Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-94fs2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-08T06:33:45.732Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

3 - Invading New Environments: A Mechanistic Framework Linking Motor Diversity and Cognition to Establishment Success

from Part I - Behaviour and the Invasion Process

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 October 2016

Judith S. Weis
Affiliation:
Rutgers University, New Jersey
Daniel Sol
Affiliation:
National Spanish Research Council (CSIC)
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2016

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Allen, T. and Clarke, J. (2005). Social learning of food preferences by white-tailed ptarmigan chicks. Animal Behaviour, 70, 305310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allman, J., McLaughlin, T. and Hakeem, A. (1993). Brain weight and life-span in primate species. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 90, 118122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amiel, J.J., Tingley, R. and Shine, R. (2011). Smart moves: effects of relative brain size on establishment success of invasive amphibians and reptiles. PLoS ONE, 6, e18277.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arcediano, F., Escobar, M. and Miller, R.R. (2003). Temporal integration and temporal backward associations in human and nonhuman subjects. Learning and Behavior, 31, 242256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benson-Amram, S. and Holekamp, K.E. (2012). Innovative problem solving by wild spotted hyenas. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 279, doi: 10.1098/rspb.(2012).1450.Google ScholarPubMed
Biondi, L.M., , M.S. and Vassallo, A.I. (2010). Inter-individual and age differences in exploration, neophobia and problem-solving ability in a neotropical raptor (Milvago chimango). Animal Cognition, 13, 701710.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Björklund, D.F. and Harnishfeger, K.K. (1995). The evolution of inhibition mechanisms and their role in human cognition and behavior. In Interference and Inhibition in Cognition, ed. Dempster, F.N. and Brainerd, C.J., pp. 142169. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Blackburn, T.M., Pyšek, P., Bacher, S., et al. (2011). A proposed unified framework for biological invasions. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 26, 333339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blaisdell, A.P., Sawa, K., Leising, K.J. and Waldmann, M.R. (2006). Causal reasoning in rats. Science, 311, 10201022.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bókony, V., Kulcsár, A., Tóth, Z. and Liker, A. (2012). Personality traits and behavioral syndromes in differently urbanized populations of house sparrows (Passer domesticus). PLoS one, 7, e36639.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boogert, N.J., Reader, S.M. and Laland, K.N. (2006). The relation between social rank, neophobia and individual learning in starlings. Animal Behaviour, 72, 12291239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, G.E., Ferrari, M.C.O., Elvidge, C.K., Ramnarine, I. and Chivers, D.P. (2013). Phenotypically plastic neophobia: a response to variable predation risk. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 280, doi: 10.1098/rspb.(2012).2712.Google ScholarPubMed
Candler, S. and Bernal, X.E. (2014). Differences in neophobia between cane toads from introduced and native populations. Behavioral Ecology, 26, 97104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Changizi, M.A. (2003). Relationship between number of muscles, behavioral repertoire size, and encephalization in mammals. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 220, 157168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chapple, D.G., Simmonds, S.M. and Wong, B.B.M. (2012). Can behavioral and personality traits influence the success of unintentional species introductions? Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 27, 5764.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christidis, L. and Boles, W. (2008). Systematics and Taxonomy of Australian Birds. Collingwood, Australia: CSIRO Publishing.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cnotka, J., Güntürkün, O., Rehkämper, G., Gray, R.D. and Hunt, G.R. (2008). Extraordinary large brains in tool-using New Caledonian crows (Corvus moneduloides). Neuroscience Letters, 433, 241245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deaner, R.O., Barton, R.A. and van Schaik, C.P. (2002). Primate brains and life histories: renewing the connection. In Primate Life Histories and Socioecology, ed. Kappeler, P. M. and Pereira, M. E. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press, pp. 233265.Google Scholar
