Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gbm5v Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T06:04:45.428Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The impact of social-environmental factors on IQ in syndromic intellectual developmental disabilities

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 April 2024

Walker S. McKinney
Affiliation:
Department of Behavioral Medicine and Clinical Psychology, Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center, Cincinnati, OH, USA
Desireé N. Williford
Affiliation:
Department of Behavioral Medicine and Clinical Psychology, Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center, Cincinnati, OH, USA Department of Pediatrics, University of Cincinnati College of Medicine, Cincinnati, OH, USA
Leonard Abbeduto
Affiliation:
MIND Institute, University of California Davis, Sacramento, CA, USA Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of California Davis, Sacramento, CA, USA
Lauren M. Schmitt*
Affiliation:
Department of Behavioral Medicine and Clinical Psychology, Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center, Cincinnati, OH, USA Department of Pediatrics, University of Cincinnati College of Medicine, Cincinnati, OH, USA
*
Corresponding author: L. M. Schmitt; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Despite having the same underlying genetic etiology, individuals with the same syndromic form of intellectual developmental disability (IDD) show a large degree of interindividual differences in cognition and IQ. Research indicates that up to 80% of the variation in IQ scores among individuals with syndromic IDDs is attributable to nongenetic effects, including social-environmental factors. In this narrative review, we summarize evidence of the influence that factors related to economic stability (focused on due to its prevalence in existing literature) have on IQ in individuals with syndromic IDDs. We also highlight the pathways through which economic stability is hypothesized to impact cognitive development and drive individual differences in IQ among individuals with syndromic IDDs. We also identify broader social-environmental factors (e.g., social determinants of health) that warrant consideration in future research, but that have not yet been explored in syndromic IDDs. We conclude by making recommendations to address the urgent need for further research into other salient factors associated with heterogeneity in IQ. These recommendations ultimately may shape individual- and community-level interventions and may inform systems-level public policy efforts to promote the cognitive development of and improve the lived experiences of individuals with syndromic IDDs.

Type
Review Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Association for Clinical and Translational Science

Introduction

Intellectual developmental disabilities (IDDs), characterized by substantial limitations in intellectual and adaptive functioning, occur in about 1% of the general population [1]. Approximately 40–60% of IDDs have an identifiable genetic cause [Reference Rauch, Hoyer and Guth2,Reference Patel, Cabral, Ho and Merrick3], with a subset of these IDDs considered syndromic based on the co-occurrence of other clinical features (e.g., facial dysmorphology) [Reference Kaufman, Ayub and Vincent4]. Marked differences in intellectual functioning (hereafter referencing IQ scores, the most common and well-validated index of intellectual functioning) in individuals with syndromic IDDs are driven by a highly penetrant genetic variant; however, the variant does not always necessitate a diagnosis of IDD as IQ scores in the average range are sometimes observed within a given syndrome [Reference Hagerman, Hull and Safanda5]. Although the average range of full-scale IQ (FSIQ) scores differs between syndromes (Table 1), each syndrome’s respective genetic variant generally has a pronounced impact on IQ and serves as the primary contributor to the IDD phenotype.

Table 1. Means and standard deviations of full-scale IQ (FSIQ) for common syndromic forms of intellectual developmental disorder

Many studies are unable to accurately assess FSIQ in patients with severe cognitive impairment due to floor effects and/or behavioral challenges during testing (often instead reporting developmental quotients or deviation IQ that relay only on raw scores). The means reported here, therefore, may reflect an overestimate of the syndrome “population level” FSIQ.

Although the distribution of standardized intellectual abilities in people with syndromic IDDs is, by definition, down-shifted relative to the general population, mounting evidence suggests that IQs within the same form of syndromic IDD (e.g., fragile X syndrome, William syndrome, and Down syndrome) vary considerably across individuals. For some syndromes, the range of observed scores approximates a normal distribution after accounting for floor effects of standardized IQ measures [Reference Moreno-De-Luca, Evans and Boomer6,Reference De Smedt, Devriendt, Fryns, Vogels, Gewillig and Swillen8,Reference Whittington, Holland, Webb, Butler, Clarke and Boer11,Reference Schmitt, Will, Shaffer and Erickson15Reference Couzens, Cuskelly and Jobling17]. Put another way, the population of individuals with the same form of syndromic IDD may have their own “syndrome-specific” down-shifted, nearly normal IQ distribution. This finding suggests that interindividual variability in IQ may be driven by a similar combination of genetic (both pathogenic and familial), environmental (shared and non-shared), interactive (e.g., epigenetic), and random factors, just as in the typically developing (TD) population [Reference Dickens and Flynn18]. We thus propose a “double hit” model that also incorporates measurement of nongenetic, environmental factors that may explain a significant amount of the variability in IQ among people with the same form of syndromic IDD (Fig. 1). This model specifies that the pathogenic genetic variant contributes to the initial large “hit,” or reduction, in IQ, whereas other familial genetic and nongenetic factors contribute to smaller but meaningful IQ differences in either direction.

Figure 1. “Double hit” model. The pathogenic genetic variant contributes to the primary reduction in IQ in people with syndromic IDDs. Secondary genetic, environmental, epigenetic, and random factors contribute to smaller variations in IQ and result in a downshifted, but widely distributed, range of IQ scores. IQ = intelligence quotient; IDD = intellectual developmental disabilities.

Due to the high penetrance of the many genetic variants associated with IDDs, research into factors contributing to individual differences in IQ in syndromic IDDs has primarily emphasized genetic, molecular, or related biological factors. In contrast, relatively little attention has been paid to social-environmental factors (e.g., access to services, home environment, and nutrition) that might contribute to individual differences in IQ among individuals with syndromic IDDs. Two examples are considered to highlight the explanatory power of social-environmental differences. Only 30% of the variance in IQ among individuals with fragile X syndrome (FXS) is accounted for by familial genetic and molecular factors, including absence or reduction in fragile X messenger ribonucleoprotein (FMRP) production [Reference Dyer-Friedman, Glaser and Hessl10,Reference Schmitt, Will, Shaffer and Erickson15,Reference Boggs, Schmitt and McLane19]. Likewise, 17.6% of the variance in IQ in individuals with 16-p11.2 deletion syndrome is accounted for by biparental IQ, or the average of maternal and paternal IQ scores [Reference Moreno-De-Luca, Evans and Boomer6]. Together, these findings indicate that nongenetic factors account for 70–80% of the remaining variance in IQ among individuals with these syndromic IDDs. Although these values underestimate the proportion of variance accounted for by genetic factors due to the underlying assumption that parent–child IQ correlations fully capture heritable genetic effects, these findings suggest that individual differences in IQ among individuals with syndromic IDDs, as in the TD population, are strongly influenced by nongenetic factors. Consistent with studies of physical health and other psychiatric and neurological conditions (e.g., Alzheimer’s)[Reference Majoka and Schimming20], expanding upon the behavioral genetics framework by incorporating the measurement of key social-environmental variables is clearly needed to comprehensively understand and support factors that promote cognitive development. This “bioecological model” has been successfully used to explain how social-environmental factors impact the heritability of intelligence [Reference Bronfenbrenner and Ceci21,Reference Heikura, Taanila and Hartikainen22], and such an approach may help clarify associations between pathogenic genetic variants, social-environmental factors, and IQ in syndromic IDDs.

Recent research has outlined methods for and stressed the importance of conducting more equitable IDD research that recognizes individual social-environmental differences that shape the daily experiences of individuals with IDDs[Reference Kover and Abbeduto23]. The importance of this work has been further underscored by the research priorities of major funding organizations; for example, the National Institutes of Health (NIH) recently announced a new funding mechanism for research on the “exposome,” or “the totality of an individual’s exposures and the body’s response to them”[Reference Stetler24]. This includes both physical and environmental (e.g., toxicants and pollutants) and social-environmental exposures (e.g., socioeconomic conditions, social capital, and discrimination). In the present narrative review, we meet these calls to action by summarizing evidence of the mechanisms through which select social-environmental factors may influence IQ in people with syndromic IDDs.

Procedures for this narrative review included an unstructured review of the scientific literature for studies describing relations between social-environmental factors and IQ for people with syndromic IDDs as well as in other clinical and nonclinical populations when appropriate (WSM and LMS). Highly cited frameworks of social determinants of health (SDOH; “the conditions in the environments where people are born, live, learn, work, play, worship, and age that affect a wide range of health, functioning, and quality-of-life outcomes and risks;” US Department of Health and Human Services [25]) were also reviewed to identify multiple social-environmental domains that are likely relevant to IQ (Fig. 2). However, it should be noted that the influence of many SDOH on IQ has not been thoroughly examined in syndromic IDDs and the dearth of literature prohibited our ability to conduct a systematic review or meta-analysis. Based on available literature, we focus our review and resulting theoretical model on economic stability, or the ability to access resources (i.e., income/financial resources, food, housing, and employment) essential to one’s life and well-being.

Figure 2. Social determinants of health identified by the US Department of Health and Human Services’ Healthy People 2030 Initiative [25]. Domains discussed in the present narrative review are depicted in darkened boxes. Figure created with BioRender.com.

Economic stability has been the predominant factor examined thus far in syndromic IDD research. As a set of SDOH that intersect with other important SDOH domains (e.g., healthcare and quality, education access and quality), clarifying the pathways through which it contributes to differences in IQ may help identify numerous cross-cutting targets for intervention in syndromic IDDs. The goals of the present manuscript are to summarize extant literature (i.e., narrative review) and articulate a theoretical conceptualization (i.e., formulating a conceptual model), two necessary initial steps for designing and conducting research aimed to understand and intervene upon these pathways. We focus on IQ as an outcome in our model because it has been the primary outcome in the majority of studies examining relations between clinical outcomes and social-environmental factors in individuals with syndromic IDDs. Still, we acknowledge the relatively greater importance placed on IQ in the medical literature and in diagnostic processes more generally, compared to other undervalued personal characteristics (e.g., self-determination and adaptive behaviors). We also acknowledge the known cultural biases in IQ test norming[26]. However, we believe the most practical first step is to understand the contributors to differences in IQ given the state of the literature as well as the robust positive associations between IQ and independent living, other cognitive abilities (e.g., executive functions), competitive employment, and quality of life in individuals with IDDs[Reference Wehmeyer and Garner27Reference Erostarbe-Pérez, Reparaz-Abaitua, tínez-Pérez and Magallón-Recalde31]. We also aim to provide guidance for researchers, reviewers, and journal editors by identifying key research priorities: namely, other SDOH that research in the TD population suggests are relevant to cognitive development, but which have been largely unexplored in individuals with syndromic IDDs. Through our identification of salient SDOH that most strongly influence cognitive development in syndromic IDDs and our articulation of an overarching conceptual model linking SDOH to cognitive outcomes, we aim to guide research and practice that may identify innovative, multilevel (e.g., individual and community) intervention targets and inform systems-level advocacy and sociopolitical change.

