Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T18:55:25.241Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Developmental language disorder in sequential bilinguals: Characterising word properties in spontaneous speech

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 April 2022

Fódhla NÍ CHÉILEACHAIR
Affiliation:
Faculty of Arts: Neurolinguistics, Rijksuniversiteit Groningen, the Netherlands
Vasiliki CHONDROGIANNI
Affiliation:
School of Philosophy, Psychology, and Language Sciences, The University of Edinburgh, UK
Antonella SORACE
Affiliation:
School of Philosophy, Psychology, and Language Sciences, The University of Edinburgh, UK
Johanne PARADIS
Affiliation:
Faculty of Arts, Linguistics Department, The University of Alberta, Canada
Vânia DE AGUIAR*
Affiliation:
Faculty of Arts: Neurolinguistics, Rijksuniversiteit Groningen, the Netherlands
*
Corresponding author: Vania de Aguiar, Faculty of Arts: Neurolinguistics, Rijksuniversiteit Groningen, the Netherlands. E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The current study sought to investigate whether word properties can facilitate the identification of developmental language disorder (DLD) in sequential bilinguals by analyzing properties in nouns and verbs in L2 spontaneous speech as potential DLD markers. Measures of semantic (imageability, concreteness), lexical (frequency, age of acquisition) and phonological (phonological neighbourhood, word length) properties were computed for nouns and verbs produced by 15 sequential bilinguals (5;7) with DLD and 15 age-matched controls with diverse L1 backgrounds. Linear mixed modelling revealed a significant interaction of group and word category on phonological neighbourhood values but no differences across imageability, concreteness, frequency, age of acquisition, and word length measures in spontaneous speech. Outcomes suggest that group-level differences may not be apparent at the word-level, due to the heterogeneous nature of DLD and potential similarities in production during early L2 acquisition.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Developmental language disorder (DLD) is an enduring and dynamic neurodevelopmental disorder, resulting in delayed and impaired language acquisition in the absence of a biomedical cause (McMurray, Klein-Packard, McMurray & Tomblin, Reference McMurray, Klein-Packard, McMurray and Tomblin2019). DLD is characterized by a language level lagging significantly behind age-matched peers, with impairments across lexical and morphosyntactic facets of production and comprehension (Bishop, Reference Bishop2017). Recent projects estimate that DLD affects between 7-10% of children (Leonard, Reference Leonard2014; Vender, Garraffa, Sorace & Guasti, Reference Vender, Garraffa, Sorace and Guasti2016). Revisions in diagnostic practice have concluded that children with DLD form a highly heterogenous group, featuring a range of language-based impairments (Bishop, Reference Bishop2017). However, despite revisions, a persistent issue is the lack of sensitivity in language measures for early identification of DLD. This issue is particularly concerning for bilinguals, for whom there is a substantial rate of misdiagnosis (Grimm & Schulz, Reference Grimm and Schulz2014).

The present exploratory study seeks to identify whether word properties can contribute to accurate DLD identification among sequential bilinguals. As typically-developing (TD) bilinguals can exhibit transient delays in acquisition compared to monolinguals, monolingually-normed instruments can produce a misdiagnosis of DLD (Bedore & Peña, Reference Bedore and Peña2008). These conditions pose particular challenges for sequential bilinguals, who acquire their second language (L2) later than their first language (Paradis, Reference Paradis2010) and may be likely to display apparent delays in L2 acquisition compared to monolinguals. As sequential bilinguals are among the largest bilingual subgroups, due to bilingual education schemes and global immigration, identification of sensitive single-language testing in L2 is currently sought, as resources and services in a minority L1 may prove challenging to access (Paradis, Schneider & Sorenson-Duncan, Reference Paradis, Schneider and Sorenson-Duncan2013). Given the implications for high misdiagnosis rates, clarification on characteristics of bilingual DLD across several facets is required. The present study therefore proposes an analysis of semantic, lexical, and phonological word properties in the speech of affected and TD bilinguals for a comprehensive view of word knowledge in bilingual DLD.

Bilingualism and the diagnosis of DLD

In the bilingual context, in which an individual uses two or more languages regularly in daily life (Grosjean, Reference Grosjean1997), prevalent use of standardized measures normed for monolinguals can contribute substantially to misdiagnosis (Vender et al., Reference Vender, Garraffa, Sorace and Guasti2016). Use of such measures can result in TD bilinguals performing below monolingual peers in vocabulary and morphosyntactic acquisition, in the absence of impairment (Chondrogianni & Marinis, Reference Chondrogianni and Marinis2011). Generally, a number of factors influence bilingual development, including daily language use, length and amount of L2 exposure, language dominance, the number of speakers with whom a bilingual interacts, and environmental factors, such as parental proficiency and home language use (Chondrogianni & Marinis, Reference Chondrogianni and Marinis2011). Each of these factors may be neglected by the use of monolingual normed-measures and result in overdiagnosis. Misdiagnosis can extend to either extreme, however; resulting in overdiagnosis of DLD among TD bilinguals due to expectations from monolingual norms or underdiagnosis of DLD, from lack of sensitivity to differences between affected and TD bilinguals (Bedore & Peña, Reference Bedore and Peña2008).