Dempster, F.N. (1992). The rise and fall of the inhibitory mechanism: toward a unified theory of cognitive development and aging. Developmental Review, 12, 4575.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dhami, M.K. and Nagle, B. (2009). Review of the biology and ecology of the common myna (Acridotheres tristis) and some implications for management of this invasive species. Report. Auckland, New Zealand: Pacific Invasives Initiatives, pp. 128.Google Scholar
Diquelou, M., Griffin, A.S. and Sol, D. (2015). Solving new foraging problems: Motor processes are key to behavioural innovation in birds. Behavioral Ecology, doi:10.1093/beheco/arv190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dukas, R. (2004). Evolutionary biology of animal cognition. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics, 35, 347374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duncan, R.P., Blackburn, T.M. and Sol, D. (2003). The ecology of bird introductions. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics, 34, 7198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elias, M.F. (1970). Spatial discrimination reversal learning for mice genetically selected for differing brain size: a supplementary report. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 30, 239245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fanselow, M.S. (2000). Contextual fear, Gestalt memories, and the hippocampus. Behavioural Brain Research, 110, 7381.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feare, C.J. (2010). The use of Starlicide® in preliminary trials to control invasive common myna Acridotheres tristis populations on St Helena and Ascension islands, Atlantic Ocean. Conservation Evidence, 7, 5261.Google Scholar
Galef, B.G.J. (1996). Social enhancement of food preferences in Norway rats: A brief review. In Social Learning in Animals: The Roots of Culture, ed. Heyes, C.M. and Galef, B.G.J. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, pp. 4964.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaser, C. and Schlaug, G. (2003). Gray matter differences between musicians and nonmusicians. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 999, 514517.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenberg, R.S. and Mettke-Hofmann, C. (2001). Ecological aspects of neophilia and neophobia in birds. Current Ornithology, 16, 119178.Google Scholar
Griffin, A.S. (2004). Social learning about predators: a review and prospectus. Learning and Behavior, 32, 131140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Griffin, A.S. (2008). Social learning in Indian mynahs, Acridotheres tristis: the role of distress calls. Animal Behaviour, 75, 7989.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffin, A.S. (2009). Temporal limitations on social learning of novel predators by Indian mynahs, Acridotheres tristis. Ethology, 115, 287295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffin, A.S. (2010). Learning and conservation. In Encyclopedia of Animal Behavior, Vol. 2, ed. Breed, M.D. and Moore, J. Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 259264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffin, A.S. (2016). Innovativeness as an emergent property: a new alignment of comparative and experimental research on animal innovation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0544.Google Scholar
Griffin, A.S. and Boyce, H.M. (2009). Indian mynahs, Acridotheres tristis, learn about dangerous places by observing the fate of others. Animal Behaviour, 78, 7984.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffin, A.S. and Diquelou, M. (2015). Innovative problem solving in birds: a cross-species comparison of two highly successful Passerines. Animal Behaviour, 100, 8494.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffin, A.S. and Guez, D. (2014). Innovation and problem solving: a review of common mechanisms. Behavioural Processes, 109, 121134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Griffin, A.S. and Guez, D. (2016). Bridging the gap between cross-taxon and within-species analy-ses of behavioral innovations in birds: making sense of discrepant cognition–innovation relationships and the role of motor diversity. In Advances in the Study of Behavior, ed. Naguib, M., et al. New York, NY: Academic Press, pp. 1–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffin, A.S. and Haythorpe, K. (2011). Learning from watching alarmed demonstrators: does the cause of alarm matter? Animal Behaviour, 81, 11631169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffin, A.S., Boyce, H.M. and MacFarlane, G.R. (2010). Social learning about places: observers may need to detect both social alarm and its cause in order to learn. Animal Behaviour, 79, 459465.