothesis and some relatively likely alternatives are more strongly predicted than less likely ones even though they are not the ‘dominant’ prediction.

Bayesian models provide a more sophisticated explanation for multiple forms of learning and help explain many learning-related phenomena such as overshadowing, backward blocking and latent inhibition (for a review, see Courville et al. Reference Courville, Daw and Touretzky2006). Bayesian models can also help explain why animals learn faster when surprised (e.g. Kamin Reference Kamin1967), as these models incorporate how prediction errors affect learning (Courville et al. Reference Courville, Daw and Touretzky2006). It is beyond the scope of this paper to provide an in-depth review of the many advantages of considering animals as “Bayesian learners” (Gershman Reference Gershman2015), but the advantages highlighted by animal learning theorists may also apply to the study of animal emotions.

Influence of past experiences on the affective responses of animals

Some experimental designs in the animal emotion literature are based on the idea that animals learn from past experiences, and that this knowledge can influence affective responses. One key example is fear conditioning. Before conditioning, the animal is expected to show little response to a neutral stimulus such as an audible tone. However, if this tone is repeatedly paired with a negative stimulus, like an electric shock, the animal will begin to respond negatively to the tone. Fear conditioning is best explained by probabilistic inference (Tzovara et al. Reference Tzovara, Korn and Bach2018) and may involve both implicit and explicit expectations (Benedetti Reference Benedetti2020), as evidenced by experiments showing that temporal contiguity between the conditioned and unconditioned stimuli is not enough to elicit fear-conditioned responses (Kamin Reference Kamin1967). Furthermore, some experiments with animals have shown ‘trace conditioning’, a variant of classical conditioning where the time between the unconditioned stimulus (a predicting cue) and the conditioned stimulus (e.g. a reward) progressively increases (Clark & Squire Reference Clark and Squire1998). For instance, hens could be trained to associate a cue with a reward despite a trace of up to 22 s, as illustrated by anticipatory behaviours displayed between the CS and US presentations (Moe et al. Reference Moe, Nordgreen, Janczak, Spruijt, Zanella and Bakken2009); these results illustrate one approach to measuring expectations in animals.

Animals can make sophisticated trade-offs between rewards and punishments (e.g. Rushen Reference Rushen1996). In one study from our research group, Ede et al. (Reference Ede, Von Keyserlingk and Weary2018) showed that dairy calves would accept a painful experience (caused by an intramuscular injection) if they expected a substantial reward (1 L of milk), but fewer calves would do so when they learned that less milk would be provided. Calves trained to expect a less painful subcutaneous injection were willing to accept this even when the smaller reward was expected. This example illustrates how animals can make trade-offs based on expectations regarding both how much pain they will experience and how much reward they will receive. This thinking is implicit in most consumer demand studies where animals learn to expect a specific reward for their work and trade-off that work for an expected outcome (e.g. von Keyserlingk et al. Reference Von Keyserlingk, Amorim Cestari, Franks, Fregonesi and Weary2017; Wenker et al. Reference Wenker, Bokkers, Lecorps, von Keyserlingk, van Reenen, Verwer and Weary2020). In paradigms aiming to assess impulse control, animals may also be asked to make a choice between a higher valued but delayed reward versus an immediate reward of lesser value. For instance, Zebunke et al. (Reference Zebunke, Kreiser, Melzer, Langbein and Puppe2018) showed that piglets could learn to wait for up to 24 s when expecting to receive a better-quality reward, showing that animals can frame expectations between two options as a function of time.

Research on stress responses in animals has also emphasised the importance of expectations (for a review, see Koolhaas et al. Reference Koolhaas, de Boer, Coppens and Buwalda2010). In a well-known study, rats (Rattus norvegicus) given the ability to predict the occurrence of electrical shocks suffered less from stress-related disease compared to those that could not predict when shocks would occur, although both groups received the same number of shocks (Weiss Reference Weiss1972). This result highlights that shocks alone do not account for the risk of stress-related disease. Unpredictability is now commonly seen as worsening the effect of an aversive situation, and something animals will choose to avoid if they can (Bassett & Buchanan-Smith Reference Bassett and Buchanan-Smith2007). Unpredictable housing (Harding et al. Reference Harding, Paul and Mendl2004) or unpredictable exposure to stressors (Doyle et al. Reference Doyle, Lee, Deiss, Fisher, Hinch and Boissy2011) can lead to negative mood.

Providing animals with additional knowledge about the stressor (e.g. magnitude, duration) and control allows animals to act on this knowledge, for example, by changing their behaviour in ways that allow them to minimise exposure to the stressor. For instance, rats given control over shocks (by pressing a lever to make them stop) were at lower risk of developing stress-related pathologies than rats that received the same number of shocks but could not control their occurrence (Weiss Reference Weiss1972). Similarly, sheep that were trained to respond to cues to avoid electric shocks (delivered via a virtual fence) did not show elevated stress responses (Kearton et al. Reference Kearton, Marini, Cowley, Belson, Keshavarzi, Mayes and Lee2020). Perceived control allows individuals to benefit from a type of placebo effect, reducing the expectation and experience of unpleasant affect.

Seeing learnt responses from a Bayesian perspective may help refine experimental approaches. Studies often provide animals with a limited number of experiences from which to gain new knowledge. In the case of push-gate studies, where weight is typically increased over time, animals need to continuously update their expectations regarding how much work is required. As prior updating is probabilistic, animals would likely benefit from multiple exposures to update their previously established model, perhaps even training animals to expect variation in weight (rather than a specific weight); in this way animals should build a model less affected by the previous weight that they encountered and thus be more prone to continue trying. Previous work has used partial (Foster et al. Reference Foster, Temple, Cameron and Poling1997) or variable (de Jonge et al. Reference de Jonge, Ooms, Kuurman, JHR and Spruijt2008a) reinforcement schedules to reduce the risk that future behaviours are highly influenced by recent experiences, sometimes as a means to slow down learning (e.g. Neave et al. Reference Neave, Daros, Costa, von Keyserlingk and Weary2013). The Bayesian view also pushes us to consider elements that are rarely considered, such as the animal’s confidence in their expectations, their ability to collect information about the situation, the predicted consequences, and whether the animal expects to be able to cope with the situation. All of these aspects, which refer to how animals seek and process knowledge, require attention in future studies as they may change the way a situation is experienced. Although, some meta-cognitive aspects (e.g. how confident animals are in their expectations) may prove hard to explore, other aspects (e.g. the effect of knowledge about the stressor) may be relatively easy to manipulate and assess.