Current measures of diagnosing DLD in the bilingual context tend to be language-specific and within the domain of morphosyntax (Garraffa, Vender, Sorace & Guasti, Reference Garraffa, Vender, Sorace and Guasti2019). Differences between TD bilinguals and bilinguals with DLD have been observed in the impaired production of tense-marking morphemes in French among French–English bilinguals (Paradis, Crago, Genesee & Rice, Reference Paradis, Crago, Genesee and Rice2003) and in erroneous subject-verb agreement in Dutch among Frisian–Dutch bilinguals (Spoelman & Bol, Reference Spoelman and Bol2012), among others. Beyond morphosyntax, differentiating markers may manifest in phonological markers, such as non-word repetition and/or sentence-repetition tasks (Gathercole & Baddeley, Reference Gathercole and Baddeley1990; Vender et al., Reference Vender, Garraffa, Sorace and Guasti2016). Notably, children with DLD also tend to perform poorly on non-word repetition tasks compared to TD peers (Arslan, Broc, Mathy & Olive, Reference Arslan, Broc, Mathy and Olive2020). Considering the rate of misdiagnosis in the bilingual context (Bedore & Peña, Reference Bedore and Peña2008), however, these markers may be insufficient for accurate evaluation. The recurrent issue of misdiagnosis poses a significant difficulty in clinical fields, as failing to recognize the needs of bilinguals limits their access to language-based services. Testing the language capacities of sequential bilinguals requires markers of greater sensitivity to the bilingual experience of DLD. In particular, examining markers beyond morphosyntax may be beneficial. The approach of the current study, therefore, focuses on single-language, L2 testing and seeks to investigate whether certain word properties in speech can illustrate differences between TD sequential bilinguals and those with DLD.

Word Learning Impairments in DLD

Word-learning deficits in DLD are prevalent cross-linguistically across grammatical categories (Skipp, Windfuhr & Conti-Ramsden, Reference Skipp, Windfuhr and Conti-Ramsden2002), and manifest in slow vocabulary acquisition and poor performance on naming and fluency tasks (McGregor & Appel, Reference McGregor and Appel2002). Children with DLD also tend to rely on nouns more than verbs, due to the syntactic complexity of the latter (Gentner, Reference Gentner, Hirsh-Pasek and Golinkoff2006). While no clear cause of DLD has been determined, word-learning impairments in DLD have been attributed to potential processing deficits in phonological short-term memory and weaker skills in matching form and semantics, constraining the ability to acquire novel words (Gathercole & Baddeley, Reference Gathercole and Baddeley1990; Nash & Donaldson, Reference Nash and Donaldson2005). The interplay between these concepts may be best represented using a model of single-word processing, from Rofes, Mandonnet, de Aguiar, Rapp, Tsapkini and Miceli (Reference Rofes, Zakariás, Ceder, Lind, Johansson, de Aguiar, Bjekić, Fyndanis, Gavarró, Simonsen, Sacristán, Kambanaros, Kraljević, Martínez-Ferreiro, Mavis, Orellana, Sör, Lukács, Tunçer and Howard2018) (based on Whitworth, Webster & Howard, Reference Whitworth, Webster and Howard2014), where word production depends on the feed forward progression from lexical-semantics to the phonological output lexicon for form retrieval. The flow then progresses to the phonological output buffer, which functions as a temporary storage unit for assembled phonological units prior to articulation (Dotan & Friedmann, Reference Dotan and Friedmann2015). Deficits within DLD are attributed to weaknesses within this flow, resulting in word-learning difficulties.

Lexical processing deficits in DLD

Regarding lexical entries in DLD, Kail, Hale, Leonard and Nippold (Reference Kail, Hale, Leonard and Nippold1984) developed the storage-elaboration hypothesis, in which delayed language development may result in impoverished lexical representations in DLD. In accordance with the storage-elaboration hypothesis, children with DLD retain smaller lexicons and reduced familiarity with words in their lexicons, incurring access and retrieval issues (McGregor & Appel, Reference McGregor and Appel2002). Impoverished lexical entries may manifest at (1) the conceptual level within lexical-semantics, as limited semantic detail per entry or (2) at the level of the lexeme within the phonological output lexicon and/or the phonological buffer, as reduced knowledge of and/or ability to retain phonological forms. In either case, impoverished entries are posited to result in difficulties connecting to other entries, given insufficient detail to form connections based on semantic or phonological similarity, and influencing aspects of processing, due to network limitations between items (Brackenbury & Pye, Reference Brackenbury and Pye2005; Gathercole & Baddeley, Reference Gathercole and Baddeley1990; Nash & Donaldson, Reference Nash and Donaldson2005). As such, measures reflecting the richness of semantic representations (e.g., imageability, concreteness) or relations between representations (e.g., phonological neighbourhood) may be diagnostically informative.

The case of impoverished lexical entries in DLD has received empirical support (McGregor, Oleson, Bahnsen & Duff, Reference McGregor, Oleson, Bahnsen and Duff2013; Seiger-Gardner & Schwartz, Reference Seiger-Gardner and Schwartz2008). In assessing production of definitions, McGregor et al. (Reference McGregor, Oleson, Bahnsen and Duff2013) observed that children with DLD retain less knowledge for lexical items by measuring both vocabulary breadth; the number of known words, and vocabulary depth; how well these words are known. Deficits were exemplified by both measures, as children with DLD knew fewer words than their TD counterparts and produced limited definitions of known words. Seiger-Gardner and Schwartz (Reference Seiger-Gardner and Schwartz2008) obtained complementary results using cross-modal picture-word interference tasks to measure picture-naming with both semantic and phonological distractors. As affected participants exhibited significantly longer times to rule out semantically-related distractors, the authors argue that semantic deficits feature as primary issues in DLD.