Pathways through which economic stability influences cognitive development

Although economic stability has been shown to be an important domain within SDOH frameworks, it should be noted that the factors which comprise economic stability vary across models and the scientific literature. As defined by the US Department of Health and Human Services (US HHS) Healthy People 2030 Initiative, economic stability reflects a person’s access to economic resources, including employment opportunities, quality food and nutrition, safe and affordable housing, and a livable income [25]. Conversely, Hill and colleagues identify three domains of economic stability – income and employment stability, family stability, and benefits stability – which collectively support stability in housing, childcare, and access to healthcare [Reference Hill, Romich, Mattingly, Shamsuddin and Wething32]. Other researchers use the related term “economic security” to reflect income stability, medical spending, and accumulated wealth that protects families from large, unexpected expenses or loss of income [Reference Hacker, Huber and Nichols33]. The dearth of research on the influence of many of these economic components on the IQ of individuals with syndromic IDDs precludes the use of a singular organizational framework or definition in this review. Therefore, while we acknowledge the importance of each of the economic factors, we focus our discussion on those areas that have been explicitly studied in syndromic IDDs or the general population and demonstrate associations with IQ.

Access to care

We propose that aspects of economic stability influence IQ in individuals with syndromic IDDs through multiple bidirectional, indirect, and interrelated pathways (Fig. 3). First, economic stability may account for some of the variability in IQ among individuals with syndromic IDDs due to the increased access to diagnostic, intervention, and educational services often afforded by higher income (Fig. 3, left side). Families experiencing poverty may face many barriers to obtaining services, including difficulties with payment and/or reliable transportation, which impede access to early diagnostic assessment and intervention [Reference Kachi, Kato and Kawachi34,Reference Shi and Stevens35]. Similar barriers frequently prevent uninsured or underinsured families from accessing well-child visits [36] that typically serve as the initial referral source for state early intervention programs funded by the Individuals with Disabilities Education Act (IDEA). Limited access to these services may in turn impede gains in IQ fostered by these programs [Reference Ramey and Ramey37Reference Winarni, Schneider, Borodyanskara and Hagerman40] and could account for interindividual differences in IQ among people with syndromic IDDs. Limited service access also may stem from limited service availability, which can be driven by factors related to rurality and/or low provider/service density rather than factors exclusively related to economic stability. In fact, recent work demonstrates that service access is as limited for individuals with IDDs living in nonmetropolitan areas as it is in the general population [Reference Fortney and Tasse41]. Furthermore, access to syndromic IDD clinics, where providers with specialized training dedicated to a specific syndromic IDD deliver specialized care, is limited by the scarcity of these clinics and their location almost exclusively in major metropolitan areas. Thus, higher family income facilitates access (i.e., travel and affordability of services) to these specialty clinics [Reference Kidd, Raspa and Clark42,Reference Joslyn, Berger and Skotko43] and may promote cognitive gains through specialized service access.

Figure 3. Potential pathway model for the impact of economic instability on IQ in individuals with IDDs. Note that many effects are bidirectional (e.g., economic instability limits access to reliable transportation which further fuels economic instability). Solid boxes and arrows reflect factors and pathways, respectively, that have been examined in individuals with syndromic IDDs. Dashed boxes and arrows reflect hypothesized factors and pathways that are presently unexplored in syndromic IDDs. IQ = intelligence quotient; IDD = intellectual developmental disabilities.

Higher income also may facilitate increased educational opportunities for parents (e.g., increased college attendance) [Reference Tompsett and Knoester44] and children (e.g., greater availability and higher quality of childcare, school, and behavioral intervention programs) [Reference Cloney, Cleveland, Hattie and Tayler45,Reference Queralt and Witte46] that drive cognitive growth. For example, access to higher-quality preschool programming and early behavioral intervention improves IQ in children with and without IDDs [Reference Meghir, Attanasio and Jervis47,Reference Peters-Scheffer, Didden, Mulders and Korzilius48]. Family income also is positively associated with subjective and objective indicators of behavioral health service quality [Reference Strickland, Jones, Newacheck, Bethell, Blumberg and Kogan49,Reference Hare, Pratt, Burton, Bromley and Emerson50], indicating that the quality of services, in addition to service access, may mediate the association between economic factors and IQ.

Importantly, the impact of economic stability on service access and subsequent changes in IQ also intersects with systemic racial and ethnic discrimination and bias: people from marginalized racial and ethnic backgrounds are disproportionately more likely than others in the population to experience poverty and to be un- or under-insured, ultimately limiting access to these high-quality services [36,51]. As one example in the context of syndromic IDDs, Black families face systemic barriers in accessing specialized neurodevelopmental healthcare services [Reference Pearson, Meadan, Malone and tin52] and, therefore, they receive their child’s diagnosis of FXS later in life [Reference Crawford, Meadows and Newman53]. This IDD-specific finding is consistent with the broader pattern of lower rates of confirmed genetic diagnoses in people from marginalized racial and ethnic backgrounds linked to implicit bias in providers, limited availability of specialized genetic diagnostic services, and higher rates of inconclusive genetic testing results [Reference Fraiman and Wojcik54]. These barriers delay access to treatment options that are more readily available following receipt of a medical diagnosis (e.g., targeted medical management for issues known to be associated with the specific genetic variant) that would foster positive cognitive and adaptive development.

Home environment, home enrichment, and family stress

Economic instability and the stress it creates may significantly influence the family home environment, including home enrichment and caregiver–child interactions, both of which impact cognitive development (Fig. 3, center). The quality of the family’s home environment promotes cognitive development through the provision of enriching learning opportunities (e.g., trips outside the home, access to early educational materials, and role-playing toys) and thus may account for interindividual differences in IQ in individuals with syndromic IDDs. Several studies of FXS have examined the impact of the home environment on cognitive outcomes in children. For example, improved quality of the home environment is associated with higher IQ, although this association is substantially stronger in boys with FXS than it is in girls with FXS [Reference Dyer-Friedman, Glaser and Hessl10]. Similarly, a more enriching home environment is associated with greater adaptive skills in boys, but not girls, with FXS [Reference Glaser, Hessl and Dyer-Friedman55]. Sex (as assigned at birth) differences in the association between home environment and developmental outcomes may be related to sex-associated genotypic and phenotypic differences specific to FXS, wherein females with FXS typically have less pronounced deficits due to X-inactivation [Reference Bartholo, Lee, Bruno, Lightbody and Reiss56]. Furthermore, Glaser and colleagues hypothesized that families of boys with FXS, relative to families of girls with FXS, may be more likely to enrich their home environment out of necessity due to their boys’ increased support needs [Reference Glaser, Hessl and Dyer-Friedman55]. In other words, differences in the home environment may have less of an impact on females with FXS because their stronger cognitive and adaptive skills, relative to males with FXS, may not necessitate as significant of environmental intervention (e.g., behavioral therapy and changes to home structure). Future work examining the association between home environment and IQ, therefore, should be mindful of the moderating impact of biological factors, including sex assigned at birth (especially if related to phenotypic differences for the syndrome of interest), and cognitive and adaptive behavioral strengths.

Economic instability also contributes to family stress, which may influence IQ through several pathways. The direct impact of family stress due to economic hardship likely begins prenatally, as maternal stress is associated with birth complications (e.g., preterm birth and low birth weight) linked to developmental delays and lower IQ [Reference Wadhwa, Entringer, Buss and Lu57,Reference Wadhwa, Sandman, Porto, Dunkel-Schetter and Garite58]. One neurodevelopmental mechanism underpinning lower IQ may be reduced postnatal brain volume resulting from maternal stress and socioeconomic disadvantage occurring during pregnancy [Reference Triplett, Lean and Parikh59]. Long-term physiological stress or “allostatic load,” also may exacerbate the detrimental effects other factors have on IQ, as has been demonstrated in studies of environmental pollutants [Reference Vishnevetsky, Tang and Chang60]. The influence of economic stability on family stress and child IQ likely continues postnatally. Heightened parental stress and the increased parental attention required to fulfill the family’s basic needs may influence parent–child interactions, which play a crucial role in cognitive development [Reference Perkins, Finegood and Swain61,Reference McLoyd62], as demonstrated in the general population [Reference Jackson, Choi and Preston63]. For example, caregivers of youth with IDDs are more likely to experience unemployment [Reference Seltzer, Greenberg, Floyd, Pettee and Hong64], possibly due to barriers related to the increased caregiving needs inherent in raising a child with IDD (e.g., safety monitoring, attending frequent appointments, limited availability of other skilled caregivers, or respite care). Unemployment subsequently contributes to greater family stress and may influence the nature of caregiver–child interactions. Consistent with this hypothesized pathway linking parent–child relational factors, stress, and child IQ outcomes, several studies indicate that lower family distress and closer parental relationships are associated with greater verbal and nonverbal IQ in individuals with FXS [Reference Del Hoyo Soriano, Thurman, Harvey, Ted Brown and Abbeduto65] as well as stronger verbal skills in individuals with FXS or DS [Reference Del Hoyo Soriano, Thurman, Harvey, Kover and Abbeduto66]. Specifically, del Hoyo Soriano and colleagues found that both closer mother–child relationships and lower maternal distress predicted greater IQ scores in a cross-sectional sample of youth with FXS, whereas genetic/biological factors (e.g., higher FMRP levels and lower proportion of affected chromosomes) predicted longitudinal gains in nonverbal IQ [Reference Del Hoyo Soriano, Thurman, Harvey, Ted Brown and Abbeduto65]. Similarly, a more responsive parenting style is associated with stronger language skills in individuals with FXS [Reference Warren, Brady, Sterling, Fleming and quis67].

The impact of caregiver stress due to economic instability on child IQ may be especially pronounced for verbal skills due to the unique impact caregiver stress has on caregiver–child interactions important for language development [Reference Justice, Jiang and Purtell68,Reference Fowler, McGrath and Henry69]. Specifically, more frequent reciprocal parent–child verbal interactions promote language development and verbal problem-solving through parental scaffolding and modeling [Reference Justice, Jiang and Purtell68Reference Song, Spier and Tamis-Lemonda72]. These interactions are amenable to intervention in caregivers of youth with neurodevelopmental disabilities, including FXS and other forms of syndromic IDDs [Reference Wang, Loh, Tian and Chen73Reference Bullard, McDuffie and Abbeduto75], highlighting the potential efficacy of caregiver-mediated interventions for improving IQ given the malleability of caregiver behaviors through behavioral interventions.

Importantly, family stress is affected by other broader sociocultural and systemic factors, such as racial and ethnic discrimination. Discrimination-induced stress begins to influence neurodevelopmental and health outcomes, such as birth weight, that are linked to differences in IQ prenatally [Reference Spann, Alleyne and Holland76,Reference Earnshaw, Rosenthal and Lewis77] and therefore may have long-lasting impacts on cognition in individuals with syndromic IDDs whose birth mothers hold marginalized racial and ethnic identities. Further, some stress-related pathways may be cyclical. For example, marital stress in families of youth with neurodevelopmental disabilities is strongly influenced by economic disadvantage and is linked to changes in family makeup (e.g., divorce and shifts to a single-parent household) [Reference Hatton, Emerson, Graham, Blacher and Llewellyn78], which may further contribute to family-level stress due to increased caregiving demands [Reference Jackson, Choi and Preston63]. These added demands may subsequently influence caregiver–child interactions.