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffin, A.S., Lermite, F., Perea, M. and Guez, D. (2013). To innovate or not: contrasting effects of social groupings on safe and risky foraging in Indian mynahs. Animal Behaviour, 86, 12911300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffin, A.S., Diquelou, M. and Perea, M. (2014). Innovative problem solving in birds: a key role of motor diversity. Animal Behaviour, 92, 221227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gronenberg, W. and Couvillon, M.J. (2010). Brain composition and olfactory learning in honey bees. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 93, 435443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guez, D. and Audley, C. (2013). Transitive or not: a critical appraisal of transitive inference in animals. Ethology, 119, 703726.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Healy, S.D. and Rowe, C. (2007). A critique of comparative studies of brain size. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 274, 453464.Google ScholarPubMed
Healy, S.D. and Rowe, C. (2013). Costs and benefits of evolving a larger brain: doubts over the evidence that large brains lead to better cognition. Animal Behaviour, 86, e1e3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klopfer, P.H. 1967. Behavioural stereotypy in birds. Wilson Bulletin, 79, 290300.Google Scholar
Kotrschal, A., Rogell, B., Bundsen, A., et al. (2013a). Artificial selection on relative brain size in the guppy reveals costs and benefits of evolving a larger brain. Current Biology, 23, 168171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kotrschal, A., Rogell, B., Bundsen, A., et al. (2013b). The benefit of evolving a larger brain: big-brained guppies perform better in a cognitive task. Animal Behaviour, 86, e4e6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laland, K.N. (2004). Social learning strategies. Learning and Behavior, 32, 414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lissek, S., Diekamp, B. and Güntürkün, O. (2002). Impaired learning of a color reversal task after NMDA receptor blockade in the pigeon (Columbia livia) associative forebrain (neostriatum caudolaterale). Behavioral Neuroscience, 116, 523529.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lowe, S., Browne, M., Boudjelas, S. and de Porter, M. (2000). 100 of the World's Worst Invasive Alien Species. A Selection from the Global Invasive Species Database. Auckland: The Invasive Species Specialist Group (ISSG), a specialist group of the Species Survival Commission (SSC) of the World Conservation Union (IUCN).Google Scholar
MacLean, E.L., Hare, B., Nunn, C.L. et al. (2014). The evolution of self-control. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 111, E2140–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mangalam, M. and Singh, M. (2013). Flexibility in food extraction techniques in urban free-ranging bonnet macaques, Macaca radiata. PLoS ONE, 8, e85497.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marples, N.M. and Roper, T.J. (1997). Response of domestic chicks to methyl anthranilate odour. Animal Behaviour, 53, 12631270.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, L.B. and Fitzgerald, L. (2005). A taste for novelty in invading house sparrows, Passer domesticus. Behavioral Ecology, 16, 702707.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marzluff, J.M., Walls, J., Cornell, H.N., Withey, J.C. and Craig, D.P. (2010). Lasting recognition of threatening people by wild American crows. Animal Behaviour, 79, 699707.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mettke-Hofmann, C. (2007). Object exploration of garden and Sardinian warblers peaks in spring. Ethology, 113, 174182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mettke-Hofmann, C., Winkler, H. and Leisler, B. (2002). The significance of ecological factors for exploration and neophobia in parrots. Ethology, 108, 249272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mettke-Hofmann, C., Ebert, C., Schmidt, T., Steiger, S. and Stieb, S. (2005). Personality traits in resident and migratory warbler species. Behaviour, 142, 13571375.Google Scholar
Mettke-Hofmann, C., Lorentzen, S., Schlicht, E., Schneider, J. and Werner, F. (2009). Spatial neophilia and spatial neophobia in resident and migratory warblers (Sylvia). Ethology, 115, 482492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miranda, A.C., Schielzeth, H., Sonntag, T. and Partecke, J. (2013). Urbanization and its effects on personality traits: a result of microevolution or phenotypic plasticity? Global Change Biology, 19, 26342644.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nottebohm, F. (1981). A brain for all seasons: cyclical anatomical changes in song control nuclei of the canary brain. Science, 214, 13681370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Overington, S.E., Cauchard, L., Côté, K.-A. and Lefebvre, L. (2011). Innovative foraging behaviour in birds: what characterizes an innovator? Behavioural Processes, 87, 274285.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penfield, W. and Rasmussen, T. (1950). The Cerebral Cortex of Man: A Clinical Study of Localization of Function. Oxford, UK: Macmillan Edn.Google Scholar