Phonological Deficits in DLD

Phonological aspects of word knowledge may also reflect vulnerabilities in DLD (Gathercole & Baddeley, Reference Gathercole and Baddeley1990; Leonard, Reference Leonard2014; Sheng & McGregor, Reference Sheng and McGregor2010). Notably, Sheng and McGregor (Reference Sheng and McGregor2010) observed that children with DLD produce phonologically-driven responses rather than semantically-related words in word association tasks. While school-attending children typically shift from a reliance on phonological information to semantic information in word-association tasks (Cronin, Reference Cronin2002), children with DLD may exhibit an enduring reliance on phonological qualities. Moreover, deficits in phonological working memory have been specifically implicated in monolinguals with DLD (Arslan et al., Reference Arslan, Broc, Mathy and Olive2020; Gathercole & Baddeley, Reference Gathercole and Baddeley1990) and sequential bilinguals with DLD (Engel de Abreu & Gathercole, Reference Engel de Abreu and Gathercole2012). Gathercole and Baddeley (Reference Gathercole and Baddeley1990) originally postulated that limitations in phonological storage impinge on word-learning in DLD, particularly as measures of phonological memory correlate positively with vocabulary. Difficulties temporarily storing phonological information within the phonological buffer may constrain word-learning ability and vice versa, as affected children perform consistently poorly on non-word repetition tasks (Coady & Evans, Reference Coady and Evans2008; Vender et al., Reference Vender, Garraffa, Sorace and Guasti2016). Among pertinent factors to consider when examining phonological memory, word length may play a role in word-learning, as children with DLD may struggle to acquire words of greater length in light of deficits within phonological working memory, relying disproportionately on shorter words (Gathercole & Baddeley, Reference Gathercole and Baddeley1990). Over time, phonological memory deficits may impact adversely on semantic aspects of word knowledge, as the flow of mapping meaning to form is disrupted (Nash & Donaldson, Reference Nash and Donaldson2005).

Word Learning in Typically-Developing Bilinguals

Application of the storage-elaboration hypothesis and phonological storage deficits in DLD may clarify differences between TD sequential bilinguals and bilinguals with DLD. While TD bilinguals may appear to demonstrate delays in vocabulary development (Gollan, Montoya, Cera & Sandoval, Reference Gollan, Montoya, Cera and Sandoval2008), storage deficits along semantic and/or phonological aspects of lexical acquisition are not considered the root cause. The frequency-lag hypothesis of Gollan and colleagues (Reference Gollan, Montoya, Cera and Sandoval2008) postulates that bilingual children may demonstrate word processing delays as using a dual-language system may result in splitting the frequency of word use between both languages. Bilinguals may typically have fewer opportunities for word exposure and production in both languages, resulting in potentially lower frequencies of activation for items (Gollan et al., Reference Gollan, Montoya, Cera and Sandoval2008). Moreover, the effect of divided engagement between both languages may lead to the formation of ‘weaker links’ between the semantics and phonology of lexical entries when compared to monolingual peers (Gollan et al., Reference Gollan, Montoya, Cera and Sandoval2008). Consequently, word representations of TD bilinguals are not considered impoverished, however, but merely more difficult to access, given the frequency reduction per item.

Looking to differentiating markers between affected and TD bilinguals, Marini, Sperindè, Ruta, Savegnago and Avanzini (Reference Marini, Sperindè, Ruta, Savegnago and Avanzini2019) noted that Italian–German bilinguals with DLD between the ages of 7-10 years demonstrated differences in lexical skills and phonological memory abilities when compared to age-matched controls. Bilinguals with DLD demonstrated reduced phonological short-term memory capacities, lower scores on vocabulary comprehension and naming tasks, and a significantly higher rate of semantic errors. Differences in processing abilities between bilinguals with DLD and TD controls between the ages of 9 – 14 years in single-language testing were also observed by Degani, Kreiser and Novogrodsky (Reference Degani, Kreiser and Novogrodsky2019). Specifically, the authors contrasted monolingual Hebrew-speaking and Hebrew–English bilingual groups with and without DLD on picture-naming tasks to mark interaction between bilingualism, DLD, and item frequency. Bilinguals with DLD exhibited significantly poorer performances and displayed larger item frequency effects than TD peers. In this sense, bilinguals with and without DLD can be accurately differentiated from one another and, moreover, the investigation of semantic, lexical and phonological word properties may aid group differentiation.

Characterising word properties in nouns and verbs

Measures of word properties in nouns and verbs have previously been used to characterize the language profiles of individuals with aphasia (Rofes, de Aguiar, Ficek, Wendt, Webster & Tsapkini, Reference Rofes, de Aguiar, Ficek, Wendt, Webster and Tsapkini2019). As children with DLD are posited to retain impoverished lexical entries whether due to or in addition to phonological storage deficits, a similar approach may clarify these positions by examining the spontaneous speech of affected bilinguals. Moreover, children with DLD tend to struggle with verbs more so than with nouns in English (Thordardottir & Weismer, Reference Thordardottir and Weismer2001). This disparity is attributed to the syntactic complexity of verbs, rendering them more difficult to acquire (Gentner, Reference Gentner, Hirsh-Pasek and Golinkoff2006; Skipp et al., Reference Skipp, Windfuhr and Conti-Ramsden2002). In general, children with DLD appear restricted to a smaller verb lexicon, tending to produce a greater number of nouns. In this case, analysis of noun and verb use and their contingent word properties in speech may aid DLD characterization in bilinguals.