It is critical that measures of the quality of the home environment capture differences in both home enrichment and parent–child interactions. The Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (HOME), for example, includes a qualitative assessment of the availability of enriching toys and media, frequency and types of visits outside the home, home factors (e.g., light and cleanliness), parental interactions with the child (e.g., discipline, verbal communication, and keeping child in view), child responsibilities and expectations, and other indicators of a safe and nurturing home [Reference Bradley79]. It also is important to recognize that other social-environmental factors may impact home environments and indirectly influence cognitive development. For example, caregiver education, extended family support, mental health literacy, and incarceration collectively or in isolation influence the home environment and parent–child interactions [Reference Garrett, Ng'andu and Ferron80Reference Kamp Dush, Schmeer and Taylor83]. The influence of these factors is largely unexplored in syndromic IDDs, and research into these areas is needed to clarify additional targets for multilevel and systemic interventions, including public policy solutions. Ultimately, the collective influence of these home factors occurring across multiple levels highlights the importance of individual-, family-, and systems-level strategies that foster economic success, reduce family stress, and promote positive caregiver–child interactions that contribute to gains in IQ among individuals with syndromic IDDs.

Nutritional differences

Economic stability may influence differences in IQ in individuals with syndromic IDDs due to the impact of high-quality nutrition and food security on neurodevelopment (Fig. 3, right side). As with stress, the influence of nutrition begins prenatally: better maternal nutrition during pregnancy is associated with greater childhood IQ at 8 years of age in the TD population [Reference Barker, Kirkham, Ng and Jensen84]. Adequate maternal nutrition (e.g., micronutrient consumption and protein intake) improves birth outcomes such as birth weight [Reference shall, Abrams and Barbour85] and, through positive effects on neurodevelopment, may contribute to greater IQ. Access to healthcare and health literacy both likely intersect with prenatal nutritional outcomes, such that mothers experiencing barriers to accessing prenatal healthcare appointments (e.g., transportation [Reference Heaman, Moffatt and Elliott86] and insurance [Reference Nothnagle, chi, Egerter and Braveman87]) are less likely to receive nutritional guidance during their pregnancy. Although no study to date has specifically looked at postnatal relations between food security and IQ in syndromic IDDs, food insecurity is known to increase the likelihood of developmental delay even after accounting for household income [Reference Rose-Jacobs, Black and Casey88]. Infantile malnutrition also is associated with lower IQ scores in childhood and adulthood [Reference Waber, Bryce, Girard, Zichlin, Fitzmaurice and Galler89,Reference Belsky, Moffitt, Arseneault, Melchior and Caspi90]. We hypothesize that these same relations exist for individuals with syndromic IDDs, but this is an important area of consideration for future research.

Bidirectional effects

Many of the above-hypothesized pathways influencing IQ likely are bidirectional, and many factors are interrelated and intersectional (Fig. 3) [Reference Lee, Zhao, Reesor-Oyer, Cepni and Hernandez91,Reference Johnson, Palakshappa, Basu, Seligman and Berkowitz92]. For example, greater food insecurity is associated with higher healthcare expenditures, likely due to the negative impact of food insecurity on health, and higher healthcare expenditures are associated with a greater likelihood of being food-insecure, likely due to income previously allotted for food instead being allotted to pay for medical care [Reference Johnson, Palakshappa, Basu, Seligman and Berkowitz92]. Similarly, caregiver stress driven by economic instability may cyclically contribute to greater economic instability due to the impact of caregiver mental health on employment and income, although it is important to note that socioeconomic disadvantage itself uniquely limits neurodevelopmental outcomes (i.e., above and beyond its role in generating stress) [Reference Triplett, Lean and Parikh59]. Longitudinal studies indicate that suboptimal mental health symptoms predict and are predicted by unemployment and loss of income [Reference Butterworth, Leach, Pirkis and Kelaher93Reference Whooley, Kiefe and Chesney95]; in other words, it is harder to find and maintain employment while under significant stress and while living with anxiety and depression [Reference Whooley, Kiefe and Chesney95], and difficulties finding and maintaining employment subsequently contribute to stress, anxiety, and depression [Reference Crowe and Butterworth96,Reference Diette, Goldsmith, Hamilton and Darity97]. Together, these findings suggest that a constellation of interrelated pathways, including the bidirectional impact of unemployment/underemployment and loss of income on access to intervention services, stress and mental health, and home environment, collectively contribute to individual differences in IQ among individuals with syndromic IDDs. The impact of individual and collective facets of economic stability on IQ is wide-ranging and likely transdiagnostic, highlighting the critical importance of developing interventions and public policy solutions that drive systems-level change across and within social-environmental contexts and which operate independent of an individual’s specific genetic diagnosis.

Evidence for the association between economic stability and IQ in syndromic IDDs

Although possible mechanisms extend beyond those described in this article, the developmental benefits (e.g., higher IQ) of economic stability through the mechanisms described above are well established in the general population. However, these pathways have not yet been comprehensively examined in individuals with syndromic IDDs. However, several key studies have examined relations between IQ and global measures of economic stability that correlate with factors more directly linked to differences in IQ in individuals with syndromic IDDs, although results have been inconsistent [Reference Dyer-Friedman, Glaser and Hessl10,Reference Jenni, Fintelmann, Caflisch, Latal, Rousson and Chaouch98Reference Skinner, Hooper and Hatton102]. Equivocal findings from studies investigating the association between child IQ and economic factors in syndromic IDDs are likely driven by (1) study-specific measurement differences, (2) the IQ domain assessed (e.g., verbal, nonverbal, or full-scale), and (3) age and developmental effects.

These studies have primarily used four different indices to measure economic stability, each with relative strengths and weaknesses. First, economic stability has been directly assessed using continuous measures of adjusted gross family income controlling for area cost of living [Reference Dyer-Friedman, Glaser and Hessl10]. This is a useful measure due to ease of interpretation and standardization across regions but fails to account for important contextual factors like accumulated wealth, employment stability, and job benefits. Second, economic stability has been assessed using measures of socioeconomic status (SES) and conversion of parental occupation to a standardized SES index score [Reference Jenni, Fintelmann, Caflisch, Latal, Rousson and Chaouch98]. These index scores are useful because, relative to measures of gross annual household income, they allow for the more stable and reliable comparison of income across regions and countries while incorporating more abstract qualities such as occupational prestige and socioeconomic mobility [Reference Ganzeboom, De Graaf and Treiman103]. However, their scoping nature impedes interpretation of the specific mechanisms through which economic stability influences IQ. Third, several studies have relied on parental education (typically as a proxy for parental IQ), which may clarify the association between economic stability and child IQ due to the robust, albeit indirect, association between household income and access to postsecondary education [Reference Tompsett and Knoester44]. Although parental education is easily assessed and reliably associated with income, it is heavily influenced by and confounded with familial genetic factors [Reference Willoughby, McGue, Iacono, Rustichini and Lee104]. Last, economic stability has been measured as part of a composite sociodemographic family variable comprised of factors such as parental IQ, household income, and parental education (e.g., Kover et al., 2013) [Reference Kover, Pierpont, Kim, Brown and Abbeduto99]. As with standardized SES indices, these composite variables may capture multiple facets of socioeconomic advantage but lack specificity. Ultimately, a combination of measures is needed to comprehensively capture individual differences in economic stability while also identifying mechanistic pathways amenable to intervention.

Inconsistent findings also may be attributed to the association between economic stability and the diverse range of cognitive domains reflected in IQ scores. Malich and colleagues [Reference Malich, Largo, Schinzel, Molinari and Eiholzer100], quantifying SES as a composite of two six-point scales measuring paternal occupation and maternal education, demonstrated positive associations between parental SES and child FSIQ in youth with Prader–Willi syndrome. Other studies have similarly reported associations between greater parental education and greater child IQ in youth with 22q11.3 deletion syndrome [Reference Klaassen, Duijff and Swanenburg de Veye101] and adolescents with Down syndrome (DS) [Reference Evans and Uljarevic105]. In contrast, multiple studies of FXS have failed to find a significant association between family-level economic factors and IQ, although this association has primarily been assessed using measures of nonverbal IQ [Reference Dyer-Friedman, Glaser and Hessl10,Reference Kover, Pierpont, Kim, Brown and Abbeduto99,Reference Skinner, Hooper and Hatton102]. Specifically, Dyer-Friedman and colleagues [Reference Dyer-Friedman, Glaser and Hessl10] found no significant association between household income and child IQ in youth with FXS. Similarly, Skinner and colleagues [Reference Skinner, Hooper and Hatton102] similarly found no association between nonverbal IQ (as measured by the Leiter-R) and maternal education in FXS. Kover and colleagues [Reference Kover, Pierpont, Kim, Brown and Abbeduto99] also found no significant association between maternal IQ, family income, and parental education (combined into a single composite variable) and child nonverbal IQ (Leiter-R) in adolescents with FXS. These latter findings suggest that the impact of economic stability on IQ may be more pronounced for measures of verbal IQ than other domains of cognition. Indeed, there is evidence that associations between SES and IQ are stronger for verbal relative to nonverbal skills in youth with 22q11.3 deletion syndrome [Reference Klaassen, Duijff and Swanenburg de Veye101] and DS [Reference Evans and Uljarevic105]. This echoes findings from Del Hoyo and colleagues [Reference Del Hoyo Soriano, Thurman, Harvey, Kover and Abbeduto66] that higher maternal education is associated with greater increases in verbal abilities (i.e., conversational talkativeness) in adolescents with FXS and DS. Relative to nonverbal IQ, verbal IQ and language skills are more strongly influenced by environmental factors [Reference van Soelen, Brouwer, van Leeuwen, Kahn, Hulshoff Pol and Boomsma106,Reference Gornik, Jacobson, Kalb and Pritchard107], which may make them more susceptible to the influence of economic stability and related factors described above. Together, these findings indicate that economic stability accounts for some individual differences in IQ among people with syndromic IDDs, though this association may be exclusive to, or more pronounced for, verbal IQ.

Last, differences in results among these studies also may be due to sample characteristics, especially differences in age. In the general population, the heritability of IQ increases linearly with age from 41% in 9-year-old children to 66% in 17-year-old young adults [Reference Haworth, Wright and Luciano108]. Therefore, the impact of nongenetic factors, including social-environmental differences, may be amplified in early childhood. Relative to studies of the general population, few studies have examined how the heritability of IQ varies across development in individuals with syndromic IDDs. The pattern of increasing heritability of IQ with age observed in the general population appears to be present in some syndromic forms of IDDs, including DS [Reference Evans and Uljarevic105]. However, individuals with other syndromic forms of IDDs, such as 22q11.2 deletion syndrome, show more consistent levels of heritability of IQ across childhood and adolescence [Reference Olszewski, Radoeva, Fremont, Kates and Antshel109]. Other studies examining the heritability of IQ among individuals with syndromic IDDs have examined relatively broad age ranges (e.g., 3–20 years) [Reference Malich, Largo, Schinzel, Molinari and Eiholzer100], finding moderate parent-proband IQ correlations, but have not yet explicitly assessed how this association varies across development. Studies examining the moderating effect of age on relations between social-environmental factors, parental IQ, and proband IQ in families of individuals with syndromic IDDs are needed to identify the periods most sensitive to targeted efforts intervening on social-environmental disadvantage.

