Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-m6dg7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-07T21:21:50.248Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 10 - Bridging the Gap

From Textual Representations to the Experiential Level and Back

from Part V - Texts

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 July 2022

Esther Eidinow
Affiliation:
University of Bristol
Armin W. Geertz
Affiliation:
Aarhus Universitet, Denmark
John North
Affiliation:
University College London
Get access

Summary

In the chapter, I explore what may be gained by reading textual instantiations of feelings in light of current knowledge of their underlying emotional counterparts. The overall idea is to bridge the gap between ‘biology’ and ‘culture’ to examine how knowledge about basic biology may contribute to deepen the textual understanding. I seek to illustrate the theoretical discussion by providing a biological emotional reading of Paul’s First Letter to the Corinthians chapter 6. I analyze Paul’s emphasis on baptism as moral cleansing in light of recent research in moral psychology and evolutionary biology on the Lady Macbeth complex. The argument is that there is an intrinsic relationship between the use of water and cognitive notions of cleanness, purity, dirt, moral filth, and impurity. These insights may be used to cast further light on Paul’s argument, thus illustrating the overall point that historians of religion, ancient historians, and classicists will benefit considerably by including knowledge from life sciences in the historical analyses.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2022

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Annas, J. 1992. Hellenistic Philosophy of Mind. Berkeley and Los Angeles, CA.Google Scholar
Becker, A. and Reed, A. Y.. eds. 2003. The Ways that Never Parted: Jews and Christians in Late Antiquity and the Early Middle Ages. Tübingen.Google Scholar
Boyarin, D. 2004. Borderlines. The Partition of Judaeo-Christianity. Philadelphia, PA.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cairns, D. 2017. ‘Horror, Pity, and the Visual in Ancient Greek Aesthetics’, in Cairns, D. and Nelis, D. P., eds. Emotions in the Classical World: Methods, Approaches, and Directions, 5378. Stuttgart.Google Scholar
Cairns, D. and Fulkerson, L.. 2015. ‘Introduction’, in Cairns, D. and Fulkerson, L., eds. Emotions between Greece and Rome, 122. London.Google Scholar
Cairns, D. and Nelis, D. P.. 2017. ‘Introduction’, in Cairns, D. and Nelis, D. P., eds. Emotions in the Classical World: Methods, Approaches, and Directions, 718. Stuttgart.Google Scholar
Carr, E. D., Kever, A., and Winkielman, P.. 2018. ‘Embodiment of Emotion and Its Situated Nature’, in Newen, A., De Bruin, L. and Gallagher, S., eds. Oxford Handbook of 4E Cognition, 529551. Oxford.Google Scholar
Caston, R. and Kaster, R. A., eds. 2016. Hope, Joy, and Affection in the Classical World. Oxford.Google Scholar
Chaniotis, A. and Ducrey, P.. 2013. ‘Approaching Ancient Emotions in Greek and Roman History and Culture: An Introduction’, in Chaniotis, A. and Ducrey, P., eds. Unveiling Emotions II. Emotions in Greece and Rome. Texts, Images, Material Culture, 914. Stuttgart.Google Scholar
Chapman, H. A. and Anderson, A. K.. 2013. ‘Things Rank and Gross in Nature: A Review and Synthesis of Moral Disgust’, Psychological Bulletin 139(2): 300327.Google Scholar
Colombetti, G. 2018. ‘Enacting Affectivity’, in Newen, A., De Bruin, L., and Gallagher, S., eds. Oxford Handbook of 4E Cognition, 571587. Oxford.Google Scholar
Czachesz, I. 2012. The Grotesque Body in Early Christian Discourse: Hell, Scatology, and Metamorphosis. London and New York, NY.Google Scholar
Czachesz, I. 2016. ‘Religious Experience behind the Account of Isaiah’s Ascent to Heaven: Insights from Cognitive Science’, in J. N. Bremmer, T. N. Karmann, and T. R. Nicklas, eds. The Ascension of Isaiah, 235257. Leuven,Google Scholar