A Bayesian view can help understanding otherwise counter-intuitive responses in animals

Placebo effects

Placebo and nocebo effects can be explained when the role of expectations is considered, and several studies on non-human animals have shown evidence of these effects. In one example, Herrnstein (Reference Herrnstein1962) illustrated that similar changes in behaviour were observed in rats injected with scopolamine (a drug that negatively affects learning) as in rats injected with a saline solution after experiencing scopolamine injections. In another study, Nolan et al. (Reference Nolan, Price, Caudle, Murphy and Neubert2012) trained rats to tolerate non-injurious thermal pain to access a sweet solution. Morphine injections increased success in accessing the sweet solution, as expected given the analgesic effects of morphine. However, this effect persisted even when morphine was later replaced by a placebo, suggesting that the expectation of receiving an analgesic reduced the affective response to thermal pain. Interestingly, rats with higher sensitivity to morphine were more placebo sensitive, a result that mirrors findings in humans (Amanzio & Benedetti Reference Amanzio and Benedetti1999). Future studies should further explore whether animals experience placebo and nocebo effects in both pain and non-pain contexts. As we will explore, engineering positive expectations may be a powerful way to mitigate the effect of stressors.

Surprise

We have briefly touched on how surprise (or prediction error) accelerates learning. In humans, prediction errors can also result in emotional responses (e.g. Hoemann et al. Reference Hoemann, Gendron and Barrett2017; Van de Cruys & Van Dessel Reference Van de Cruys and Van Dessel2021). Similar experimental designs have been used when studying the effect of surprise on both learning and affect. These designs involve creating expectations (e.g. that food is delivered in a familiar environment) before violating them (e.g. by suddenly opening an umbrella next to the animal), and then comparing responses of animals that have been surprised or not (e.g. Courville et al. Reference Courville, Daw and Touretzky2006). Perhaps unsurprisingly, sudden events can trigger brief but intense responses in animals (e.g. horses; Lansade et al. Reference Lansade, M-F and Erhard2008). Changes in the intensity of the reinforcer have also been used to assess emotional responses to surprise in animals. For example, animals respond positively when provided larger treats than expected (Boissy et al. Reference Boissy, Manteuffel, Jensen, Moe, Spruijt, Keeling, Winckler, Forkman, Dimitrov, Langbein, Bakken, Veissier and Aubert2007) and vice versa for negative contrasts (Crespi Reference Crespi1942). Contrast effects illustrate how prediction errors can modulate (positively or negatively) the emotional response to stimuli. However, contrast effects are not always detected in animal studies; for examples, studies by Tan et al. (Reference Tan, Handasyde, Rault and Mendl2020) on zebrafish (Danio rerio) and by Davies et al. (Reference Davies, Nicol and Radford2015) on chickens both failed to find these effects. These failures may be related to methodological issues or to species differences in how expectations are processed.

The reinforcing nature of positive prediction errors can help explain the results of a series of studies on pigeons (Columba livia) choosing between a feed source that is rewarded 50% of the time versus another that is rewarded 100% of the time (see Ajuwon et al. Reference Ajuwon, Ojeda, Murphy, Monteiro and Kacelnik2023). From the perspective of optimising resource gains, one would expect pigeons to prefer the always rewarded option, but instead pigeons often display no preference (Smith & Zentall Reference Smith and Zentall2016), or even prefer the partially rewarded option (Case & Zentall Reference Case and Zentall2018). One explanation is that the intermittently rewarded option is a source of positive contrasts between the expected outcome (i.e. 50%) and the value of the reinforcement received. In other words, pigeons may prefer the 50% option because the uncertainty made each reward more rewarding. Consistent with this perspective, Zentall et al. (Reference Zentall, Andrews and Case2019) found that increasing the reinforcement rate to 75% (which reduced the contrast between expectations and reinforcement) decreased the preference for the ‘suboptimal’ alternative. Together, these results suggest that prediction errors affect the perceived value of a reward and may lead animals to mistakenly believe that one option leads to a better reward.

The exploration paradox

Clark (Reference Clark2013) noted the apparent paradox between the need to reduce prediction errors and the fact that animals engage in actions that apparently increase the risk of prediction errors (e.g. they do not “simply seek a nice dark room and stay in it”). Considering information gathering as essential to reducing prediction errors may help understanding why animals appear motivated to explore their environment despite energetic and survival costs. Animals may especially value the opportunity to refine predictions by gathering information in circumstances where predictions are weak (e.g. in a new or rapidly changing environment; Courville et al. Reference Courville, Daw and Touretzky2006).

Exploration allows animals to refine the representations they build about their world, likely driven by positive (e.g. exploration being rewarding) and negative affect (e.g. growing uncertainty leading to feelings of anxiety). This view is consistent with neurobiological evidence indicating that seeking novelty is rewarding (Franks et al. Reference Franks, Reiss, Cole, Friedrich, Thompson and Higgins2013) and can reduce feelings of anxiety associated with uncertainty (Seth & Friston Reference Seth and Friston2016). Work in primates has shown that animals prefer to receive unambiguous information about the magnitude of rewards (Bromberg-Martin & Hikosaka Reference Bromberg-Martin and Hikosaka2009; Blanchard et al. Reference Blanchard, Hayden and Bromberg-Martin2015) and can learn to actively search for cues to reduce uncertainty (Daddaoua et al. Reference Daddaoua, Lopes and Gottlieb2016). This hypothesis, that information gathering is reinforcing, has also been evaluated in rats. Animals consistently preferred options where information was available, even if the information did not change the outcome of the choice (Ajuwon et al. Reference Ajuwon, Ojeda, Murphy, Monteiro and Kacelnik2023). However, signals for positive outcomes had a greater influence on animals’ preference, consistent with the conditioned reinforcement effect (positive cues reinforce responses more than negative ones), suggesting that both uncertainty reduction and conditioned reinforcement are involved in this process.

What drives animals to explore their environment is the subject of debate (e.g. Rushen Reference Rushen1993), but we suggest that Bayesian brain theories can help explain the motivation to explore because uncertainty is a core feature of these models. If animals judge their belief (e.g. that an environment is safe) as uncertain, it makes sense to actively engage in exploration to reduce this uncertainty. From this perspective, information about the environment and the associated decrease in uncertainty, is the reward for exploration, which may explain why exploration is seen as self-rewarding (Byrne Reference Byrne2013; Franks et al. Reference Franks, Gaffney, Graham and Weary2022). The motivation to explore (or more generally to acquire information) may explain why animals perform activities that are not associated with external rewards (Huebner Reference Huebner2012). For example, primates will ‘work’ to solve mazes and puzzles even when they receive no external rewards for completing the task (Harlow Reference Harlow1950). Mice (Mus musculus) will even expose themselves to dangers to explore a new environment (Dashiell Reference Dashiell1925), hungry rats will choose to explore a new environment rather than eat (Small Reference Small1899) and prey species may explore signs of predator presence despite the associated predation risks (e.g. Brown Reference Brown2003; Monclús et al. Reference Monclús, Rödel and Von Holst2006). The costs of exploration can be high; mice that were more explorative in an open field were also at higher risk of predation (Renner Reference Renner1990). That animals are willing to accept such costs suggests that acquiring information is highly valued.