Social-environmental areas for future consideration

Thus far, we have focused on select, relatively well-researched areas for which there is the most support for their influences on IQ in individuals with syndromic IDDs. However, there are many SDOH that may contribute to differences in IQ but whose impact has not yet been examined in individuals with syndromic IDDs (see non-darkened boxes of Figure 2 and dashed pathways of Fig. 3). For example, social and community contextual areas, including social cohesion [Reference Gow, Pattie, Whiteman, Whalley and Deary110], discrimination [Reference Zahodne, Morris, Sharifian, Zaheed, Kraal and Sol111,Reference Barnes, Lewis, Begeny, Yu, Bennett and Wilson112], and incarceration [Reference Haskins113,Reference Umbach, Raine and Leonard114], have been identified as salient factors contributing to cognitive outcomes in the TD population, but these have not been fully explored in individuals with syndromic IDDs.

Social cohesion at the individual level (e.g., the strength, density, and number of a person’s social relationships [Reference Kawachi and Berkman115]) represents a promising area of study given the challenges many individuals with IDDs face in integrating into new neighborhoods and establishing and maintaining friendships, which results in high rates of loneliness [Reference Bigby116,Reference Robertson, Emerson and Gregory117]. Greater loneliness is associated with lower IQ in aging TD adults [Reference Gow, Pattie, Whiteman, Whalley and Deary110], possibly due to reduced cognitive stimulation available through social interaction, and this pattern, though largely unexplored, may hold in individuals with IDDs. At the level of the family unit, stronger social support is associated with lower parental stress and greater family adaptability in families of individuals with IDDs [Reference Hastings, Thomas and Delwiche118,Reference Lustig and Akey119], as well as greater family hardiness and quality of life in families of autistic youth [Reference Weiss, Robinson, Fung, Tint, Chalmers and Lunsky120,Reference Wang, Liu and Zhang121]. Associations between family support and positive quality of life outcomes may be due to greater availability of other family members and friends to assist with childcare in addition to the well-established social-emotional benefits (i.e., reduced stress) of having supportive relationships. Lower family stress may in turn support cognitive development through mechanisms described earlier (greater reciprocity of parent–child interactions and bidirectional positive impact on economic stability), though no studies to our knowledge have tested these mediating relationships.

Second, despite the high degree of stigma and discrimination experienced by individuals with IDDs and their family members [Reference Ali, Scior, Ratti, Strydom, King and Hassiotis122], especially those who also hold marginalized racial and ethnic identifies [Reference Hastings, Thomas and Delwiche118,Reference Lustig and Akey119,Reference Friedman123Reference Magaña, Parish, Morales, Li and Fujiura127], their impact on IQ and cognitive outcomes is not well understood in syndromic IDDs. The lack of research on these associations may be related to limited availability of validated self- or proxy-report measures of discrimination in individuals with IDDs, as self-report measures of discrimination are limited to individuals with mild to moderate IDD [Reference Ali, Strydom, Hassiotis, Williams and King128]. Despite limited availability of suitable self-report measures to assess discrimination, studies assessing other outcomes linked to cognitive development (e.g., health and quality of life) in people with IDDs from marginalized racial and ethnic backgrounds may help clarify the impact of discrimination. For example, non-White individuals with IDDs or other neurodevelopmental disabilities (e.g., autism) experience a lower quality of life on average than their White peers, in part due to a disproportionately limited number of decision-making opportunities [Reference Friedman123,Reference Friedman124]. Similarly, adults with IDDs who are from marginalized racial and ethnic backgrounds experience poorer health outcomes and/or face a greater number of barriers to accessing care compared to both White adults with IDDs as well as adults who are from marginalized racial and ethnic backgrounds without IDDs [Reference Magaña, Parish, Morales, Li and Fujiura127,Reference Williamson, Chico-Jarillo and Sasse129]. The multiplicative consequences of discrimination based on disability and racial/ethnic discrimination cut across these and other areas relevant to development, which may in turn drive divergent cognitive outcomes in individuals with syndromic IDDs. Increased stigma around IDD in some cultures also limits family belonginess and may subsequently impact the ability of an individual with syndromic IDDs to establish social relationships important for development, increase family stress, and limit access to educational and recreational opportunities (please see review for further discussion of this issue and possible interventions [Reference sen-van Vuuren and Aldersey130]). Ultimately, given the intersection of stigma and discrimination with most social-environmental issues [Reference Shannon, Morgan and Zeinali131], clinical studies incorporating the measurement of stigma and discrimination (both due to disability and other marginalized identities) should be prioritized.

The effects of racial and ethnic discrimination experienced by family members also may have an indirect impact on IQ in individuals with syndromic IDDs. As previously described, the negative impact of systemic racial and ethnic discrimination on IQ in individuals with syndromic IDDs may be partly attributable to prenatal exposure to stress [Reference Spann, Alleyne and Holland76,Reference Earnshaw, Rosenthal and Lewis77], delayed access to services [Reference Pearson, Meadan, Malone and tin52Reference Fraiman and Wojcik54], poverty [51], and other factors. Discrimination individually directed toward family members also increases caregiver stress, which may in turn alter parent–child interactions that shape cognitive development and IQ, as noted above.

Last, the incarceration of family members of individuals with IDDs also may contribute to interindividual differences in IQ, but no studies to our knowledge have explicitly examined these relations in this population. Incarceration of a primary caregiver limits caregiving support and family income which subsequently increases family stress and financial hardship, especially in parents of children with greater healthcare needs such as those with syndromic IDDs [Reference Jackson, Testa and Turney132,Reference Schwartz-Soicher, Geller and Garfinkel133]. Greater family stress and economic instability driven by incarceration may in turn hinder the cognitive development of individuals with IDDs through the pathways identified in Figure 2.

Limitations

The present narrative review is an important first step to advance syndromic IDD literature, yet is not without its limitations. First, IQ is not the only area in which individuals with syndromic IDDs show large interindividual differences; however, we focused on IQ due to its emphasis in the existing literature. IDDs are characterized by limitations in adaptive behaviors (e.g., daily living skills), which also show large interindividual differences among people with syndromic IDDs [Reference Di Nuovo and Buono134]. Correlations between IQ and adaptive behaviors in individuals with IDDs are generally low [Reference Di Nuovo and Buono134,Reference Tassé and Kim135], and it is not clear whether the same factors affecting IQ also affect adaptive skills. For example, a study of preschoolers exposed to the Flint water crisis suggests family SES disproportionately impacts IQ, whereas home environment disproportionately impacts adaptive behavior [Reference Zheng, LeWinn, Ceja, Hanna-Attisha, O’Connell and Bishop136]. Consistent with this, in individuals with FXS, family income is not significantly associated with adaptive behavior after accounting for the quality of the home environment [Reference Glaser, Hessl and Dyer-Friedman55]. Together, these findings suggest that the quality of the home environment may mediate the impact SES has on adaptive development, although this pathway has not been explicitly modeled to our knowledge. Given the impact that adaptive behaviors have on independence and quality of life, research into factors that maximize adaptive development is needed. Moving beyond measures of IQ also is critical to avoid conflating meaningful differences in cognitive development resulting from malleable social-environmental factors with differences caused by known systemic cultural bias in test norming [26].

Second, we chose to focus our discussion on the impact of economic stability and related mechanistic pathways reflecting the greatest volume of research in these areas. We acknowledge that other key social-environmental factors, such as neighborhood crime or violence, housing quality, and social cohesion, are potentially important and have received limited attention in the literature, despite being strongly associated with key economic factors such as neighborhood income and poverty [Reference Graif and Matthews137Reference Ridge139]. Studies assessing the association between additional SDOH factors and IQ are needed to fully understand ways to optimize outcomes for individuals with syndromic IDDs.

Finally, our narrative review is not exhaustive. We chose to pursue a narrative review over a systematic review or meta-analytic approaches due to the overall dearth of IDD-specific research in this area. We acknowledge that there may be studies of social-environmental factors as they relate to IQ not discussed in our narrative review. However, this choice allowed us to more readily integrate findings from the general population and broader neurodevelopmental disabilities (e.g., autism) literature into the proposed theoretical model on relations between social-environmental factors and IQ. The use of a narrative review allows us to build on these findings to discuss future directions for the field of IDD. We also acknowledge that by pursuing a narrative review, there is a greater risk for bias in the studies we have discussed. Our authorship team’s expertise is in FXS and DS (WSM, LA, and LMS), and we are more likely to draw on the literature in those areas. Ultimately, we believe our review reflects the preponderance of literature in this area because FXS and DS two of the most well-researched syndromic IDDs due to their prevalence and penetrance – FXS is the most common heritable cause of IDD and DS is the most common genetic cause of IDD. However, we have done our best to combat this bias by explicitly targeting studies of other forms of syndromic IDDs and by conceptualizing our model in collaboration with an author with expertise in public health and SDOH in pediatric populations outside of FXS and DS (DNW).

Recommendations

As previously described [Reference Kover and Abbeduto23], systemic changes are needed to advance IDD research, and we propose that research which promotes and clarifies relations between social-environmental factors and IQ in individuals with syndromic IDDs is one way to do so. Building upon the extant literature and our proposed theoretical model, we make three aligned recommendations for researchers, reviewers, and journal editors to propel the field and address current research gaps (Table 2).

Table 2. Recommendations to delineate relations between social-environmental factors and IQ in individuals with syndromic IDDs

SES = socioeconomic status; IQ = intelligence quotient; IDD = intellectual developmental disabilities.

Recommendation 1: Measure and report demographic information

Researchers should measure and provide more details about demographic factors, including information on race, ethnicity, recruitment catchment area, socioeconomic resources (e.g., income or, preferably, more comprehensive standardized measures such as the Child Opportunity Index or Area Deprivation Index), age of first diagnosis, insurance status, and receipt of services. Given the inconsistent reporting of this information in the neurodevelopmental and broader pediatric literatures [Reference Steinbrenner, McIntyre and Rentschler140,Reference Rees, Stewart and Mehta141], we encourage journal editors and reviewers to emphasize the importance of including this information and communicate previously published guidelines on terminology and language for authors to consider when reporting this data [Reference Flanagin, Frey and Christiansen142]. For example, multiple prominent medical, psychological, and IDD-adjacent journals require that authors report participants’ race, ethnicity, age, gender, and SES. It also is critical to extend these basic measures by also assessing factors and pathways thought to have a more direct impact on cognition, which may clarify the mechanisms through which social-environmental factors impact development in syndromic IDDs. For example, there is a rich literature on relations between family stress and child outcomes in caregivers of youth with IDDs [Reference Del Hoyo Soriano, Thurman, Harvey, Ted Brown and Abbeduto65,Reference Emerson143,Reference Miodrag and Peters144].