Considering the storage-elaboration hypothesis, deficits in semantic representations of lexical items may manifest in semantic word properties, such as imageability and concreteness. Imageability, defined as the ease with which a mental image is evoked by a word, is considered a measure of semantic feature richness (Bird, Howard & Franklin, Reference Bird, Howard and Franklin2003). Concreteness, while thematically similar to imageability and highly correlated, refers to the degree to which a concept is tangible (Brysbaert, Warriner & Kuperman, Reference Brysbaert, Warriner and Kuperman2014; Rofes et al., Reference Rofes, Zakariás, Ceder, Lind, Johansson, de Aguiar, Bjekić, Fyndanis, Gavarró, Simonsen, Sacristán, Kambanaros, Kraljević, Martínez-Ferreiro, Mavis, Orellana, Sör, Lukács, Tunçer and Howard2018). Both have been implicated as highly relevant factors in early word-learning, as Howell and Becker (Reference Howell and Becker2001) argue that both define the ease with which a word is learnt. Highly imageable words also tend to be processed with greater ease (Montgomery, Gillam, Evans, Schwartz & Fargo, Reference Montgomery, Gillam, Evans, Schwartz and Fargo2019) and highly concrete words are deemed easier in recall tasks than abstract items (Sadoski, Goetz & Fritz, Reference Sadoski, Goetz and Fritz2016). Given the roles attributed to imageability and concreteness in early word-learning, it is predicted that children with DLD rely on highly imageable and concrete words in their production.

Frequency and age of acquisition are two lexical measures related to the ease of lexical selection and/or access (Gibson, Peña & Bedore, Reference Gibson, Peña and Bedore2014) and may reflect characteristic differences between bilinguals with/out DLD. Firstly, a reliance on highly frequent words may be a discernible trait among sequential bilinguals with DLD (Levie, Ben-Zvi & Ravid, Reference Levie, Ben-Zvi and Ravid2017; Nash & Donaldson, Reference Nash and Donaldson2005). This position is supported by Brackenbury and Pye (Reference Brackenbury and Pye2005), who posit that lexical deficits among children with DLD are connected to impairments within the phonological output lexicon. Frequency effects may reflect the organization of lexical entries or access to these entries (Friedmann, Biran & Dotan, Reference Friedmann, Biran and Dotan2013). Examination of frequency measures may be sufficiently sensitive to provide accurate distinctions between TD and affected sequential bilinguals, as bilinguals with DLD exhibit greater reliance on frequent items when compared to control groups (Degani et al., Reference Degani, Kreiser and Novogrodsky2019). The age of acquisition (AoA) of a word also influences word processing and correlates negatively with frequency measures (Montgomery et al., Reference Montgomery, Gillam, Evans, Schwartz and Fargo2019). Words learnt early in life tend to be processed faster and with greater ease than words acquired later (Ghyselinck, Lewis & Brysbaert, Reference Ghyselinck, Lewis and Brysbaert2004). Both lexical frequency and AoA are argued to be potent predictors of lexical naming and processing (Colombo & Burani, Reference Colombo and Burani2002), in which highly frequent words and those acquired at a younger age facilitate processing.

Looking to phonological word properties and word category, verbs generally tend to be shorter than nouns in English (Black & Chiat, Reference Black and Chiat2003). Word-learning is also affected by word length (Gathercole & Baddeley, Reference Gathercole and Baddeley1990) and phonological neighbourhood, defined as the number of lexemes with overlapping phonology when one phoneme of that word is altered, either through deletion, substitution or addition (Leonard, Reference Leonard2014). Mainela-Arnold, Evans and Coady (Reference Mainela-Arnold, Evans and Coady2010) proposed that children with DLD struggle to distinguish phonologically similar words, indicating that words with dense neighbourhoods cause great difficulty in production. Other studies have indicated the words with a greater number of neighbours may generally be accessed with greater ease than those with fewer neighbours (Vitevich & Sommers, Reference Vitevich and Sommers2003). As such, the influence of phonological neighbourhood density on word-learning remains inconclusive. However, words with many phonological neighbours are also typically shorter and neighbourhood effects related to lexical impairments may co-exist with greater ease in producing longer words with fewer neighbours (Storkel, Reference Storkel2004). Word length may otherwise affect performance in the opposite direction, as shorter words may be processed with greater ease than longer words. This word length effect may be attributed to a storage deficit in phonological working memory, specifically the phonological buffer (Gathercole & Baddeley, Reference Gathercole and Baddeley1990).

The present study

The present study proposes exploratory analysis of word properties in nouns and verbs in the production of age-matched sequential bilinguals with and without DLD. Spontaneous speech samples in L2 English will be analysed for possible effects of word properties by comparing affected sequential bilinguals and their TD peers. The following six-word properties will be analysed: i) imageability, ii) concreteness, iii) frequency, iv) age of acquisition, v) phonological neighbourhood and vi) word length in phonemes, extracted separately for nouns and verbs. As theories of phonological working memory deficits and/or the presence of impoverished lexical representations in DLD have gained empirical support, potential manifestations of these impairments will be examined through the effects of these word properties. Given the exploratory nature of this study, the heterogeneity of DLD and influential effects of bilingual factors, such as typological distance between languages, general predictions for the performance of sequential bilinguals with DLD were formulated alone.

Research Question

Do patterns of word properties within nouns and verbs in spontaneous speech reflect group-level differences between TD sequential bilinguals and sequential bilinguals with DLD?

Predictions for Sequential Bilinguals with DLD

Sequential bilinguals with DLD are predicted to demonstrate:

  1. i) Predominant use of nouns, rather than verbs, in spontaneous speech,

In relation to word properties therein,

  1. i) Greater reliance on highly imageable, and concrete lexical items,

  2. ii) Greater reliance on highly frequent lexical items with low AoA values,

  3. iii) Greater reliance on lexical items with smaller numbers of phonemes and/or a reliance on words with smaller phonological neighbourhoods.