Similarly, the observation that animals will often work for a resource that they can otherwise get for free (‘contrafreeloading’; e.g. de Jonge et al. Reference de Jonge, S-L, Baars and Spruijt2008b; Van Os et al. Reference Van Os, Mintline, DeVries and Tucker2018) is likely related to the motivation of animals to understand their surroundings. Some have argued that animals may be rewarded by acting to get a reward (i.e. a feeling of agency; Inglis et al. Reference Inglis, Forkman and Lazarus1997), but we favour the idea that these behaviours are related to an inherent motivation to maintain accurate predictions (i.e. the information gathering function; Hintze & Yee Reference Hintze and Yee2023) about different options as suggested by the finding that animals will access rewards using a combination of work and free-access options when given the choice (e.g. Rosenberger et al. Reference Rosenberger, Simmler, Nawroth, Langbein and Keil2020).

The continued use of apparently non-optimal options is often taken as evidence of poor learning ability, but such continued sampling can instead be considered as a type of information gathering (Bednekoff & Balda Reference Bednekoff and Balda1997). Continued ‘errors’ may reflect a motivation to sample even when the likelihood of reward is low, and this type of explorative sampling behaviour allows animals to update reward probabilities in a potentially changing world. Overall, evidence from exploratory behaviour, contrafreeloading, and continued sampling in learning tasks reflects the importance of Bayesian prediction errors in understanding animal behaviour and affect.

Failures in updating expectations

Some authors contend that a “general deficiency in prediction updating” is core to the emergence and maintenance of affective disorders in humans (Van de Cruys & Van Dessel Reference Van de Cruys and Van Dessel2021; Van den Bergh et al. Reference Van den Bergh, Brosschot, Critchley, Thayer and Ottaviani2021). We suggest that this line of thinking should also stimulate research in animal welfare, with special implications for animal suffering (Weary Reference Weary and McMillan2019). As we shall review in the section that follows, a predictive processing perspective provides a basis for some ideas already of interest in the animal welfare literature, and in some instances leads to novel predictions with welfare implications that have not yet been explored. In this section, we review recent progress on the role of predictive processing in mental distress and psychopathologies in humans and use this to generate predictions regarding subjective well-being of captive animals.

Persistent, generalised negative beliefs and emergence of affective disorders

Many cognitive vulnerabilities (e.g. pessimistic outlook, intolerance to uncertainty) have been linked with the risk of developing affective disorders such as depression or anxiety. Research suggests that these cognitive risk factors share a common core of dysfunctional negative beliefs (Hong & Cheung Reference Hong and Cheung2015; Kube & Rozenkrantz Reference Kube and Rozenkrantz2021). These beliefs may lead a person “to appraise situations as threatening, to hold negative anticipations about oneself and the world, to experience negative mood states and emotions, and to show poor emotion regulation” (Van den Bergh et al. Reference Van den Bergh, Brosschot, Critchley, Thayer and Ottaviani2021).

Negative beliefs may emerge when experiencing negative situations, especially if they conflict with initial predictions. Active inference theories (Friston Reference Friston2010) propose that to reduce prediction error, one may attempt to either act on what it experiences to improve the fit between their existing model and their current experience or update their model (belief update) to better fit their current experiences (Barrett & Simmons Reference Barrett and Simmons2015). What drives the use of either strategy is not well understood, but the consequences for well-being are marked. The latter process may lead to the belief that, regardless of their actions, the negative experiences will persist. This typically results in a growing lack of confidence in the efficacy of one’s actions to mitigate negative feelings and leads to the perception of being powerless, resulting in the adoption of strategies based on avoidance (e.g. social withdrawal) and to extending negative beliefs to oneself.

Generalised negative beliefs and avoidance coping mechanisms make it hard to update priors, as limited contradictory new evidence is presented to the model. For example, people will adapt to injuries by limiting movements that trigger pain. Once the pain is gone, mobility can resume. However, fear of pain can lead to people displaying protective behaviours long after the pain is gone, contributing to long-term disability (Meulders et al. Reference Meulders, Meulders, Stouten, De Bie and Vlaeyen2017). From the perspective of the predictive brain, if one avoids moving because of the fear of pain, one cannot know that these movements are no longer painful and thus the model is not challenged by new evidence.

The nature of the predictive processing system may also push people to seek situations or evidence that confirm existing biases, producing “a powerful feedback loop where the belief in one’s inability to reduce prediction error through action leads the agent to sample the environment for evidence of this inability, which confirms and supports the negative belief” (Miller et al. Reference Miller, Kiverstein and Rietveld2022). In other words, people may show selective attention to confirmatory evidence, may interpret new data in a way that confirms pre-existing beliefs, and avoid information that would contradict these beliefs (Bruineberg et al. Reference Bruineberg, Kiverstein and Rietveld2018; Kube & Rozenkrantz Reference Kube and Rozenkrantz2021). To our knowledge, evidence for this type of confirmation bias has yet to be explored in animals.

The maintenance of negative beliefs, and their generalisation to one’s competencies and to the future, can lead to a narrowed ability to update priors, as the individual is no longer experiencing situations that could provide contradicting evidence, or because the limited contradicting evidence experienced is rejected given that it does not fit the individual’s well-established model. Such effects are illustrated in depressed patients (Everaert et al. Reference Everaert, Podina and Koster2017) and people with anxiety disorders (Kube & Rozenkrantz Reference Kube and Rozenkrantz2021) who sometimes struggle to update established negative beliefs. In the section that follows, we examine the examples of psychological processes in non-human animals that may relate to failures in updating priors and thus interfere with predictive processes.

Evidence of deficits in updating processes in non-human animals

Kube and Rozenkrantz (Reference Kube and Rozenkrantz2021) identified three mechanisms by which people affected by different psychological disorders display reduced prior updating capacities: 1) attention precision given to prior beliefs (i.e. increased weight given to existing beliefs rather than to new information), 2) interpretation biases (a tendency to interpret new information as confirmatory), and 3) reappraisal (questioning the validity of dis-confirmatory evidence). Here, we highlight work related to re-learning deficits and persistent negative expectations in animals that show intriguing similarities with the reduced prior updating capacities seen in humans. More work is needed to explore this phenomenon in non-human animals, but any of the following markers might be used to infer about animals’ reduced ability to update their model of the world that we refer to as ‘Bayesian blindness’ – an inability to update or access expectations.

Re-learning deficits

Studies have demonstrated that, in certain situations, animals may experience difficulties in updating priors and lack cognitive flexibility or a capacity to re-learn (often assessed by a reversal task; e.g. if A was rewarded and B punished, then B becomes rewarded, and A punished). Some animals behave as if they are incapable of learning a new rule and instead appear to be stuck in their pre-existing model. This reduced cognitive flexibility has been observed in animals reared in suboptimal conditions, such as individually housed dairy calves (e.g. Meagher et al. Reference Meagher, Daros, Costa, von Keyserlingk, Hötzel and Weary2015). This type of cognitive deficit may stem from any of the three processes highlighted by Kube and Rozenkrantz (Reference Kube and Rozenkrantz2021); further work is required to determine if this arises from attention, judgment, or memory deficits.