As previously noted, studies assessing relations between economic stability and IQ in individuals with syndromic IDDs have measured economic factors using varying methodologies, with a general focus on household income or caregiver SES. As recommended by Kover and colleagues [Reference Kover, Pierpont, Kim, Brown and Abbeduto99], the impact of broader economic stability on IQ requires adopting a comprehensive, standardized measure of economic opportunity, such as the International Socio-Economic Index [Reference Ganzeboom, De Graaf and Treiman103], Child Opportunity Index [Reference Acevedo-Garcia, McArdle and Hardy145], or the Area Deprivation Index [Reference Kind and Buckingham146], the latter two of which incorporate multiple measures of economic disadvantage (e.g., employment opportunities, housing quality, and income). The association between greater neighborhood-level deprivation and lower IQ is strong in the general population [Reference Kalb, Lieb and Ludwig147], but its relation with IQ has not yet been studied in individuals with syndromic IDDs. The Child Opportunity Index and Area Deprivation Index are worth incorporating in future studies of syndromic IDDs, especially given the low administrative and participant burden for their measurement (i.e., home address). Further, incorporating additional measures assessing the actual mechanisms (e.g., nutritional differences) through which these wide-reaching economic characteristics influence IQ will clarify the nature and malleability of these relations.

Researchers in this area also may consider incorporating well-validated measures of stress due to racism and discrimination, such as the Race-Based Traumatic Stress Symptom Scale [Reference Carter, Mazzula and Victoria148], as stress due to racism and discrimination, rather than race or ethnicity itself, hinders developmental outcomes. In addition to reporting demographic information, research teams also should prioritize recruitment of groups historically excluded from and underrepresented in IDD research, including non-English-speaking families, families affected by poverty, and families holding marginalized racial and ethnic identities [Reference Sinclair, Hansen and Machalicek149].

Recommendation 2: Assess relations between social-environmental factors and clinical outcomes

Researchers should focus on examining the relation between social-environmental factors and clinical outcomes (including IQ) in studies of syndromic IDDs to address current gaps in understanding, such as those identified in Figures 2 and 3. In practical terms, this entails incorporating social-demographic variables into models predicting clinical outcomes during their stepwise construction (e.g., Dyer-Friedman et al., 2002) [Reference Dyer-Friedman, Glaser and Hessl10] to determine the additional variance in clinical outcomes captured by social or environmental factors. Consider, for example, researchers studying the efficacy of a parent-mediated intervention on language development of youth with IDDs. Researchers typically examine how basic demographic characteristics, including child age or sex, explain differences in treatment response. However, there are a host of other social-environmental factors that likely influence treatment fidelity or response, including age of diagnosis (as a proxy for delayed access to care), parent education, family-level stress driven by economic instability, and incarceration limiting caregiver availability. An initial model assessing treatment response may include treatment condition (novel treatment vs. treatment as usual), child age, and child sex. Subsequent models may add these relevant social-environmental factors to assess their moderating impact on treatment response. In this example, researchers would then be able to assess whether families experiencing high stress due to economic factors are less likely to benefit from a parent-mediated treatment. Thus, incorporating these factors in models assessing treatment response could ultimately inform treatment decisions when the intervention is translated into community practice.

Conducting this work as part of interdisciplinary teams and using team science approaches will leverage the expertise of related fields of research (e.g., sociology, epidemiology, and economics) to better inform the development of individual, community, and systems-level interventions that target these pathways. We further encourage researchers to examine these relations even in studies of multiple forms of syndromic or non-syndromic IDDs, as these associations are likely transdiagnostic. However, no study to our knowledge has systematically examined the differential impact of social-environmental factors on IQ across multiple forms of IDDs. Although such studies are inherently difficult to conduct due to the rarity of syndromic IDDs, they may identify associations and intervention targets that vary across syndromes. Relatedly, no study to our knowledge has examined how the strength of these associations varies in individuals with syndromic IDDs relative to the general population. It is not known, for example, if economic factors contribute more or less strongly to individual differences in IQ in individuals with syndromic IDDs than they do in the TD population.

Recommendation 3: Adopt a common framework organizing relevant social-environmental factors

The field would benefit from a unifying framework identifying the full breadth of relevant SDOH and systemic factors and their specific applications and considerations for individuals living with IDD and their families. This framework would help identify other relevant areas beyond those identified in Figure 3 in which studies of general and clinical populations suggest are linked to cognitive outcomes, including IQ. Examples include exposure to pollutants/toxicants [Reference Carrington, Devleesschauwer, Gibb and Bolger150], housing stability [Reference Shinn, Schteingart and Williams151], exposure to community violence [Reference Butler, Yang, Laube, Kuhn and Immordino-Yang152], discrimination [Reference Kira, Lewandowski, Ashby, Somers, Chiodo and Odenat153], and other areas. Although the theoretical model proposed in the present narrative review highlights a few key SDOH, there is insufficient research on other SDOH factors in individuals with syndromic IDDs to leverage and expand the model beyond its current form. Research examining the impact of SDOH, including expanding our definition and assessment of SDOH factors in research, is needed to more fully understand the etiology of interindividual differences in syndromic IDDs.

The identification of pathways by which these factors impact IQ is crucial to determine which advocacy efforts and interventions may be the most fruitful. For example, the American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities (AAIDD) regularly publishes a public policy agenda and has published several policy statements informed by this research. One position statement relevant to the present review advocates for significantly increased funding for universal, community-based, early intervention services for young children with or at increased risk for developmental delays [154], which may contribute to gains in IQ and improved quality of life. Our hope is that research clarifying these pathways may help develop a line of evidence-based SDOH-targeted interventions that can be adapted to clinical care.

Conclusions

Social-environmental factors may explain many interindividual differences in IQ among individuals with syndromic IDDs. Although we discuss evidence of the pathways through which economic instability may influence IQ in this population, there is a dearth of research on the contributions of other social-environmental factors to IQ in individuals with syndromic IDDs. We urge researchers and those who review and publish research (e.g., reviewers and journal editors) to consider the value and importance of incorporating social-demographic information into future work. The addition of this information, especially factors embedded in well-established SDOH frameworks, will support the development of individual-, community-, and systems-level interventions that promote cognitive development and adaptive functioning in individuals with syndromic IDDs.

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to thank the patients and families seen in the Cincinnati Fragile X Center, whose lived experiences directly informed this work. The authors also wish to thank Drs. Andrea Fidler, Amy Lang, Melissa Miller, and Bridget Murphy for their valuable feedback on earlier drafts of this manuscript.

Author contributions

LMS and WSM conceptualized and designed the review. All authors (WSM, DNW, LA, and LMS) drafted and edited the manuscript. All authors reviewed and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Funding statement

This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health (PI: Dr Craig Erickson, grant numbers U54HD082008, U54HD104461), (LMS, grant number K23HD101416), and (LA, grant number P50HD103526).

Competing interests

None.

Footnotes

Authors self-identify as a cis-gender, non-Hispanic, white man (WSM, LA); a cis-gender, multi-racial, Afro-Latina-Indigenous woman (DNW); and a cis-gender, non-Hispanic, white woman (LMS).