Method

Participants

Spontaneous speech transcripts of 30 sequential bilinguals were included in the current study: 15 TD bilinguals and 15 bilinguals with a diagnosis of DLD. Participants were selected from a larger sample, which was originally collected by Paradis and colleagues (Reference Paradis, Schneider and Sorenson-Duncan2013) in Edmonton and Toronto regions of Canada. The original sample comprised 252 TD sequential bilinguals and 28 sequential bilinguals with DLD. Generally, these children came from newcomer families who had immigrated to Canada with parents born outside of Canada, all of whom spoke a non-English L1. Exclusionary criteria for the overall sample included diagnosis of ASD, diagnosis of hearing impairment, known speech-sound disorders and/or evidence of severe intellectual disability. While diversity of L1 is notable across the sample (see Table 1), all children were primarily exposed to their L1 during the first 2-3 years of life and began acquiring English as L2 upon attending English-medium preschool programmes. The current sample was selected using the matched-pairs approach: each child within the DLD grouping was matched with a counterpart from the TD group, where possible, using L1, age at testing and length of exposure to English (months) as control variables.

Table 1. L1 Background of Participants

Typically-developing sequential bilinguals

15 TD sequential bilinguals were included in the current study; 11 male and 4 female. The mean chronological age of children was 69 months (range = 58-78 months, standard deviation = 5.7 months). TD children were exposed to an average of 23.5 months of English prior to recruitment. During original data collection, it was established that the home language was predominantly L1 for twelve participants, both L1 and English for one participant and predominantly English for two participants. Regarding child language production in the home, nine participants produced mostly their L1, four participants produced both their L1 and English equally, and two participants produced mostly English. For the original sample, participating TD children were recruited from both schools in the region and from contact with agencies that aid newly-immigrated families (see Paradis et al., Reference Paradis, Schneider and Sorenson-Duncan2013 for details on recruitment).

Sequential bilinguals with DLD

15 sequential bilinguals with a diagnosis of DLD were selected for the current study; 11 male and 4 female. The mean age of children within this grouping was 67 months (range = 60-76 months, standard deviation = 5.11 months). Children within this grouping had an average of 26 months of exposure to English. The home language across bilinguals with DLD was predominantly L1 for seven participants, both L1 and English for five participants, and predominantly English for three participants. In terms of child language production, six participants produced mainly L1, one participant typically produced both L1 and English equally, and eight participants produced mostly English at home. Sequential bilinguals with a diagnosis of DLD had been referred to the original study by speech and language therapists within schools and/or from specific preschool programmes catering to children with language delays.

Both groups were matched for chronological age of testing and exposure length to English (see Table 2). Additional characteristics pertaining to both groups are displayed in Table 2, including measures of language and development, nonverbal IQ, and socioeconomic status as represented by the years of mothers’ education. A significant difference was observed in mothers’ years of education between both groups (t = 2.28, p < .05), in which the mothers of TD sequential bilinguals had spent a greater number of years in education. Standardised scores on the Columbia Mental Maturity Scales (CMMS; Burgemeister, Blum & Lorge, Reference Burgemeister, Blum and Lorge1954) accompanied the transcripts of the current sample, and indicated that nonverbal IQ scores among TD sequential bilinguals were significantly higher than those within the DLD grouping, (t = 4.571, p < .05).

Table 2. Participant Characteristics across TD and DLD Groups

* indicates a significant difference

Additionally, measures of language development were obtained for each participating child. During the original data collection of Paradis et al. (Reference Paradis, Schneider and Sorenson-Duncan2013), scores of language and development were compiled using the Alberta Language Development Questionnaire (ALDEQ; Paradis, Emmerzael & Sorenson-Duncan, Reference Paradis, Emmerzael and Sorenson Duncan2010), values of mean length of utterance (MLU) and two measures of lexical diversity, defined as the scope of vocabulary used by an individual: Type Token Ratio (TTR) and D (Owen & Leonard, Reference Owen and Leonard2002) using CLAN (MacWhinney, Reference MacWhinney2003). TTR refers to the total number of different words used divided by the total number of words uttered, while D is the index of modelling a curve of multiple TTR samplings and identifying a model of best fit. Both TTR and D are considered reliable indices of lexical diversity in child language (Owen & Leonard, Reference Owen and Leonard2002).

As anticipated, total scores from the ALDEQ indicated that TD bilinguals obtained higher scores in language development compared to their peers with DLD (t = 7.838, p < .05). Similarly, differences between groups based on MLU were observed (t = 2.445, p < 0.05), where MLU values across TD sequential bilinguals were higher than those of bilinguals with DLD. Neither measure of lexical diversity indicated significant differences between groups; D (t = 1.348, p > 0.05) and TTR (t = -0.05, p > 0.05).

Procedure

Transcripts of free-play interactions were obtained from the original research (Paradis et al., Reference Paradis, Schneider and Sorenson-Duncan2013), which took place within school and home settings. During the recording sessions, children were engaged in conversation with the researcher in the presence of an additional observer. For each participating child in the current sample, a transcript with a minimum of 100 utterances during each recording session was analysed.

Data analysis

Nouns and verbs in each transcript were identified using CLAN (MacWhinney, Reference MacWhinney2003) with the following code: freq +t*CHI +z100u. Using the freq command, frequency information for each word produced by the child was tabulated within their first 100 utterances. Nouns and verbs were then manually extracted. In the case of ambiguity regarding lexical category – for instance, a lexical item that is used as both a noun and a verb (e.g hug, dress) – grammatical category was determined by examining the transcript for the speaker’s use and then labelling it accordingly. Nouns and verbs produced by each participant were then compiled into separate files and loaded into the N-Watch programme (Davis, Reference Davis2005) separately for each participant. Values for frequency (CELEX database: (Baayen, Gulikers & Piepenbrock, Reference Baayen, Gulikers and Piepenbrock1995)), the number of phonemes, phonological neighbourhood, and imageability (Bristol norms database: Stadthagen-Gonzalez & Davis, Reference Stadthagen-Gonzalez and Davis2006) were obtained within the N-Watch programme. Values for concreteness (from Brysbaert et al., Reference Brysbaert, Warriner and Kuperman2014) and AoA (from Kuperman, Stadthagen-Gonzalez & Brysbaert, Reference Kuperman, Stadthagen-Gonzalez and Brysbaert2012) were then identified manually for each item.