Negative situations can trigger quick and efficient learning. For instance, animals can associate a specific environment (e.g. a distinctly coloured enclosure) or a specific cue (e.g. a light) with the experience of pain or fear (e.g. Liberzon & Knox Reference Liberzon and Knox2012) and will then avoid these cues in the future. Although any learnt response is expected to wane once the conditioned stimulus is no longer paired with the unconditioned stimulus (i.e. after a period of extinction training), in some cases conditioned fear responses can persist far longer than expected, suggesting that the animal is unable to update its negative expectations. For instance, rats subjected to a single prolonged stressor (used in studies intending to model post-traumatic stress disorders) show deficits in fear extinction learning. They continue to display freezing behaviours (> 20% of the time) long after (up to ten days vs less than four days for controls) the environment is no longer paired with shocks (Winters et al. Reference Winters, Hardy, Sullivan, Powell, Qutaish, Nair, Heimann, Ghayoor, Polyak, Chaby, Rodriguez, Chaar, Oscherwitz and Liberzon2021). Other factors may affect fear extinction learning. For instance, mice that are susceptible to social defeats (i.e. they quickly display prolonged social avoidance) also show extended fear learning (Chou et al. Reference Chou, Huang and Sen2014). Thus, some animals appear to be prone to developing beliefs about fear-eliciting situations and to maintain these expectations; this individual propensity may be related to an earlier trauma or stem from stable individual differences.

Learned helplessness provides another example of a learned response that is resistant to extinction. After repeated experiences with an unescapable threat, some animals will stop responding (e.g. they will no longer show escape attempts). To some extent, this response can be mistaken for habituation, as in both cases the behavioural response wanes to what was initially an aversive experience. The crucial difference is that, in the case of habituation, the reduced response reflects that the stimulus is no longer experienced as aversive, while in the case of learned helplessness, the reduced response is due to the animal no longer perceiving its response to be effective. Helpless rats show a reduction in reward sensitivity, a higher susceptibility to conditioned fear, and a deficit in fear extinction (Shumake et al. Reference Shumake, Barrett and Gonzalez-Lima2005). This phenotype suggests that helpless animals display some characteristics consistent with deficits in updating processes, perhaps linked to deficits in reappraisal (given that drugs promoting memorisation typically improve extinction learning in helpless rats; Wrubel et al. Reference Wrubel, Barrett, Shumake, Johnson and Gonzalez-Lima2007). We suggest that learned helplessness illustrates how encountering repeated negative affective experiences can lead to negative beliefs about animals’ own competencies.

These examples show how animals can sometimes appear to be stuck in the past and unable to learn from new experiences, and suggest that this cognitive deficit can be associated with early-life trauma, lack of cognitive stimulations, and persistent exposure to negative events. We predict that animals exposed to such conditions are more likely to experience deficits in their ability to update expectations, negatively affecting both their current welfare and their future capacity to cope with new events. Until now, only limited research has explored how cognitive flexibility, including the capacity to update priors, improves animals’ coping abilities.

Persistent negative expectations

The term fearfulness is often used in the animal welfare literature to describe a condition that may be similar to persistent anxiety in humans. Animals are described as fearful if they are averse to novelty (e.g. show reduced exploration of novel stimuli) and are highly reactive to stimuli perceived as threatening. The factors that lead animals to become fearful and the lifelong implications of fearfulness are poorly understood. Absence of maternal care (in chicks) or early social isolation (in dairy calves) are examples of environmental factors that lead to increased fearfulness (Meagher et al. Reference Meagher, Daros, Costa, von Keyserlingk, Hötzel and Weary2015; Edgar et al. Reference Edgar, Held, Jones and Troisi2016). Persistent fearfulness may be due to, or lead to, deficits in predictive processing, similarly to what has been suggested for people exhibiting signs of anxiety disorders or personality traits, such as pessimism and neuroticism (Carver & Scheier Reference Carver and Scheier2014; Van den Bergh et al. Reference Van den Bergh, Brosschot, Critchley, Thayer and Ottaviani2021). How animals respond to new or threatening situations can be detected early in life and, in some individuals, remains stable over time (De Haas et al. Reference De Haas, Kops, Bolhuis, Groothuis, Ellen and Rodenburg2012; Lecorps et al. Reference Lecorps, Weary and von Keyserlingk2018b; Christensen et al. Reference Christensen, Beblein and Malmkvist2020; Neave et al. Reference Neave, Costa, Weary and von Keyserlingk2020). Fearfulness has received some attention in the animal welfare literature, but mostly as an outcome (e.g. poor conditions lead to heightened fearfulness). New work is required on how fearfulness develops and how it affects the way animals cope with challenges. We suggest that fearfulness in animals may lead to persistent updating issues, to the extent that this reduces the animal’s exposure to situations that can contradict their beliefs.

Stressors can also generate negative expectations about unrelated situations. For example, the same rats that displayed diminished fear extinction following a single-prolonged stressor also showed fear overgeneralisation (Winters et al. Reference Winters, Hardy, Sullivan, Powell, Qutaish, Nair, Heimann, Ghayoor, Polyak, Chaby, Rodriguez, Chaar, Oscherwitz and Liberzon2021), meaning that non-threatening new environments also triggered fear responses. Accumulating evidence suggest that stressors impact how animals interpret ambiguous information, with a shift towards greater pessimism. Unpredictable housing (Harding et al. Reference Harding, Paul and Mendl2004) and pain (Neave et al. Reference Neave, Daros, Costa, von Keyserlingk and Weary2013), among many other experiences (for a review, see Lagisz et al. Reference Lagisz, Zidar, Nakagawa, Neville, Sorato, Paul, Bateson, Mendl and Løvlie2020; Neville et al. Reference Neville, Nakagawa, Zidar, Paul, Lagisz, Bateson, Lovlie and Mendl2020), can induce negative perceptions of otherwise ambiguous situations. Long-term changes in judgment bias in response to stressors have rarely been explored (e.g. Paul et al. Reference Paul, Browne, Mendl, Caplen, Held, Trevarthen and Nicol2023), but persistent pessimism (as a trait) has been observed in several species and is thought to make animals more vulnerable to stressors (for a review, see Lecorps et al. Reference Lecorps, Weary and von Keyserlingk2021a) with examples in rats (e.g. Rygula et al. Reference Rygula, Papciak and Popik2013), dairy calves (Lecorps et al. Reference Lecorps, Kappel, Weary and von Keyserlingk2018a, Reference Lecorps, Nogues, von Keyserlingk and Weary2020) and pigs (Horback & Parsons Reference Horback and Parsons2019).