References

American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual Of Mental Disorders. 5th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association Publishing; 2022.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rauch, A, Hoyer, J, Guth, S, et al. Diagnostic yield of various genetic approaches in patients with unexplained developmental delay or mental retardation. Am J Med Genet A. 2006;1(19):20632074. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.31416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patel, DR, Cabral, MD, Ho, A, Merrick, J. A clinical primer on intellectual disability. Transl Pediatr. 2020;9(Suppl 1):S23S35. doi: 10.21037/tp.2020.02.02.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufman, L, Ayub, M, Vincent, JB. The genetic basis of non-syndromic intellectual disability: a review. J Neurodev Disord. 2010;2(4):182209. doi: 10.1007/s11689-010-9055-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hagerman, RJ, Hull, CE, Safanda, JF, et al. High functioning fragile X males: demonstration of an unmethylated fully expanded FMR-1 mutation associated with protein expression. Am J Med Genet. 1994;51(4):298308. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.1320510404.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moreno-De-Luca, A, Evans, DW, Boomer, KB, et al. The role of parental cognitive, behavioral, and motor profiles in clinical variability in individuals with chromosome 16p11.2 deletions. JAMA Psychiatry. 2015;72(2):119126. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.2147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Basile, E, Villa, L, Selicorni, A, Molteni, M. The behavioural phenotype of Cornelia de Lange syndrome: a study of 56 individuals. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2007;51(Pt 9):671681. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2007.00977.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Smedt, B, Devriendt, K, Fryns, JP, Vogels, A, Gewillig, M, Swillen, A. Intellectual abilities in a large sample of children with velo-cardio-facial syndrome: an update. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2007;51(Pt 9):666670. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2007.00955.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fishler, K, Koch, R, Donnell, GN. Comparison of mental development in individuals with mosaic and trisomy 21 down’s syndrome. Pediatrics. 1976;58(5):744748.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dyer-Friedman, J, Glaser, B, Hessl, D, et al. Genetic and environmental influences on the cognitive outcomes of children with fragile X syndrome. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2002;41(3):237244. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200203000-00002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whittington, J, Holland, A, Webb, T, Butler, J, Clarke, D, Boer, H. Cognitive abilities and genotype in a population-based sample of people with Prader-Willi syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2004;48(Pt 2):172187. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2004.00556.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ajmone, PF, Avignone, S, Gervasini, C, et al. Rubinstein-Taybi syndrome: new neuroradiological and neuropsychiatric insights from a multidisciplinary approach. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2018;177(4):406415. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.32628.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Madduri, N, Peters, SU, Voigt, RG, Llorente, AM, Lupski, JR, Potocki, L. Cognitive and adaptive behavior profiles in Smith-Magenis syndrome. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2006;27(3):188192. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200606000-00002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bellugi, U, Lichtenberger, L, Jones, W, Lai, Z, St George, MI. The neurocognitive profile of Williams syndrome: a complex pattern of strengths and weaknesses. J Cogn Neurosci. 2000;12(Supplement 1):729. doi: 10.1162/089892900561959.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmitt, LM, Will, M, Shaffer, R, Erickson, C. A paradigm shifting view of intellectual disability: a near normal distribution of IQ in fragile X syndrome. Res Sq. 2023;rs.3:rs-2869313. doi: 10.21203/rs.3.rs-2869313/v1.Google Scholar
Hessl, D, Nguyen, DV, Green, C, et al. A solution to limitations of cognitive testing in children with intellectual disabilities: the case of fragile X syndrome. J Neurodev Disord. 2009;1(1):3345. doi: 10.1007/s11689-008-9001-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Couzens, D, Cuskelly, M, Jobling, A. The stanford binet fourth edition and its use with individuals with down syndrome: cautions for clinicians. Int J Disabil Dev Educ. 2004;51(1):3956. doi: 10.1080/1034912042000182193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dickens, WT, Flynn, JR. Heritability estimates versus large environmental effects: the IQ paradox resolved. Psychol Rev. 2001;108(2):346369. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.108.2.346.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boggs, AE, Schmitt, LM, McLane, RD, et al. Optimization, validation and initial clinical implications of a luminex-based immunoassay for the quantification of fragile X protein from dried blood spots. Sci Rep. 2022;12(1):5617. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-09633-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Majoka, MA, Schimming, C. Effect of social determinants of health on cognition and risk of Alzheimer disease and related dementias. Clin Ther. 2021;43(6):922929. doi: 10.1016/j.clinthera.2021.05.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bronfenbrenner, U, Ceci, SJ. Nature-nurture reconceptualized in developmental perspective: a bioecological model. Psychol Rev. 1994;101(4):568586. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.101.4.568.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heikura, U, Taanila, A, Hartikainen, AL, et al. Variations in prenatal sociodemographic factors associated with intellectual disability: a study of the 20-year interval between two birth cohorts in northern Finland. Am J Epidemiol. 2008;15(2):169177. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwm291.Google Scholar
Kover, ST, Abbeduto, L. Toward equity in research on intellectual and developmental disabilities. Am J Intellect Dev Disabil. 2023;1(5):350370. doi: 10.1352/1944-7558-128.5.350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stetler, C. NIH to fund exposome research coordinating center. National Institutes of Health, 2023. https://factor.niehs.nih.gov/2023/10/science-highlights/nih-funding-to-support-exposome-research-coordinating-center. Accessed November 4, 2023.Google Scholar
US Department of Health and Human Services. Social Determinants of Health. https://health.gov/healthypeople/priority-areas/social-determinants-health. Accessed October 15, 2023.Google Scholar
Shuttleworth-Edwards AB. Generally representative is representative of none: commentary on the pitfalls of IQ test standardization in multicultural settings. Clin Neuropsychol. 2016;30(7):975–998. doi: 10.1080/13854046.2016.1204011.Google Scholar
Wehmeyer, ML, Garner, NW. The impact of personal characteristics of people with intellectual and developmental disability on self-determination and autonomous functioning. J Appl Res Intellect Disabil. 2003;16(4):255265. doi: 10.1046/j.1468-3148.2003.00161.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nota, L, Ferrari, L, Soresi, S, Wehmeyer, M. Self-determination, social abilities and the quality of life of people with intellectual disability. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2007;51(Pt 11):850865. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2006.00939.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Su, CY, Chen, CC, Wuang, YP, Lin, YH, Wu, YY. Neuropsychological predictors of everyday functioning in adults with intellectual disabilities. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2008;52(Pt 1):1828. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2007.00969.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Firkowska-Mankiewicz, A. Adult careers: does childhood IQ predict later life outcome? J Policy Pract Intel. 2011;8(1):19. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-1130.2011.00281.x.Google Scholar
Erostarbe-Pérez, M, Reparaz-Abaitua, C, tínez-Pérez, L, Magallón-Recalde, S. Executive functions and their relationship with intellectual capacity and age in schoolchildren with intellectual disability. J Intell Disabil Res. 2022;66(1-2):5067. doi: 10.1111/jir.12885.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hill, HD, Romich, J, Mattingly, MJ, Shamsuddin, S, Wething, H. An introduction to household economic instability and social policy. Soc Serv Rev. 2017;91(3):371389. doi: 10.1086/694110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hacker, JS, Huber, GA, Nichols, A, et al. The economic security index: a new measure for research and policy analysis. Rev Income Wealth. 2014;60(S1):S5S32. doi: 10.1111/roiw.12053.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kachi, Y, Kato, T, Kawachi, I. Socio-economic disparities in early childhood education enrollment: Japanese population-based study. J Epidemiol. 2020;5(3):143150. doi: 10.2188/jea.JE20180216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shi, L, Stevens, GD. Disparities in access to care and satisfaction among U.S. children: the roles of race/ethnicity and poverty status. Public Health Rep. 2005;120(4):431441. doi: 10.1177/003335490512000410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Institute of Medicine (US) Committee on Health Insurance Status and Its Consequences. America’s Uninsured Crisis: Consequences for Health and Health Care. Washington, DC: National Academies Press; 2009.Google Scholar
Ramey, CT, Ramey, SL. Prevention of intellectual disabilities: early interventions to improve cognitive development. Prev Med. 1998;27(2):224232. doi: 10.1006/pmed.1998.0279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eldevik, S, Hastings, RP, Hughes, JC, Jahr, E, Eikeseth, S, Cross, S. Using participant data to extend the evidence base for intensive behavioral intervention for children with autism. Am J Intellect Dev Disabil. 2010;115(5):381405. doi: 10.1352/1944-7558-115.5.381.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, JF, Cecil, HS, Barker, WF. Early intervention with retarded children: some encouraging results. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1984;26(1):4755. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1984.tb04405.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winarni, TI, Schneider, A, Borodyanskara, M, Hagerman, RJ. Early intervention combined with targeted treatment promotes cognitive and behavioral improvements in young children with fragile x syndrome. Case Rep Genet. 2012;2012:280813–4. doi: 10.1155/2012/280813.Google ScholarPubMed
Fortney, S, Tasse, MJ. Urbanicity, health, and access to services for people with intellectual disability and developmental disabilities. Am J Intellect Dev Disabil. 2021;1(6):492504. doi: 10.1352/1944-7558-126.6.492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kidd, SA, Raspa, M, Clark, R, et al. Attendance at fragile X specialty clinics: facilitators and barriers. Am J Intellect Dev Disabil. 2017;122(6):457475. doi: 10.1352/1944-7558-122.6.457.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joslyn, N, Berger, H, Skotko, BG. Geospatial analyses of accessibility to down syndrome specialty care. J Pediatr. 2020;218:146150 e1. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2019.10.058.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tompsett, J, Knoester, C. Family socioeconomic status and college attendance: a consideration of individual-level and school-level pathways. PLoS One. 2023;18(4):e0284188. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0284188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cloney, D, Cleveland, G, Hattie, J, Tayler, C. Variations in the availability and quality of early childhood education and care by socioeconomic status of neighborhoods. Early Educ Dev. 2016;27(3):384401. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2015.1076674.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Queralt, M, Witte, AD. Influences on neighborhood supply of child care in Massachusetts. Soc Serv Rev. 1998;72(1):1746. doi: 10.1086/515744.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meghir, C, Attanasio, O, Jervis, P, et al. Early stimulation and enhanced preschool: a randomized trial. Pediatrics. 2023;151(Suppl 2):e2023060221H. doi: 10.1542/peds.2023-060221H.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peters-Scheffer, N, Didden, R, Mulders, M, Korzilius, H. Low intensity behavioral treatment supplementing preschool services for young children with autism spectrum disorders and severe to mild intellectual disability. Res Dev Disabil. 2010;31(6):16781684. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2010.04.008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strickland, BB, Jones, JR, Newacheck, PW, Bethell, CD, Blumberg, SJ, Kogan, MD. Assessing systems quality in a changing health care environment: the 2009-10 national survey of children with special health care needs. Matern Child Health J. 2015;19(2):353361. doi: 10.1007/s10995-014-1517-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hare, DJ, Pratt, C, Burton, M, Bromley, J, Emerson, E. The health and social care needs of family carers supporting adults with autistic spectrum disorders. Autism. 2004;8(4):425444. doi: 10.1177/1362361304047225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pew Research Center. On Views of Race and Inequality, Blacks and Whites Are Worlds Apart. 2016. pewresearch.org/social-trends/2016/06/27/on-views-of-race-and-inequality-blacks-and-whites-are-worlds-apart/. Accessed November 1, 2023.Google Scholar
Pearson, JN, Meadan, H, Malone, KM, tin, BM. Parent and professional experiences supporting African-American children with autism. J Racial Ethn Health Disparities. 2020;7(2):305315. doi: 10.1007/s40615-019-00659-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crawford, DC, Meadows, KL, Newman, JL, et al. Prevalence of the fragile X syndrome in African-Americans. Am J Med Genet. 2002;1(3):226233. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.10427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fraiman, YS, Wojcik, MH. The influence of social determinants of health on the genetic diagnostic odyssey: who remains undiagnosed, why, and to what effect? Pediatr Res. 2021;89(2):295300. doi: 10.1038/s41390-020-01151-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glaser, B, Hessl, D, Dyer-Friedman, J, et al. Biological and environmental contributions to adaptive behavior in fragile X syndrome. Am J Med Genet A. 2003;15(1):2129. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.10549.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bartholo, KL, Lee, CH, Bruno, JL, Lightbody, AA, Reiss, AL. Closing the gender gap in fragile X syndrome: review on females with FXS and preliminary research findings. Brain Sci. 2019;9(1): doi: 10.3390/brainsci9010011.Google Scholar