For nouns, where a child produced the plural form of a nouns (e.g., dogs), the plural form was analysed for frequency, phonological neighbourhood, and length in phonemes. As most plural nouns were not available for imageability, concreteness, and AoA, ratings for the singular form were used where a plural rating was unavailable (n = 113 entries, 13% of items). Verb entries were initially examined in the form the child produced (e.g., past tense inflection.). Where the original form was unlisted, values for the base form were used (n = 68 entries, 8%). Words returning the value of “-1” across categories were excluded from the analysis, as this value served to indicate that the word was unlisted in the N-Watch programme. Out of 866 unique entries, the number of missing values across categories are as follows: imageability (n = 155 entries, 18%), concreteness (n = 8 entries, < 1%), frequency (n = 2 entries, < 1%), and AoA (n = 16 entries, 1%). Both phonological variables had complete datasets. After compiling values for each variable per target word, descriptive statistics were calculated per participant and grammatical category using RStudio (R Core Team, 2018; R Studio Team, 2019).

Statistical analysis

Proportional differences in the production of nouns and verbs were assessed using a two-sample test for equality of proportions. Word properties were then assessed using linear mixed models and the lme4 package (Bates, Mächler, Bolker & Walker, Reference Bates, Mächler, Bolker and Walker2015) with RStudio (R Core Team, 2018; R Studio Team, 2019). Additional packages used included “tidyverse” (Wickham et al., Reference Wickham, Averick, Bryan, Chang, McGovern, François, Grolemund, Hayes, Henry, Hester, Kuhn, Pedersen, Miller, Bache, Müller, Ooms, Robinson, Seidel, Spinu and Yutani2019) “ggplot2” (Wickham, Reference Wickham2016) and the “sjPlot” package (Lüdecke, Reference Lüdecke2021) for data visualisation. Given skewed distributions across imageability, concreteness, frequency and age of acquisition measures as anticipated for child language, these four variables were log-transformed to comply with assumptions of linear mixed modelling. Measures of phonological neighbourhood and number of phonemes were treated as count-based variables and analysed by generalised linear mixed models using the Poisson distribution and log canonical link. Each word property was analysed in a separate model with group membership (k = 2; TD, DLD) and grammatical category (k = 2; nouns, verbs) acting as predictor variables and with the inclusion of participant as a random effect. Preliminary models including (i) word and (ii) participant L1 as additional random effects resulted in model overfitting in both cases and were subsequently excluded from each model of best fit, as appraised using likelihood ratio tests described by Winter (Reference Winter2013).

Results

In this section, statistics relating to the proportional use of nouns and verbs are first presented, followed by descriptive and inferential statistics for each word property. Properties have been grouped within respective categories; semantic properties of imageability and concreteness are first presented, followed by lexical properties of frequency and AoA, and phonological properties of neighbourhood (PN) and word length in phonemes thereafter. For specifics relating to each model, see Appendices B and C.

Word Category

The production of nouns and verbs amongst TD sequential bilinguals and sequential bilinguals with DLD was tabulated, with the total number of produced nouns and verbs reported in Table 3. To examine the proportions of nouns and verbs used between sequential bilinguals with DLD and TD sequential bilinguals, a two-sample test for equality of proportions was computed using RStudio (R Core Team, 2018; R Studio Team, 2019). It was observed that the use of both nouns and verbs did not appear to differ proportionally between groups, ( $ {\chi}^2 $ = 0.6038, p = 0.435). Sequential bilinguals with DLD and TD sequential bilinguals did not differ in their proportional production of nouns and verbs, indicating that high reliance on nouns is not disproportionate in cases of DLD in this sample.

Table 3. Production of Grammatical Category across Groups

Word properties across nouns and verbs

To test whether values of word properties differed across groups and word category, separate linear mixed models were computed for i) imageability, ii) concreteness iii) AoA and iv) frequency using the lme4 package (Bates et al., Reference Bates, Mächler, Bolker and Walker2015) in R (R Core Team, 2018; R Studio Team, 2019) with the alpha level established at 0.05. PN and word length were treated as count response variables and required separate generalised linear mixed models using the Poisson distribution and log link function (Anderson, Verkuilen & Johnson, Reference Anderson, Verkuilen and Johnson2012). In each case, word properties were treated as functions of the following fixed effects; group (k = 2; TD/DLD) and word category (k = 2; Noun/Verb). As the overall dataset contained multiple observations per participant, participant was included as a random effect in each model to satisfy the independence assumption. For details relating to group means across L1, see Appendix A. In relation to contrasts, predictor variables were sum contrast coded (-0.5, 0.5) across both linear and generalised methods of model analyses for mean-centring to aid interpretation of main effects (Schad, Vasishth, Hohenstein & Kliegl, Reference Schad, Vasishth, Hohenstein and Kliegl2020). Table 4 reports means and standard deviations for each word property across group and word category.