There is a rich history of animal research on persistent changes in anxiety-related and depressive-like behaviours that involve persistent negative expectations such as anhedonia (the apparent de-valuation of rewards), typically in response to sub-optimal housing or chronic exposure to uncontrollable stressors (Wang et al. Reference Wang, Timberlake, Prall and Dwivedi2017). Such behaviours are consistent with generalised negative expectations about potential threats (such that the animal tends to overestimate the negative consequences) and underestimation of potential rewards (the likelihood/value of rewards). The latter is particularly interesting given that, despite consuming some sucrose water, anhedonic animals no longer prefer this to water (e.g. Rygula et al. Reference Rygula, Abumaria, Flügge, Fuchs, Rüther and Havemann-Reinecke2005). These results suggest that some top-down mechanisms downplay the rewarding property of sucrose or the ability to remember this as rewarding. Similarly, it has been reported that patients with major depressive disorder will enjoy accessible rewards but display no motivation to access these (Treadway & Zald Reference Treadway and Zald2011), suggesting that these patients display deficits in prior precision such that they do not perceive a treat to be rewarding until it is consumed. Persistent anhedonia-like responses have been shown in a variety of species living in stressful conditions (e.g. pigs; Figueroa et al. Reference Figueroa, Solà-Oriol, Manteca, Pérez and Dwyer2015) or after potentially traumatic experiences (e.g. in dairy cows; Lecorps et al. Reference Lecorps, Welk, Weary and von Keyserlingk2021b), but these responses remain poorly understood. Future work should explore whether anhedonic animals display a generalised and persistent devaluation of any rewarding activities and whether this state is associated with perceptual, appraisal, or memorisation deficits.

Limitations

We have reviewed some of the literature that suggest animals act as Bayesian entities and attempted to illustrate how this could affect their affective responses and welfare, but there are several limitations to our conceptual review. First, we have looked at evidence illustrating key aspects of the application of this perspective to animal affect, including examples that are difficult to understand when using a bottom-up view of affective experiences. These examples were selected based on what we judged to be their relevance, as in any narrative review. The examples provide evidence of how a Bayesian perspective can prove useful, but our approach should not be seen as a definitive test. We conclude simply that Bayesian brain theories can usefully generate hypotheses, and that future tests of these hypotheses may lead to an improved understanding of animal affective responses.

Second, although the application of the Bayesian theorem is recognised as one of the most influential ideas in cognitive sciences (Melnikoff & Strohminger Reference Melnikoff and Strohminger2024), some areas of human cognition, such as belief polarisation (e.g. when a person strengthens their belief despite strong evidence that it is wrong), appear inconsistent with this theorem (Mandelbaum et al. Reference Mandelbaum, Won, Gross and Firestone2020). Our goal is not to convince readers that animals must always be considered Bayesian entities, but rather that Bayesian theories can help generate useful predictions and insights regarding what they feel.

Third, we have deliberately taken a generic stance in applying the concepts discussed to ‘animals’; we acknowledge that species are likely to differ in the way they process information and use predictive processes, but argue that in many cases understanding the predictive element will allow for an improved understanding of affective states and welfare. Whether some species process information in a non-Bayesian way remains to be explored.

Animal welfare implications and conclusion

The Bayesian brain hypothesis has led to new insights regarding human cognition, affect and well-being. We reviewed evidence suggesting that these theories can be applied to non-human animals and explored how they can help understand affective responses in animals. Animals are predicted to react to situations in accordance with their past experiences; understanding how past experiences can affect beliefs is thus of broad importance to work exploring how animals respond to stressors, a key aspect of many welfare studies. Moreover, we point to the possibility that deficits in an animal’s ability to update or access their beliefs (Bayesian blindness) can result in persistent and broad negative welfare impacts, as shown in humans. We urge new work to focus attention on the contextual factors that explain why animals react the way they do, as well as more applied research seeking to prevent the formation and maintenance of negative beliefs. As an example of the latter, work is required on how best to engineer more positive expectations, such as those that can be achieved via positive reinforcement training, shifting the animals’ perception of mildly aversive stimuli to neutral or even positive. We also encourage new basic research on the factors that interfere with the development of expectations in animals, including housing and management practices that diminish learning or otherwise undermine the animal’s ability to develop an accurate model of their world and the efficacy of their actions.

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by Natural Science and Engineering Research Council (NSERC) Discovery grants (RGPIN-2016-04620 and RGPIN-2023-04350). General funding for the UBC Animal Welfare industrial partners the Dairy Farmers of Canada (Ottawa, ON, Canada), Saputo Inc (Montreal, QC, Canada), British Columbia Dairy Association (Burnaby, BC, Canada), Alberta Milk (Edmonton, AB, Canada), Intervet Canada Corporation (Kirkland, QC, Canada), Boehringer Ingelheim Animal Health (Burlington, ON, Canada), BC Cattle Industry Development Fund (Kamloops, BC, Canada), The Semex Alliance (Guelph, ON, Canada), Lactanet (Sainte-Anne-de-Bellevue, QC, Canada), Dairy Farmers of Manitoba (Winnipeg, MB, Canada), and the Saskatchewan Milk Marketing Board (Regina, SK, Canada). The authors wish to thank Jillian Hendricks, Mike Mendl, Liz Paul and Amelia St-John Wallis for their help and comments in earlier versions of this manuscript.

Competing interest

None.

Footnotes

Author contributions: Conceptualisation: BL, DW; Investigation: BL, DW; Project administration: BL, DW; Writing – original draft: BL, DW; Writing – review & editing: BL, DW