Wadhwa, PD, Entringer, S, Buss, C, Lu, MC. The contribution of maternal stress to preterm birth: issues and considerations. Clin Perinatol. 2011;38(3):351384. doi: 10.1016/j.clp.2011.06.007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wadhwa, PD, Sandman, CA, Porto, M, Dunkel-Schetter, C, Garite, TJ. The association between prenatal stress and infant birth weight and gestational age at birth: a prospective investigation. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 1993;169(4):858865. doi: 10.1016/0002-9378(93)90016-c.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Triplett, RL, Lean, RE, Parikh, A, et al. Association of prenatal exposure to early-life adversity with neonatal brain volumes at birth. JAMA Network Open. 2022;5(4):e227045e227045. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2022.7045.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vishnevetsky, J, Tang, D, Chang, HW, et al. Combined effects of prenatal polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons and material hardship on child IQ. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2015;49:7480. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2015.04.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perkins, SC, Finegood, ED, Swain, JE. Poverty and language development: roles of parenting and stress. Clin Neurosci. 2013;10(4):1019.Google ScholarPubMed
McLoyd, VC. The impact of economic hardship on black families and children: psychological distress, parenting, and socioemotional development. Child Dev. 1990;61(2):311346. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1990.tb02781.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackson, AP, Choi, J-K, Preston, KSJ. Harsh parenting and black boys’ behavior problems: single mothers’ parenting stress and nonresident fathers’ involvement. Fam Relat. 2019;68(4):436449. doi: 10.1111/fare.12373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seltzer, MM, Greenberg, JS, Floyd, FJ, Pettee, Y, Hong, J. Life course impacts of parenting a child with a disability. Am J Ment Retard. 2001;106(3):265286. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2001)106.2.0.CO;2>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Del Hoyo Soriano, L, Thurman, AJ, Harvey, DJ, Ted Brown, W, Abbeduto, L. Genetic and maternal predictors of cognitive and behavioral trajectories in females with fragile X syndrome. J Neurodev Disord. 2018;10(1):22. doi: 10.1186/s11689-018-9240-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Del Hoyo Soriano, L, Thurman, AJ, Harvey, D, Kover, ST, Abbeduto, L. Expressive language development in adolescents with down syndrome and fragile X syndrome: change over time and the role of family-related factors. J Neurodev Disord. 2020;12(1):18. doi: 10.1186/s11689-020-09320-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warren, SF, Brady, N, Sterling, A, Fleming, K, quis, J. Maternal responsivity predicts language development in young children with fragile X syndrome. Am J Intellect Dev Disabil. 2010;115(1):5475. doi: 10.1352/1944-7558-115.1.54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Justice, LM, Jiang, H, Purtell, KM, et al. Conditions of poverty, parent-child interactions, and toddlers’early language skills in low-income families. Matern Child Health J. 2019;23(7):971978. doi: 10.1007/s10995-018-02726-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fowler, PJ, McGrath, LM, Henry, DB, et al. Housing mobility and cognitive development: change in verbal and nonverbal abilities. Child Abuse Negl. 2015;48:104118. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2015.06.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sandbank, M, Yoder, P. The association between parental mean length of utterance and language outcomes in children with disabilities: a correlational meta-analysis. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. 2016;1(2):240251. doi: 10.1044/2015_AJSLP-15-0003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fusaroli, R, Weed, E, Fein, D, Naigles, L. Hearing me hearing you: reciprocal effects between child and parent language in autism and typical development. Cognition. 2019;183:118. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.10.022.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Song, L, Spier, ET, Tamis-Lemonda, CS. Reciprocal influences between maternal language and children’s language and cognitive development in low-income families. J Child Lang. 2014;41(2):305326. doi: 10.1017/S0305000912000700.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, Z, Loh, SC, Tian, J, Chen, QJ. A meta-analysis of the effect of the early start denver model in children with autism spectrum disorder. Int J Dev Disabil. 2022;68(5):587597. doi: 10.1080/20473869.2020.1870419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, SS, Monlux, KD, Rodriguez, AB, Jo, B, Pollard, JS. Telehealth-enabled behavioral treatment for problem behaviors in boys with fragile X syndrome: a randomized controlled trial. J Neurodev Disord. 2020;12(1):31. doi: 10.1186/s11689-020-09331-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bullard, L, McDuffie, A, Abbeduto, L. Distance delivery of a parent-implemented language intervention for young boys with fragile X syndrome. Autism Dev Lang Impair. 2017;2:239694151772869. doi: 10.1177/2396941517728690.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spann, MN, Alleyne, K, Holland, CM, et al. The effects of experience of discrimination and acculturation during pregnancy on the developing offspring brain. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2023;49(2):476485. doi: 10.1038/s41386-023-01765-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Earnshaw, VA, Rosenthal, L, Lewis, JB, et al. Maternal experiences with everyday discrimination and infant birth weight: a test of mediators and moderators among young, urban women of color. Ann Behav Med. 2013;45(1):1323. doi: 10.1007/s12160-012-9404-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hatton, C, Emerson, E, Graham, H, Blacher, J, Llewellyn, G. Changes in family composition and ital status in families with a young child with cognitive delay. J Appl Res Intellect. 2010;23(1):1426. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-3148.2009.00543.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradley, RH. Children’s home environments, health, behavior, and intervention efforts: a review using the HOME inventory as a ker measure. Genet Soc Gen Psychol Monogr. 1993;119(4):437490.Google Scholar
Garrett, P, Ng'andu, N, Ferron, J. Poverty experiences of young children and the quality of their home environments. Child Dev. 1994;65(2):331345. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1994.tb00754.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dumont, DM, Wildeman, C, Lee, H, Gjelsvik, A, Valera, P, Clarke, JG. Incarceration, maternal hardship, and perinatal health behaviors. Matern Child Health J. 2014;18(9):21792187. doi: 10.1007/s10995-014-1466-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luster, T, Perlstadt, H, McKinney, M, Sims, K, Juang, L. The effects of a family support program and other factors on the home environments provided by adolescent mothers. Family Relations. 1996;45(3):255264. doi: 10.2307/585497.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kamp Dush, CM, Schmeer, KK, Taylor, M. Chaos as a social determinant of child health: reciprocal associations? Soc Sci Med. 2013;95:6976. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.01.038.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barker, ED, Kirkham, N, Ng, J, Jensen, SK. Prenatal maternal depression symptoms and nutrition, and child cognitive function. Br J Psychiatry. 2013;203(6):417421. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.113.129486.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
shall, NE, Abrams, B, Barbour, LA, et al. The importance of nutrition in pregnancy and lactation: lifelong consequences. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2022;226(5):607632. doi: 10.1016/j.ajog.2021.12.035.Google Scholar
Heaman, MI, Moffatt, M, Elliott, L, et al. Barriers, motivators and facilitators related to prenatal care utilization among inner-city women in Winnipeg, Canada: a case-control study. BMC Pregnancy Childb. 2014;14(1):227. doi: 10.1186/1471-2393-14-227.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nothnagle, M, chi, K, Egerter, S, Braveman, P. Risk factors for late or no prenatal care following medicaid expansions in California. Matern Child Healt J. 2000;4(4):251259. doi: 10.1023/A:1026647722295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rose-Jacobs, R, Black, MM, Casey, PH, et al. Household food insecurity: associations with at-risk infant and toddler development. Pediatrics. 2008;121(1):6572. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-3717.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waber, DP, Bryce, CP, Girard, JM, Zichlin, M, Fitzmaurice, GM, Galler, JR. Impaired IQ and academic skills in adults who experienced moderate to severe infantile malnutrition: a 40-year study. Nutr Neurosci. 2014;17(2):5864. doi: 10.1179/1476830513Y.0000000061.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belsky, DW, Moffitt, TE, Arseneault, L, Melchior, M, Caspi, A. Context and sequelae of food insecurity in children’s development. Am J Epidemiol. 2010;1(7):809818. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwq201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, CY, Zhao, X, Reesor-Oyer, L, Cepni, AB, Hernandez, DC. Bidirectional relationship between food insecurity and housing instability. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2021;121(1):8491. doi: 10.1016/j.d.2020.08.081.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, KT, Palakshappa, D, Basu, S, Seligman, H, Berkowitz, SA. Examining the bidirectional relationship between food insecurity and healthcare spending. Health Serv Res. 2021;56(5):864873. doi: 10.1111/1475-6773.13641.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butterworth, P, Leach, LS, Pirkis, J, Kelaher, M. Poor mental health influences risk and duration of unemployment: a prospective study. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2012;47(6):10131021. doi: 10.1007/s00127-011-0409-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olesen, SC, Butterworth, P, Leach, LS, Kelaher, M, Pirkis, J. Mental health affects future employment as job loss affects mental health: findings from a longitudinal population study. BMC Psychiatry. 2013;24 13(1):144. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-13-144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whooley, MA, Kiefe, CI, Chesney, MA, et al. Depressive symptoms, unemployment, and loss of income: the CARDIA study. Arch Intern Med. 2002;9-23(22):26142620. doi: 10.1001/archinte.162.22.2614.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crowe, L, Butterworth, P. The role of financial hardship, mastery and social support in the association between employment status and depression: results from an Australian longitudinal cohort study. BMJ Open. 2016;6(5):e009834. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2015-009834.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diette, TM, Goldsmith, AH, Hamilton, D, Darity, W Jr. Causality in the Relationship Between Mental Health and Unemployment. Reconnecting To Work: Policies To Mitigate Long-term Unemployment and Its Consequences. Kalamazoo, MI: W.E. Upjohn Institute for Employment Research; 2012.Google Scholar
Jenni, OG, Fintelmann, S, Caflisch, J, Latal, B, Rousson, V, Chaouch, A. Stability of cognitive performance in children with mild intellectual disability. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2015;57(5):463469. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.12620.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kover, ST, Pierpont, EI, Kim, JS, Brown, WT, Abbeduto, L. A neurodevelopmental perspective on the acquisition of nonverbal cognitive skills in adolescents with fragile X syndrome. Dev Neuropsychol. 2013;38(7):445460. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2013.820305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malich, S, Largo, RH, Schinzel, A, Molinari, L, Eiholzer, U. Phenotypic heterogeneity of growth and psychometric intelligence in prader-willi syndrome: variable expression of a contiguous gene syndrome or parent-child resemblance? Am J Med Genet. 2000;10(4):298304. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-8628(20000410)91:.3.0.CO;2-G>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klaassen, P, Duijff, S, Swanenburg de Veye, H, et al. Explaining the variable penetrance of CNVs: parental intelligence modulates expression of intellectual impairment caused by the 22q11.2 deletion. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2016;171(6):790796. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.32441.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skinner, M, Hooper, S, Hatton, DD, et al. Mapping nonverbal IQ in young boys with fragile X syndrome. Am J Med Genet A. 2005;1(1):2532. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.30353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ganzeboom, HBG, De Graaf, PM, Treiman, DJ. A standard international socio-economic index of occupational status. Soc Sci Res. 1992;21(1):156. doi: 10.1016/0049-089X(92)90017-B.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Willoughby, EA, McGue, M, Iacono, WG, Rustichini, A, Lee, JJ. The role of parental genotype in predicting offspring years of education: evidence for genetic nurture. Mol Psychiatry. 2021;26(8):38963904. doi: 10.1038/s41380-019-0494-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, DW, Uljarevic, M. Parental education accounts for variability in the IQs of probands with down syndrome: a longitudinal study. Am J Med Genet A. 2018;176(1):2933. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.38519.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Soelen, IL, Brouwer, RM, van Leeuwen, M, Kahn, RS, Hulshoff Pol, HE, Boomsma, DI. Heritability of verbal and performance intelligence in a pediatric longitudinal sample. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2011;14(2):119128. doi: 10.1375/twin.14.2.119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gornik, AE, Jacobson, LA, Kalb, LG, Pritchard, AE. If opportunity knocks: understanding contextual factors’ influence on cognitive systems. Res Child Adolesc Psychopathol. 2023;52(4):521, 533. doi: 10.1007/s10802-023-01134-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haworth, CM, Wright, MJ, Luciano, M, et al. The heritability of general cognitive ability increases linearly from childhood to young adulthood. Mol Psychiatry. 2010;15(11):11121120. doi: 10.1038/mp.2009.55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olszewski, AK, Radoeva, PD, Fremont, W, Kates, WR, Antshel, KM. Is child intelligence associated with parent and sibling intelligence in individuals with developmental disorders? An investigation in youth with 22q11.2 deletion (velo-cardio-facial) syndrome. Res Dev Disabil. 2014;35(12):35823590. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2014.08.034.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gow, AJ, Pattie, A, Whiteman, MC, Whalley, LJ, Deary, IJ. Social support and successful aging: investigating the relationships between lifetime cognitive change and life satisfaction. J Individ Differ. 2007;28(3):103115. doi: 10.1027/1614-0001.28.3.103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zahodne, LB, Morris, EP, Sharifian, N, Zaheed, AB, Kraal, AZ, Sol, K. Everyday discrimination and subsequent cognitive abilities across five domains. Neuropsychology. 2020;34(7):783790. doi: 10.1037/neu0000693.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnes, LL, Lewis, TT, Begeny, CT, Yu, L, Bennett, DA, Wilson, RS. Perceived discrimination and cognition in older African Americans. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2012;18(5):856865. doi: 10.1017/S1355617712000628.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haskins, AR. Beyond boys’ bad behavior: paternal incarceration and cognitive development in middle childhood. Social Forces. 2016;95(2):861892. doi: 10.1093/sf/sow066.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Umbach, R, Raine, A, Leonard, NR. Cognitive line as a result of incarceration and the effects of a CBT/MT intervention: a cluster-randomized controlled trial. Crim Justice Behav. 2018;45(1):3155. doi: 10.1177/0093854817736345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kawachi, I, Berkman, LF. Social capital, social cohesion, and health. In: Social Epidemiology. 2nd ed. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2014.Google Scholar
Bigby, C. Known well by no-one: trends in the informal social networks of middle-aged and older people with intellectual disability five years after moving to the community. J Intellect Dev Disabil. 2008;33(2):148157. doi: 10.1080/13668250802094141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robertson, J, Emerson, E, Gregory, N, et al. Social networks of people with mental retardation in residential settings. Ment Retard. 2001;39(3):201214. doi: 10.1352/0047-6765(2001)039.2.0.CO;2>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hastings, RP, Thomas, H, Delwiche, N. Grandparent support for families of children with down’s syndrome. J Appl Res Intellect Disabil. 2002;15(1):97104. doi: 10.1046/j.13602322.2001.00097.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lustig, DC, Akey, T. Adaptation in families with adult children with mental retardation: impact of family strengths and appraisal. Educ Train Mental Retardat Dev Disabil. 1999;34(3):260270.Google Scholar
Weiss, JA, Robinson, S, Fung, S, Tint, A, Chalmers, P, Lunsky, Y. Family hardiness, social support, and self-efficacy in mothers of individuals with Autism spectrum disorders. Res Autism Spect Dis. 2013;7(11):13101317. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2013.07.016.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, R, Liu, Q, Zhang, W. Coping, social support, and family quality of life for caregivers of individuals with autism: meta-analytic structural equation modeling. Pers Indiv Differ. 2022;186:111351. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2021.111351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ali, A, Scior, K, Ratti, V, Strydom, A, King, M, Hassiotis, A. Discrimination and other barriers to accessing health care: perspectives of patients with mild and moderate intellectual disability and their carers. PLOS ONE. 2013;8(8):e70855. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0070855.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedman, C. Ableism, racism, and the quality of life of black, indigenous, people of colour with intellectual and developmental disabilities. J Appl Res Intellect Disabil. 2023;36(3):604614. doi: 10.1111/jar.13084.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedman, C. The impact of human service provider quality on the personal outcomes of people with intellectual and developmental disabilities. Front Rehabil Sci. 2021;2:780168. doi: 10.3389/fresc.2021.780168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitter, N, Ali, A, Scior, K. Stigma experienced by family members of people with intellectual and developmental disabilities: multidimensional construct. BJPsych Open. 2018;4(5):332338. doi: 10.1192/bjo.2018.39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collings, S, Dew, A, Gordon, T, Spencer, M, Dowse, L. Intersectional disadvantage: exploring differences between aboriginal and non-aboriginal parents with intellectual disability in the New South Wales child protection system. J Public Child Welf. 2018;12(2):170189. doi: 10.1080/15548732.2017.1379456.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Magaña, S, Parish, S, Morales, MA, Li, H, Fujiura, G. Racial and ethnic health disparities among people with intellectual and developmental disabilities. Intellect Dev Disabil. 2016;54(3):161172. doi: 10.1352/1934-9556-54.3.161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ali, A, Strydom, A, Hassiotis, A, Williams, R, King, M. A measure of perceived stigma in people with intellectual disability. Br J Psychiatry. 2008;193(5):410415. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.107.045823.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williamson, HJ, Chico-Jarillo, TM, Sasse, S, et al. A scoping review of health research with racially/Ethnically minoritized adults with intellectual and developmental disabilities. Dev Disabil Netw J. 2023;3(2):14 Google ScholarPubMed
sen-van Vuuren, J, Aldersey, HM. Stigma, acceptance and belonging for people with IDD across cultures. Curr Dev Disord Rep. 2020;7(3):163172. doi: 10.1007/s40474-020-00206-w.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shannon, G, Morgan, R, Zeinali, Z, et al. Intersectional insights into racism and health: not just a question of identity. Lancet. 2022;400(10368):21252136. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(22)02304-2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson, DB, Testa, A, Turney, K. Unpacking the connection between parental incarceration and parenting stress: the mediating role of child health and health care strains. J Crim Just. 2022;81:101918. doi: 10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2022.101918.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz-Soicher, O, Geller, A, Garfinkel, I. The effect of paternal incarceration on material hardship. Soc Serv Rev. 2011;85(3):447473. doi: 10.1086/661925.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Di Nuovo, S, Buono, S. Behavioral phenotypes of genetic syndromes with intellectual disability: comparison of adaptive profiles. Psychiatry Res. 2011;30(3):440445. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.03.015.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tassé, MJ, Kim, M. Examining the relationship between adaptive behavior and intelligence. Behav Sci (Basel). 2023;13(3): doi: 10.3390/bs13030252.Google ScholarPubMed
Zheng, S, LeWinn, K, Ceja, T, Hanna-Attisha, M, O’Connell, L, Bishop, S. Adaptive behavior as an alternative outcome to intelligence quotient in studies of children at risk: a study of preschool-aged children in Flint, MI, USA. Front Psychol. 2021;12:692330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graif, C, Matthews, SA. The long arm of poverty: extended and relational geographies of child victimization and neighborhood violence exposures. Justice Q. 2017;34(6):10961125. doi: 10.1080/07418825.2016.1276951.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tunstall, R, Bevan, M, Bradshaw, J, et al. The Links Between Housing and Poverty: An Evidence Review. New York: Joh Rowntree Foundation; 2013.Google Scholar
Ridge, T. Childhood Poverty: A Barrier to Social Participation and Inclusion. Children, Young People and Social Inclusion, in Children, Young People and Social Inclusion: Participation for What? Bristol, UK: Bristol University Press; 2006:23–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steinbrenner, JR, McIntyre, N, Rentschler, LF, et al. Patterns in reporting and participant inclusion related to race and ethnicity in autism intervention literature: data from a large-scale systematic review of evidence-based practices. Autism. 2022;26(8):20262040. doi: 10.1177/13623613211072593.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rees, CA, Stewart, AM, Mehta, S, et al. Reporting of participant race and ethnicity in published US pediatric clinical trials from 2011 to 2020. JAMA Pediatr. 2022;176(5):e220142. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2022.0142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flanagin, A, Frey, T, Christiansen, SL, AMA Manual of Style Committee. Updated guidance on the reporting of race and ethnicity in medical and science journals. JAMA. 2021;17(7):621627. doi: 10.1001/jama.2021.13304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Emerson, E. Mothers of children and adolescents with intellectual disability: social and economic situation, mental health status, and the self-assessed social and psychological impact of the child’s difficulties. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2003;47(Pt 4-5):385399. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2788.2003.00498.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miodrag, N, Peters, S. Parent stress across molecular subtypes of children with Angelman syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2015;59(9):816826. doi: 10.1111/jir.12195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Acevedo-Garcia, D, McArdle, N, Hardy, EF, et al. The child opportunity index: improving collaboration between community development and public health. Health Aff (Millwood). 2014;33(11):19481957. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.2014.0679.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kind, AJH, Buckingham, WR. Making neighborhood-disadvantage metrics accessible - the neighborhood atlas. N Engl J Med. 2018;378(26):24562458. doi: 10.1056/NEJMp1802313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kalb, L, Lieb, R, Ludwig, N, et al. Association between neighborhood deprivation and child cognition in clinically referred youth. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2023;44(8):e543e550.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carter, RT, Mazzula, S, Victoria, R, et al. Initial development of the race-based traumatic stress symptom scale: assessing the emotional impact of racism. Psychol Trauma Theory Res Pract Policy. 2013;5(1):19. doi: 10.1037/a0025911.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sinclair, J, Hansen, SG, Machalicek, W, et al. A 16-year review of participant diversity in intervention research across a selection of 12 special education journals. Except Children. 2018;84(3):312329. doi: 10.1177/0014402918756989.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carrington, C, Devleesschauwer, B, Gibb, HJ, Bolger, PM. Global burden of intellectual disability resulting from dietary exposure to lead, 2015. Environ Res. 2019;172:420429. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2019.02.023.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shinn, M, Schteingart, JS, Williams, NC, et al. Long-term associations of homelessness with children’s well-being. Am Behav Sci. 2008;51(6):789809. doi: 10.1177/0002764207311988.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butler, O, Yang, XF, Laube, C, Kuhn, S, Immordino-Yang, MH. Community violence exposure correlates with smaller gray matter volume and lower IQ in urban adolescents. Hum Brain Mapp. 2018;39(5):20882097. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23988.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kira, IA, Lewandowski, L, Ashby, JS, Somers, C, Chiodo, L, Odenat, L. Does bullying victimization suppress IQ? The effects of bullying victimization on IQ in Iraqi and African American adolescents: a traumatology perspective. J Aggress Maltreatment Trauma. 2014;23(5):431453. doi: 10.1080/10926771.2014.904463.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities. Early Intervention. https://www.aaidd.org/news-policy/policy/position-statements/early-intervention. Accessed October 20, 2023.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Means and standard deviations of full-scale IQ (FSIQ) for common syndromic forms of intellectual developmental disorder

Figure 1

Figure 1. “Double hit” model. The pathogenic genetic variant contributes to the primary reduction in IQ in people with syndromic IDDs. Secondary genetic, environmental, epigenetic, and random factors contribute to smaller variations in IQ and result in a downshifted, but widely distributed, range of IQ scores. IQ = intelligence quotient; IDD = intellectual developmental disabilities.

Figure 2

Figure 2. Social determinants of health identified by the US Department of Health and Human Services’ Healthy People 2030 Initiative [25]. Domains discussed in the present narrative review are depicted in darkened boxes. Figure created with BioRender.com.

Figure 3

Figure 3. Potential pathway model for the impact of economic instability on IQ in individuals with IDDs. Note that many effects are bidirectional (e.g., economic instability limits access to reliable transportation which further fuels economic instability). Solid boxes and arrows reflect factors and pathways, respectively, that have been examined in individuals with syndromic IDDs. Dashed boxes and arrows reflect hypothesized factors and pathways that are presently unexplored in syndromic IDDs. IQ = intelligence quotient; IDD = intellectual developmental disabilities.

Figure 4

Table 2. Recommendations to delineate relations between social-environmental factors and IQ in individuals with syndromic IDDs