Phillips, B.L. and Suarez, S.D. (2012). The role of behavioural variation in the invasion of new areas. In Behavioural Responses to a Changing World: Mechanisms and Consequences, ed. Candolin, U. and Wong, B.B.M. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, pp. 190200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rescorla, R.A. (2014). Pavlovian Second-Order Conditioning. Studies in Associative Learning (Psychology Revivals). New York: Psychology Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roth, G. and Dicke, U. (2005). Evolution of the brain and intelligence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 250257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sandel, A.A., MacLean, E.L. and Hare, B. (2009). Inhibitory control in an object retrieval task in five lemur species. American Journal of Primatology, 71, 80.Google Scholar
Sanes, J. and Donoghue, J. (2000). Plasticity and primary motor cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 23, 393415.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schuppli, C., Sofia, F. and van Schaik, C.P. (2014). How sociality affects independent exploration: Evidence gathered in two populations of wild orangutans. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 153, 234.Google Scholar
Seed, A., Emery, N. and Clayton, N. (2009). Intelligence in corvids and apes: a case of convergent evolution? Ethology, 115, 401420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seok An, Y., Kriengwatana, B., MacDougall-Shackleton, E., Newman, A. and MacDougall-Shackleton, S. (2011). Social rank, neophobia and observational learning in black-capped chickadees. Behaviour, 148, 5569.Google Scholar
Shettleworth, S.J. (2010). Cognition, Evolution, and Behavior, 2nd edn. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Smaers, J.B. and Soligo, C. (2013). Brain reorganization, not relative brain size, primarily characterizes anthropoid brain evolution. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 280, doi: 10.1098/rspb.2013.0269.Google Scholar
Sol, D. (2007). Do successful invaders exist? Pre-adaptations to novel environments in terrestrial vertebrates. In Biological Invasions, ed. Nentwig, W. Berlin: Springer, pp. 127144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sol, D., Duncan, R.P., Blackburn, T.M., Cassey, P. and Lefebvre, L. (2005). Big brains, enhanced cognition, and response of birds to novel environments. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 102, 54605465.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sol, D., Bacher, S., Reader, S.M. and Lefebvre, L. (2008). Brain size predicts the success of mammal species introduced into novel environments. The American Naturalist, 172 Suppl., S6371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sol, D., Griffin, A.S., Bartomeus, I. and Boyce, H. (2011). Exploring or avoiding novel food resources? The novelty conflict in an invasive bird. PLoS ONE, 6, e19535.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sol, D., Bartomeus, I. and Griffin, A.S. (2012a). The paradox of invasion in birds: competitive superiority or ecological opportunism? Oecologia, 169, 553564.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sol, D., Griffin, A.S. and Barthomeus, I. (2012b). Consumer and motor innovation in the common myna: the role of motivation and emotional responses. Animal Behaviour, 83, 179188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suchail, S., Guez, D. and Belzunces, L.P. (2000). Characteristics of imidacloprid toxicity in two Apis mellifera subspecies. Environmental Toxicology and Chemistry, 19, 19011905.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tebbich, S., Stankewitz, S. and Teschke, I. (2012). The relationship between foraging, learning abilities and neophobia in two species of Darwin's finches. Ethology, 118, 135146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thorndike, E.L. (1898). Animal intelligence: An experimental study of the associative processes in animals. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 2, 11251127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thornton, A. and Samson, J. (2012). Innovative problem solving in wild meerkats. Animal Behaviour, 83, 14591468.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wright, T.F., Eberhard, J.R., Hobson, E.A., Avery, M.L. and Russello, M.A. (2010). Behavioral flexibility and species invasions: the adaptive flexibility hypothesis. Ethology Ecology and Evolution, 22, 393404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×