References

Ajuwon, V, Ojeda, A, Murphy, RA, Monteiro, T and Kacelnik, A 2023 Paradoxical choice and the reinforcing value of information. Animal Cognition 26: 623637. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-022-01698-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amanzio, M and Benedetti, F 1999 Neuropharmacological dissection of placebo analgesia: Expectation- activated opioid systems versus conditioning-activated specific subsystems. Journal of Neuroscience 19: 484494. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.19-01-00484.1999CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrett, LF and Simmons, WK 2015 Interoceptive predictions in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 16: 419429. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3950CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bassett, L and Buchanan-Smith, HM 2007 Effects of predictability on the welfare of captive animals. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 102: 223245.10.1016/j.applanim.2006.05.029CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bednekoff, PA and Balda, RP 1997 Clark’s nutcracker spatial memory: Many errors might not be due to forgetting. Animal Behaviour 54: 691698. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1997.0473CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benedetti, F 2020 A modern view of placebo and placebo-related effects. Placebo Effects pp 193243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bingel, U, Wanigasekera, V, Wiech, K, Mhuircheartaigh, RN, Lee, MC, Ploner, M and Tracey, I 2011 The effect of treatment expectation on drug efficacy: Imaging the analgesic benefit of the opioid remifentanil. Science Translational Medicine 3: 110. https://doi.org/10.1126/scitranslmed.3001244CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blanchard, TC, Hayden, BY and Bromberg-Martin, ES 2015 Orbitofrontal cortex uses distinct codes for different choice attributes in decisions motivated by curiosity. Neuron 85: 602614. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2014.12.050CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boissy, A, Manteuffel, G, Jensen, MB, Moe, RO, Spruijt, B, Keeling, LJ, Winckler, C, Forkman, B, Dimitrov, I, Langbein, J, Bakken, M, Veissier, I and Aubert, A 2007 Assessment of positive emotions in animals to improve their welfare. Physiology & Behavior 92: 375397. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.02.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bromberg-Martin, ES and Hikosaka, O 2009 Midbrain dopamine neurons signal preference for advance information about upcoming rewards. Neuron 63: 119126. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2009.06.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, EC and Brüne, M 2012 The role of prediction in social neuroscience. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 6: 119. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00147CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, GE 2003 Learning about danger: Chemical alarm cues and local risk assessment in prey fishes. Fish and Fisheries 4: 227234. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1467-2979.2003.00132.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruineberg, J, Kiverstein, J and Rietveld, E 2018 The anticipating brain is not a scientist: the free-energy principle from an ecological-enactive perspective. Synthese 195: 24172444. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-016-1239-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Büchel, C, Geuter, S, Sprenger, C and Eippert, F 2014 Placebo analgesia: A predictive coding perspective. Neuron 81: 12231239. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2014.02.042CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrne, RW 2013 Animal curiosity. Current Biology 23: R469R470. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.02.058CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carver, CS and Scheier, MF 2014 Dispositional optimism. Trends in Cognition Science 18: 293299. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.02.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Case, JP and Zentall, TR 2018 Suboptimal choice in pigeons: Does the predictive value of the conditioned reinforcer alone determine choice? Behavioural Processes: 01. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2018.07.018CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chou, D, Huang, CC and Sen, Hsu K 2014 Brain-derived neurotrophic factor in the amygdala mediates susceptibility to fear conditioning. Experimental Neurology 255: 1929. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.expneurol.2014.02.016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christensen, JW, Beblein, C and Malmkvist, J 2020 Development and consistency of fearfulness in horses from foal to adult. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 232: 105106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2020.105106CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, A 2013 Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36: 181204. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X12000477CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, RE and Squire, LR 1998 Classical conditioning and brain systems: The role of awareness. Science 280. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(02)02041-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Courville, AC, Daw, ND and Touretzky, DS 2006 Bayesian theories of conditioning in a changing world. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10: 294300. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.05.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crespi, LP 1942 Quantitative variation of incentive and performance in the white rat. The American Journal of Psychology 55: 467. https://doi.org/10.2307/1417120CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daddaoua, N, Lopes, M and Gottlieb, J 2016 Intrinsically motivated oculomotor exploration guided by uncertainty reduction and conditioned reinforcement in non-human primates. Scientific Reports 6: 115. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep20202CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dashiell, JF 1925 A quantitative demonstration of animal drive. Journal of Comparative Psychology 5: 205208. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0071833CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davies, AC, Nicol, CJ and Radford, AN 2015 Effect of reward downshift on the behaviour and physiology of chickens. Animal Behaviour 105: 2128. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.04.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Haas, EN, Kops, MS, Bolhuis, JE, Groothuis, TGG, Ellen, ED and Rodenburg, TB 2012 The relation between fearfulness in young and stress-response in adult laying hens, on individual and group level. Physiology and Behavior 107: 433439. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.08.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Jonge, FH, Ooms, M, Kuurman, WW, JHR, Maes and Spruijt, BM 2008a Are pigs sensitive to variability in food rewards? Applied Animal Behaviour Science 114: 93104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2008.01.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Jonge, FH, S-L, Tilly, Baars, AM and Spruijt, BM 2008b On the rewarding nature of appetitive feeding behaviour in pigs (Sus scrofa): Do domesticated pigs contrafreeload? Applied Animal Behaviour Science 114: 359372. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2008.03.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doyle, RE, Lee, C, Deiss, V, Fisher, AD, Hinch, GN and Boissy, A 2011 Measuring judgement bias and emotional reactivity in sheep following long-term exposure to unpredictable and aversive events. Physiology and Behavior 102: 503510. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.01.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ede, T, Von Keyserlingk, MAG and Weary, DM 2018 Approach-aversion in calves following injections. Scientific Reports 8: 610. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-27669-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edgar, J, Held, S, Jones, C and Troisi, C 2016 Influences of maternal care on chicken welfare. Animals 6. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani6010002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, RR and Lederman, SJ 1993 The role of haptic versus visual volume cues in the size-weight illusion. Perception & Psychophysics 53: 315324. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03205186CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, RR and Lederman, SJ 1998 The golf-ball illusion: Evidence for top-down processing in weight perception. Perception 27: 193201. https://doi.org/10.1068/p270193CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Everaert, J, Podina, IR and Koster, EHW 2017 A comprehensive meta-analysis of interpretation biases in depression. Clinical Psychology Review 58: 3348. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2017.09.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figueroa, J, Solà-Oriol, D, Manteca, X, Pérez, JF and Dwyer, DM 2015 Anhedonia in pigs? Effects of social stress and restraint stress on sucrose preference. Physiology and Behavior 151: 509515. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.08.027CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Finniss, DG, Kaptchuk, TJ, Miller, F and Benedetti, F 2010 Biological, clinical, and ethical advances of placebo effects. The Lancet 375: 686695. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(09)61706-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flournoy, T 1894 Illusions de poids. L’Année Psychologique 1: 198208. https://doi.org/10.3406/psy.1894.1051CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foster, TM, Temple, W, Cameron, B and Poling, A 1997 Demand curves for food in hens: Similarity under fixed-ratio and progressive-ratio schedules. Behavioural Processes 39: 177185. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0376-6357(96)00051-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Franks, B, Gaffney, LP, Graham, C and Weary, DM 2022 Curiosity in zebrafish (Danio rerio)? Behavioral responses to 30 novel objects. Frontiers in Veterinary Science 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fvets.2022.1062420Google ScholarPubMed