Czachesz, I. 2017. Cognitive Science and the New Testament: A New Approach to Early Christian Research. Oxford.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. 1994. Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain. New York, NY.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. 2003. Looking for Spinoza: Joy, Sorrow and the Feeling Brain. London.Google Scholar
Delton, A. W., Petersen, M. B., and Robertson, T. E.. 2018. ‘Partisan Goals, Emotions, and Political Mobilization: The Role of Motivated Reasoning in Pressuring Others to Vote’, The Journal of Politics 80(3): 890902.Google Scholar
Durkheim, É. 1912. Les Formes élémentaires de la vie religieuse. Le système totémique en Australie. Paris.Google Scholar
Earp, B. D., Everett, J. A. C., Madva, E. N., and Hamlin, J. K.. 2014. ‘Out, Damned Spot: Can the “Macbeth Effect” Be Replicated?’, Basic and Applied Social Psychology 36(1): 9198.Google Scholar
Eco, U. 1979. A Theory of Semiotics. Bloomington, IN.Google Scholar
Eco, U. 1990. The Limits of Interpretation, Bloomington and Indianapolis, IN.Google Scholar
Eidinow, E. 2016. Envy, Poison and Death: Women on Trial in Classical Athens. Oxford.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. 2003. Emotions Revealed. Understanding Faces and Feelings. London.Google Scholar
Fayard, J. V., Bassi, A. K., Bernstein, D. M, and Roberts, B. W.. 2009. ‘Is Cleanliness Next to Godliness? Dispelling Old Wives’ Tales: Failure to Replicate Zhong and Liljenquist (2006)’, Journal of Articles in Support of the Null Hypothesis 6(2): 2129.Google Scholar
Haidt, J. 2012. The Righteous Mind. Why Good People Are Divided by Politics and Religion. London.Google Scholar
Harkins, A. K. 2015a. ‘The Emotional Re-experiencing of the Hortatory Narratives Found in the Admonition Section of the Damascus Document’, Dead Sea Discoveries 22(3): 285307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harkins, A. K. 2015b. ‘A Phenomenological Study of Penitential Elements and Their Strategic Arousal of Emotion in the Qumran Hodayot (1QH cols. 1[?]-8)’, in Egger-Wenzel, R. and Reif, S. C., eds. Ancient Jewish Prayers and Emotions: A Study of the Emotions Associated with Prayer in the Jewish and Related Literature of the Second Temple Period, 297316. Berlin.Google Scholar
Harkins, A. K. 2016a. ‘Ritual Mourning in Daniel’s Interpretation of Jeremiah’s Prophecy’, The Journal of Cognitive Historiography 2(1): 1432.Google Scholar
Harkins, A. K. 2016b. ‘The Pro-social Role of Grief in Ezra’s Penitential Prayer’, Biblical Interpretation 24(4–5): 466491.Google Scholar
Harkins, A. K. 2017. ‘How Should We Feel about the Teacher of Righteousness?’, in Feldman, A., Cioata, M., and Hempel, C., eds. Is There a Text in this Cave? Studies in the Textuality of the Dead Sea Scrolls in Honour of George J. Brooke, 493514. Leiden.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harkins, A. K. 2020. ‘Emotion and Law in the Book of Baruch’, in Schmitz, B. and De Troyer, K., eds. The Early Reception of the Torah, 4969. Berlin.Google Scholar
Hobson, R. P. 2018. ‘Thinking and Feeling: A Social-Developmental Perspective’, in Newen, A., De Bruin, L., and Gallagher, S., eds. Oxford Handbook of 4E Cognition, 553569. Oxford.Google Scholar
Kaster, R. A. 2005. Emotion, Restraint, and Community in Ancient Rome. Oxford.Google Scholar
Kazen, T. 2011. Emotions in Biblical Law: A Cognitive Science Approach. Sheffield.Google Scholar
Konstan, D. 1997. Friendship in the Classical World. Cambridge.Google Scholar
Konstan, D. 2007. The Emotions of the Ancient Greek: Studies in Aristotle and Classical Literature. Toronto.Google Scholar
Konstan, D. 2010. ‛Ancient Rhetoric and Emotion’, in Worthington, I., ed. Companion to Greek Rhetoric, 411425. Oxford.Google Scholar
Konstan, D. 2012. Before Forgiveness. The Origins of a Moral Idea. Cambridge.Google Scholar
Konstan, D. 2015. ‘Emotions and Morality: The View from Classical Antiquity,’ Topoi 34: 401407.Google Scholar
Larson, J. 2016. Understanding Greek Religion. London.Google Scholar
Lateiner, D. and Spatharas, D.. 2017. ‘Introduction: Ancient and Modern Modes of Understanding Disgust’, in Lateiner, D. and Spatharas, D., eds. The Ancient Emotion of Disgust, 142. Oxford.Google Scholar