Table 4. Mean and Standard Deviations of Word Properties

Semantic Properties: Imageability, Concreteness

A linear mixed model of log-transformed imageability ratings, lmer(log(Imageability) ~ Group + Word_Category + (1|participant)), revealed no significant differences across group ( $ \hat{\beta} $ = 0.016, standard error = 0.011, t = 1.494, p = 0.135). A significant main effect of word category on imageability ratings was observed ( $ \hat{\beta} $ = 0.388, se = 0.0105, t = 37.037, p < 0.05) in which nouns produced during spontaneous speech held higher ratings than verbs (see Figure 1). Similarly, a linear mixed model of log-transformed concreteness ratings, lmer(log(Concreteness) ~ Group + Word_Category + (1|participant)), revealed no significant differences across group ( $ \hat{\beta} $ = 0.0106, se = 0.0147, t = 0.725, p = 0.4683), though a main effect of word category was observed ( $ \hat{\beta} $ = 0.3789, se = 0.0121, t = 31.25, p < 0.05), in which concreteness ratings across nouns were also higher than those for verbs (see Figure 1). Model comparisons revealed that the model of best fit did not specify any interaction term between both fixed effects for imageability or concreteness. Generally, these results suggest that the words produced by TD sequential bilinguals and those with DLD do not differ based on their imageability or concreteness ratings, but that verbs produced by both groups were significantly lower in ratings of imageability and concreteness than nouns.

Figure 1. Boxplots of Imageability and Concreteness ratings across group and word category

Lexical Properties: Frequency, Age of Acquisition

A linear mixed model of log-transformed frequency ratings as a function of word category and group, lmer(log(Frequency) ~ Group + Word_Category + (1|participant)), indicated that group was not a significant predictor ( $ \hat{\beta} $ = 0.016, se = 0.0914, t = 0.179, p =0.858). A significant main effect of word category was observed for frequency ratings ( $ \hat{\beta} $ = -1.5698, se = 0.0845, t = -18.574, p < 0.05), in which verbs tended to have higher frequency ratings than the nouns produced (See Figure 2). Model comparisons revealed that the model of best fit did not specify any interaction term between both fixed effects for lexical frequency. Generally, results indicate that word category acts as a significant predictor of frequency ratings, with highly frequent verbs tending to emerge in spontaneous speech across both groups of children. A linear mixed model of log-transformed AoA ratings as a function of group and word category, lmer(log(AoA) ~ Group + Word_Category + (1|participant)), revealed that neither group ( $ \hat{\beta} $ = -0.0144, se = 0.016, t = -0.883, p = 0.377) nor word category ( $ \hat{\beta} $ = -0.012, se = 0.0120, t = -1.013, p = 0.3109) carried significant main effects (see Figure 2). In this case, effects of age of acquisition did not manifest in the use of nouns and verbs produced during spontaneous speech, revealing neither a word category effect nor a group effect. The average AoA rating for words produced across both groups was 4.23, suggesting that both groups tended to use words with lower AoA ratings than their age expectation of 5;9 and 5;7 respectively. Neither lexical property demonstrated group-led differences between patterns of frequency or AoA in words produced during spontaneous speech.

Figure 2. Boxplots of Frequency and Age of Acquisition ratings across group and word category

Phonological Properties: Phonological Neighbourhood, Word Length

A generalised linear mixed model using the Poisson distribution and the log scale as canonical link, glmer(PN ~ Group + Word_Category + Group*Word_Category +(1|participant), family = poisson(link = "log")), revealed a main effect of word category for phonological neighbourhood measures of words ( $ \hat{\beta} $ = -0.4758, SE = 0.013, z = -36.4218, p < 0.05) but no main effect of group ( $ \hat{\beta} $ = -0.0025, SE = 0.022, z = -0.1146, p = 0.909) (see Figure 3). A significant interaction between group and word category, however, was noted ( $ \hat{\beta} $ = -0.104783, SE = 0.026, z = -4.008, p < 0.05). Generally, this result indicates that sequential bilinguals with DLD and TD sequential bilinguals do not differ in production concerning the number of phonological neighbours a word may have, but that values for phonological neighbourhood for verbs were higher overall than values for nouns amongst the entire sample. Looking to the interaction effect marked in the model, as illustrated in an interaction plot in Figure 4, it is notable that the magnitude of the word category effect appears stronger across participants within the DLD grouping than their TD peers. In this case, the disparity between nouns and verbs in terms of phonological neighbours is more extreme for sequential bilinguals with DLD, who produced verbs with high numbers of phonological neighbours when compared to nouns.

Figure 3. Boxplots of Phonological Length and Phonological Neighbourhood ratings across group and word category

Figure 4. Interaction plot of Phonological Neighbourhood ratings across group and word category

Similarly to the measure of phonological neighbourhood, a generalised linear mixed model using the Poisson distribution and the log scale as canonical link was operationalised for word length in phonemes, glmer(LEN_P ~ Group + Word_Category +(1|participant), family = poisson(link = "log")), where no interaction effect was specified in the model of best fit. Values of word length in phonemes revealed a significant main effect of word category ( $ \hat{\beta} $ = 0.2499, se = 0.0246, z = 10.143, p < 0.05), in which nouns tended to be of greater length than verbs produced during spontaneous speech (see Figure 3). No significant main effect of group was observed ( $ \hat{\beta} $ = -0.0082, se = 0.0246, z = -0.333, p = 0.739). In this manner, group differences did not manifest in the word length in phonemes of nouns and verbs produced in spontaneous speech.

To account for the potential influence of length in phonemes on phonological neighbourhood, an exploratory model for phonological neighbourhood was computed to include the mean word length produced by each participant as a random slope. The exploratory generalised linear mixed model, glmer(PN ~ Group*Word_Category + (1|participant) + (mean word_length| participant), family = poisson(link = "log")), resulted in a singular fit, indicating an overfitted structure. Mean word length accounted for little variance in the overfitted model, while the interaction between group and word category remained significant while controlling for mean length across participants ( $ \hat{\beta} $ = -0.028, se = 0.006, z = -4.646, p < 0.05).