Franks, B, Reiss, D, Cole, P, Friedrich, V, Thompson, N and Higgins, ET 2013 Predicting how individuals approach enrichment: Regulatory focus in cotton-top tamarins (Sanguinus oedipus). Zoo Biology 32: 427435. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.21075CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friston, K 2010 The free-energy principle: A unified brain theory? Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11: 127138. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2787CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friston, K 2012 The history of the future of the Bayesian brain. NeuroImage 62: 12301233. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gershman, SJ 2015 A unifying probabilistic view of associative learning. PLoS Computational Biology 11: 120. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004567CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harding, EJ, Paul, ES and Mendl, M 2004 Cognitive bias and affective state. Nature 427: 312. https://doi.org/10.1038/427312aCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harlow, HF 1950 Learning and satiation of response in intrinsically motivated complex puzzle performance by monkeys. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 43: 289294. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0058114CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herrnstein, RJ 1962 Placebo effects in rats. Science: 677678.Google Scholar
Hintze, S and Yee, JR 2023 Animals in flow – towards the scientific study of intrinsic reward in animals. Biological Reviews 98: 792806. https://doi.org/10.1111/brv.12930CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoemann, K, Gendron, M and Barrett, LF 2017 Mixed emotions in the predictive brain. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 15: 5157. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.05.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hong, RY and Cheung, MWL 2015 The structure of cognitive vulnerabilities to depression and anxiety: Evidence for a common core etiologic process based on a meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychological Science 3: 892912. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702614553789CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horback, KM and Parsons, TD 2019 Judgement bias testing in group-housed gestating sows. Behavioural Processes 159: 8692. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2018.12.021CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoskin, R, Berzuini, C, Acosta-Kane, D, El-Deredy, W, Guo, H and Talmi, D 2019 Sensitivity to pain expectations: A Bayesian model of individual differences. Cognition 182: 127139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.08.022CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huebner, B 2012 Surprisal and valuation in the predictive brain. Frontiers in Psychology 3: 12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00415CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Inglis, IR, Forkman, B and Lazarus, J 1997 Free food or earned food? A review and fuzzy model of contrafreeloading. Animal Behaviour 53: 11711191. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1996.0320CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jepma, M, Koban, L, van Doorn, J, Jones, M and Wager, TD 2018 Behavioural and neural evidence for self-reinforcing expectancy effects on pain. Nature Human Behaviour 2: 838855. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0455-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kamin, L 1967 Attention-like processes in classical conditioning Miami Symposium: 931.Google Scholar
Kearton, T, Marini, D, Cowley, F, Belson, S, Keshavarzi, H, Mayes, B and Lee, C 2020 The influence of predictability and controllability on stress responses to the aversive component of a virtual fence. Frontiers in Veterinary Science 7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fvets.2020.580523CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirsch, I and Weixel, LJ 1988 Double-blind versus deceptive administration of a placebo. Behavioral Neuroscience 102: 319323. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.102.2.319CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koolhaas, JM, de Boer, SF, Coppens, CM and Buwalda, B 2010 Neuroendocrinology of coping styles: Towards understanding the biology of individual variation. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology 31: 307321. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yfrne.2010.04.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kozhevnikov, M and Hegarty, M 2001 Impetus beliefs as default heuristics: Dissociation between explicit and implicit knowledge about motion. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 8: 439453. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196179CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kruschke, JK 2008 Bayesian approaches to associative learning: From passive to active learning. Learning and Behavior 36: 210226. https://doi.org/10.3758/LB.36.3.210CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kube, T and Rozenkrantz, L 2021 When beliefs face reality: An integrative review of belief updating in mental health and illness. Perspectives on Psychological Science 16: 247274. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620931496CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lagisz, M, Zidar, J, Nakagawa, S, Neville, V, Sorato, E, Paul, ES, Bateson, M, Mendl, M and Løvlie, H 2020 Optimism, pessimism and judgement bias in animals: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 118: 317. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.07.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lansade, L, M-F, Bouissou and Erhard, HW 2008 Fearfulness in horses: A temperament trait stable across time and situations. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 115: 182200. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2008.06.011CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lecorps, B, Kappel, S, Weary, DM and von Keyserlingk, MAG 2018a Dairy calves’ personality traits predict social proximity and response to an emotional challenge. Scientific Reports 8: 16350. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-34281-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lecorps, B, Nogues, E, von Keyserlingk, MAG and Weary, DM 2020 Pessimistic dairy calves are more vulnerable to pain-induced anhedonia. PLoS One: 119. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0242100CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lecorps, B, Weary, DM and von Keyserlingk, MAG 2018b Pessimism and fearfulness in dairy calves. Scientific Reports 8: 1421. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-17214-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lecorps, B, Weary, DM and von Keyserlingk, MAG 2021a Negative expectations and vulnerability to stressors in animals. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 130: 240251. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.08.025CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lecorps, B, Welk, A, Weary, DM and von Keyserlingk, MAG 2021b Postpartum stressors cause a reduction in mechanical brush use in dairy cows. Animals 11. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11113031CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liberzon, I and Knox, D 2012 Expanding our understanding of neurobiological mechanisms of resilience by using animal models. Neuropsychopharmacology 37: 15581558. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2012.9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mandelbaum, E, Won, I, Gross, S and Firestone, C 2020 Can resources save rationality? Anti-Bayesian updating in cognition and perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 43. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X19001717CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meagher, RK, Daros, RR, Costa, JHC, von Keyserlingk, MAG, Hötzel, MJ and Weary, DM 2015 Effects of degree and timing of social housing on reversal learning and response to novel objects in dairy calves. PloS One 10: e0132828. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0132828CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Melnikoff, DE and Strohminger, N 2020 The automatic influence of advocacy on lawyers and novices. Nature Human Behaviour 4: 12581264. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-00943-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Melnikoff, DE and Strohminger, N 2024 Bayesianism and wishful thinking are compatible. Nature Human Behaviour. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-024-01819-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meulders, A, Meulders, M, Stouten, I, De Bie, J and Vlaeyen, JWS 2017 Extinction of fear generalization: A comparison between fibromyalgia patients and healthy control participants. Journal of Pain 18: 7995. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2016.10.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, M, Kiverstein, J and Rietveld, E 2022 The predictive dynamics of happiness and well-being. Emotion Review 14: 1530. https://doi.org/10.1177/17540739211063851CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moe, RO, Nordgreen, J, Janczak, AM, Spruijt, BM, Zanella, AJ and Bakken, M 2009 Trace classical conditioning as an approach to the study of reward-related behaviour in laying hens: A methodological study. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 121: 171178. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2009.10.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Monclús, R, Rödel, HG and Von Holst, D 2006 Fox odour increases vigilance in European rabbits: A study under semi-natural conditions. Ethology 112: 11861193. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01275.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nave, K, Deane, G, Miller, M and Clark, A 2020 Wilding the predictive brain. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science 11: 111. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1542Google ScholarPubMed
Neave, HW, Costa, JHC, Weary, DM and von Keyserlingk, MAG 2020 Long-term consistency of personality traits in cattle. Royal Society Open Science 7:191849. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.191849CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neave, HW, Daros, RR, Costa, JHC, von Keyserlingk, MAG and Weary, DM 2013 Pain and pessimism: Dairy calves exhibit negative judgement bias following hot-iron disbudding. PloS One 8: e80556. doi: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0080556CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neville, V, Nakagawa, S, Zidar, J, Paul, ES, Lagisz, M, Bateson, M, Lovlie, H and Mendl, M 2020 Pharmacological manipulations of judgement bias: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 108: 269286. https://doi.org/10.1101/612382CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nolan, TA, Price, DD, Caudle, RM, Murphy, NP and Neubert, JK 2012 Placebo-induced analgesia in an operant pain model in rats. Pain 153: 20092016. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2012.04.026CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ongaro, G and Kaptchuk, TJ 2019 Bayesian Brain. Pain 160. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9780262042383.001.0001Google ScholarPubMed