LeDoux, J. 1996. The Emotional Brain: The Mysterious Underpinnings of Emotional Life. New York, NY.Google Scholar
Lee, S. W. S. and Schwarz, N.. 2010. ‘Washing Away Postdecisional Dissonance’, Science 328 (7 May): 709.Google Scholar
Luomanen, P., Pyysiäinen, I., , I., and Uro, R., eds. 2007. Explaining Christian Origins and Early Judaism. Leiden.Google Scholar
Martin, D. B. 1995. The Corinthian Body. New Haven, CT.Google Scholar
Martin, L. H. and Wiebe, D., eds. 2017. Religion Explained? The Cognitive Science of Religion after Twenty-Five Years. London.Google Scholar
Mitchell, A. C. 1993. ‘Rich and Poor in the Courts of Corinth. Litigiousness and Status in 1 Corinthians 6.1-11’, New Testament Studies 39: 562586.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, M. M. 1991. Paul and the Rhetoric of Reconciliation: An Exegetical Investigation of the Language and Composition of 1 Corinthians. Tübingen.Google Scholar
Nemeroff, C. and Rozin, P., 2000. ‘The Makings of the Magical Mind: The Nature and Function of Sympathetic Magical Thinking’, in Rosengren, K. S., Johnson, C. N., and Harris, P. L., eds. Imagining the Impossible: Magical, Scientific, and Religious Thinking in Children, 134. Cambridge.Google Scholar
Nussbaum, M. 1994. The Therapy of Desire. Theory and Practice in Hellenistic Ethics. Princeton, NJ.Google Scholar
Petersen, A. K. 2004. ‘Paraenesis in Pauline Scholarship and in Paul – An Intricate Relationship’, in Starr, J. and Engberg-Pedersen, T., eds. Early Christian Paraenesis in Context, 267295. Berlin and Boston, MA.Google Scholar
Petersen, A. K. 2005. ‘At the End of the Road: Reflections on a Popular Scholarly Metaphor’, in Ådna, J., ed. The Formation of the Church, 4572. Tübingen.Google Scholar
Petersen, A. K. 2019. ‘Ritual and Texts’, in Uro, R., Day, J. J., DeMaris, R. E., and Roitto, R., eds. The Oxford Handbook of Early Christian Ritual, 370387. Oxford.Google Scholar
Petersen, A. K. 2022. ‘The Ritual Efficacy of Water and Imposition of Hands in the Context of Rituals of Initiation’, in Urizzi, P., ed. Nati due volte: segreto e iniziazione. Turin (forthcoming).Google Scholar
Petersen, M. B., Giessing, A., and Nielsen, J.. 2015. ‘Physiological Responses and Partisan Bias: Beyond Self-reported Measures of Party Identification’, PLoS ONE 10(5): e0126922.Google Scholar
Plamper, J. 2015. The History of Emotions. An Introduction. Oxford.Google Scholar
Rozin, P. and Nemeroff, C. 1990. ‘The Laws of Sympathetic Magic: A Psychological Analysis of Similarity and Contagion’, in Stigler, J. W., Shweder, R. A., and Herdt, G., eds. Cultural Psychology: Essays on Comparative Human Development, 205232. Cambridge.Google Scholar
Sanders, E. and Johncock, M. 2016. ‘Introduction’, in Sanders, E. and Johncock, M., eds. Emotion and Persuasion in Classical Antiquity, 1324. Stuttgart.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schäfer, P. 2010. Die Geburt des Judentums aus dem Geist des Christentums. Tübingen.Google Scholar
Shakespeare, W. 1996. The First Folio of Shakespeare. Based on Folios in the Folger Shakespeare Collection (2nd ed.). New York, NY and London.Google Scholar
Siev, J., Zuckerman, S. E., and Siev, J. J.. 2018. ‘The Relationship between Immorality and Cleansing: A Meta-Analysis of the Macbeth Effect’, Social Psychology 49(5): 303309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smail, D. L. 2008.On Deep History and the Brain. Berkeley, CA and London.Google Scholar
Stephan, A. 2018. ‘Critical Note: 3E’s Are Sufficient, but Don’t Forget the D’, in Newen, A., De Bruin, L., and Gallagher, S., eds. Oxford Handbook of 4E Cognition, 607619. Oxford.Google Scholar
Westen, D. 2007. The Political Brain: The Role of Emotion in Deciding the Fate of the Nation. New York, NY.Google Scholar
Zhong, C.-B. and Liljenquist, K.. 2006. ‘Washing Away Your Sins: Threatened Morality and Physical Cleansing’, Science 313(5792): 14511452.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×