Paul, ES, Browne, W, Mendl, MT, Caplen, G, Held, S, Trevarthen, A and Nicol, CJ 2023 Affective trajectories: Are hens influenced by positive and negative changes in their living conditions? Applied Animal Behaviour Science 261: 105883. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2023.105883CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pezzulo, G, Maisto, D, Barca, L and Van den Bergh, O 2019 Symptom perception from a predictive processing perspective. Clinical Psychology in Europe 1. https://doi.org/10.32872/cpe.v1i4.35952CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Renner, MJ 1990 Neglected aspects of exploratory and investigatory behavior. Psychobiology 18: 1622. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03327209CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rescorla, R and Wagner, A 1972 A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: The effectiveness of reinforcement and non-reinforcement. In Black, AH and Prokasy, WF (eds) Classical Conditioning: Current Research and Theory Appleton-Century-Crofts: New York, NY, USA.Google Scholar
Rief, W and Glombiewski, JA 2012 The hidden effects of blinded, placebo-controlled randomized trials: An experimental investigation. Pain 153: 24732477. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2012.09.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenberger, K, Simmler, M, Nawroth, C, Langbein, J and Keil, N 2020 Goats work for food in a contrafreeloading task. Scientific Reports 10. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-78931-wCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rushen, J 1993 Exploration in the pig may not be endogenously motivated. Animal Behaviour 45(1): 183184. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1993.1016CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rushen, J 1996 Using aversion learning techniques to assess the mental state, suffering, and welfare of farm animals. Journal of Animal Science 74: 19901995. https://doi.org/10.2527/1996.7481990xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rygula, R, Abumaria, N, Flügge, G, Fuchs, E, Rüther, E and Havemann-Reinecke, U 2005 Anhedonia and motivational deficits in rats: Impact of chronic social stress. Behavioural Brain Research 162: 127134. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2005.03.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rygula, R, Papciak, J and Popik, P 2013 Trait pessimism predicts vulnerability to stress-induced anhedonia in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology 38: 21882196. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2013.116CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schenk, LA, Sprenger, C, Onat, S, Colloca, L and Büchel, C 2017 Suppression of striatal prediction errors by the prefrontal cortex in placebo hypoalgesia. Journal of Neuroscience 37: 97159723. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1101-17.2017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seth, AK and Friston, KJ 2016 Active interoceptive inference and the emotional brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 371. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shumake, J, Barrett, D and Gonzalez-Lima, F 2005 Behavioral characteristics of rats predisposed to learned helplessness: Reduced reward sensitivity, increased novelty seeking, and persistent fear memories. Behavioural Brain Research 164: 222230.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Small, W 1899 Notes on the psychic development of the young white rat. The American Journal of Psychology 11: 80100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, AP and Zentall, TR 2016 Suboptimal choice in pigeons: Choice is primarily based on the value of the conditioned reinforcer rather than overall reinforcement rate. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition 42: 212220. https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000092Google ScholarPubMed
Stewart-Williams, S and Podd, J 2004 The placebo effect: Dissolving the expectancy versus conditioning debate. Psychological Bulletin 130: 324340. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.130.2.324CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tabor, A and Burr, C 2019 Bayesian learning models of pain: A call to action. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 26: 5461. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.10.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tan, SLT, Handasyde, KA, Rault, JL and Mendl, M 2020 Insensitivity to reward shifts in zebrafish (Danio rerio) and implications for assessing affective states. Animal Cognition 23: 87100. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-019-01318-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treadway, MT and Zald, DH 2011 Reconsidering anhedonia in depression: Lessons from translational neuroscience. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35: 537555. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.06.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tzovara, A, Korn, CW and Bach, DR 2018 Human pavlovian fear conditioning conforms to probabilistic learning. PLoS Computational Biology 14: 121. https://doi.org/10.1371/JOURNAL.PCBI.1006243CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van de Cruys, S and Van Dessel, P 2021 Mental distress through the prism of predictive processing theory. Current Opinion in Psychology 41: 107112. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2021.07.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van den Bergh, O, Brosschot, J, Critchley, H, Thayer, JF and Ottaviani, C 2021 Better safe than sorry: A common signature of general vulnerability for psychopathology. Perspectives on Psychological Science 16: 225246. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620950690CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Os, JMC, Mintline, EM, DeVries, TJ and Tucker, CB 2018 Domestic cattle (Bos taurus taurus) are motivated to obtain forage and demonstrate contrafreeloading. PLoS One 13: 116. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0193109CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vicovaro, M 2021 Intuitive physics and cognitive algebra: A review. Revue Europeenne de Psychologie Appliquee 71: 100610. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.erap.2020.100610CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Von Keyserlingk, MAG, Amorim Cestari, A, Franks, B, Fregonesi, JA and Weary, DM 2017 Dairy cows value access to pasture as highly as fresh feed. Scientific Reports 7: 14. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep44953CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, Q, Timberlake, MA, Prall, K and Dwivedi, Y 2017 The recent progress in animal models of depression. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry 77: 99109. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2017.04.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weary, DM 2019 Suffering, agency and the Bayesian mind. In: McMillan, FD (ed) Mental Health and Well-Being in Animals pp 135. CAB eBooks. https://doi.org/10.1079/9781786393401.0156CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiss, JM 1972 Psychological factors in stress and disease. Scientific American 226: 104113. https://doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican0672-104CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wenker, ML, Bokkers, EAM, Lecorps, B, von Keyserlingk, MAG, van Reenen, CG, Verwer, CM and Weary, DM 2020 Effect of cow-calf contact on cow motivation to reunite with their calf. Scientific Reports 10: 15. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-70927-wCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiech, K and Shriver, A 2018 Cognition doesn’t only modulate pain perception; It’s a central component of it. AJOB Neuroscience 9: 196198. https://doi.org/10.1080/21507740.2018.1496180CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winters, JJ, Hardy, LW, Sullivan, JM, Powell, NA, Qutaish, M, Nair, S, Heimann, J, Ghayoor, A, Polyak, I, Chaby, L, Rodriguez, E, Chaar, D, Oscherwitz, J and Liberzon, I 2021 Functional deficit in hippocampal activity during fear extinction recall in the single prolonged-stress model of PTSD in male rats. Behavioural Brain Research 396: 112902. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2020.112902CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wrubel, KM, Barrett, D, Shumake, J, Johnson, SE and Gonzalez-Lima, F 2007 Methylene blue facilitates the extinction of fear in an animal model of susceptibility to learned helplessness. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 87: 209217. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2006.08.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zebunke, M, Kreiser, M, Melzer, N, Langbein, J and Puppe, B 2018 Better, not just more—contrast in qualitative aspects of reward facilitates impulse control in pigs. Frontiers in Psychology 9: 112. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02099CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zentall, TR, Andrews, DM and Case, JP 2019 Contrast between what is expected and what occurs increases pigeon’s suboptimal choice. Animal Cognition 22: 8187. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-018-1223-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. A graphical representation of how expectations resulting from prior experiences (also termed ‘priors’) are affected by new sensory experiences. Panel A illustrates how the new model (illustrated in green) is a result of animal’s current sensory experiences (shown in yellow) and its former expectations (or priors, shown in blue). The extent to which the new model is updated is affected by the confidence (also termed precision) in expectations (as illustrated in Panel B, where high confidence in expectations results in minor modification to the new model) and by precision of the current sensory experience (shown in Panel C), where high confidence in current sensory inputs results in substantial modification to the new model. Artwork by Ann Sanderson. Figure redrawn from Pezzulo et al. (2019).