Discussion

The objective of the current study was to investigate whether word properties in nouns and verbs reflect differences between sequential bilinguals with and without DLD. As children with DLD are posited to exhibit impoverished lexical entries and/or phonological storage deficits, bilinguals with DLD were predicted to exhibit characteristic patterns of noun and verb use along semantic, lexical, and phonological properties. Nouns and verbs in spontaneous speech were analysed across six-word properties; imageability, concreteness, frequency, AoA, phonological neighbourhood and word length. Sequential bilinguals with DLD did not demonstrate an overreliance on nouns compared to their TD counterparts, as no proportional difference in noun and verb production between groups was noted. Using separate linear mixed-models for semantic and lexical properties and separate generalised mixed-models for phonological properties, no main effect of group emerged along any of the six word properties. Word category acted as a significant predictor of five of the listed properties, with age of acquisition as the lone exception. Phonological neighbourhood values revealed a model of best fit including an interaction term between group and word category, indicating that the magnitude of difference between nouns and verbs in this measurement was greater for sequential bilinguals with DLD. Broadly, results indicate that nouns, verbs, and their contingent word properties, with the exception of an interaction effect of group and word category on phonological neighbourhood, do not indicate characteristic differences between groups.

While sequential bilinguals with DLD are posited to rely predominantly on nouns and retain low levels of verb diversity (Sanz-Torrent, Serrat, Andreu & Serra, Reference Sanz-Torrent, Serrat, Andreu and Serra2008; Thordardottir & Weismer, Reference Thordardottir and Weismer2001), no clear difference in the proportions of nouns and verbs produced were observed between affected and TD groups. This may be partly attributable to the fact that both groups were relatively early in their acquisition of L2. Moreover, while children with DLD are posited to rely on ‘GAP verbs’ (Rice & Bode, Reference Rice and Bode1993), which may reveal possible group disparity, analysis of the quality of verbs used was beyond the scope of the current study.

Production of Word Properties in Spontaneous Speech

Regarding the storage-elaboration hypothesis (Kail et al., Reference Kail, Hale, Leonard and Nippold1984), bilinguals with DLD were predicted to rely on highly imageable and concrete words. The spontaneous speech of affected bilinguals did not differ from their TD counterparts, as both groups produced highly imageable and concrete items. This is particularly apparent in the case of concreteness, as the mean value for noun concreteness across both groups was 4.49, based on a rating scale of 1-5 (Brysbaert et al., Reference Brysbaert, Warriner and Kuperman2014). Though verbs tend to have much lower values of imageability and concreteness when compared to nouns, as affirmed by word category emerging as a significant predictor of both measures individually, verbs produced in the current study culminated in mean values toward the higher end of the scales. This pattern reflects the view of Howell and Becker (Reference Howell and Becker2001) that imageability and concreteness facilitate early word-learning, given their relation to interpretable sensorimotor experiences. The lack of group differentiation on these properties may be a further manifestation of early L2 word-learning, particularly as children in both affected and TD groups were appraised in L2 English.

Regarding lexical properties, both groups depended on highly frequent verbs, while frequency values for nouns were significantly lower. This finding follows the argument that verbs tend to be more challenging for children and specifically in the case of DLD, resulting in the use of highly frequent verbs (Thordardottir & Weismer, Reference Thordardottir and Weismer2001). This result is also consistent with predictions that sequential bilinguals with DLD were likely to rely on highly frequent items. A similar pattern was also noted for TD sequential bilinguals, however, resulting in a lack of group differentiation. This finding may align with the frequency-lag hypothesis (Gollan et al., Reference Gollan, Montoya, Cera and Sandoval2008), as TD bilinguals face reduced frequency of lexical items compared to monolinguals. Particularly as sequential bilinguals were appraised in L2 English, a reliance on highly frequent verbs in L2, regardless of impairment, may reflect an early word-learning strategy for L2. In this case, examining frequency values for words produced during spontaneous speech may be unlikely to clarify distinctions between affected and TD bilinguals. Further, AoA values reflected neither a word category nor a group difference. Generally, the average value for words produced by both groups was 4;3 years of age. Looking to the current study’s demographic, this mean value is over a year younger than the mean age for participants (see Table 2.), revealing that both groups exhibit a lag compared to norms for monolinguals. This finding is also unsurprising, as these children are acquiring English later than monolingual English speakers and will likely demonstrate a later trajectory of acquisition.

Looking to phonological properties, no group differences emerged based on word length while nouns produced by both groups were significantly longer than verbs. Generally, sequential bilinguals with DLD were predicted to rely on shorter words, as deficits within DLD are postulated to arise from a phonological storage impairment (Gathercole & Baddeley, Reference Gathercole and Baddeley1990). While this may be the case, TD sequential bilinguals demonstrated a similar pattern. Given no disparity in length effects between groups, elaboration on a possible phonological storage deficit in DLD requires further study in the bilingual population.

Additionally, research indicates that children with DLD retain difficulties in distinguishing phonological neighbours (Mainela-Arnold et al., Reference Mainela-Arnold, Evans and Coady2010; Storkel, Reference Storkel2004). While no main effect for group emerged for this property, verbs produced had significantly higher numbers of phonological neighbours than nouns. The significant interaction effect between group and word category as predictive of phonological neighbourhood density warrants further discussion, as the difference in phonological neighbour values between nouns and verbs was of greater magnitude for bilinguals with DLD. While small, this effect is contrary to expectations for acquisition in DLD, as greater density is associated with greater lexical competition, which is challenging for children with DLD with respect to phonological neighbours (Mainela-Arnold et al., Reference Mainela-Arnold, Evans and Coady2010). This finding is, however, in line with the research of Vitevich and Sommers (Reference Vitevich and Sommers2003), who generally posit that words with denser neighbourhoods, as noted among verbs in the current study, may be easier to access. As words with denser phonological neighbourhoods may be shorter in length (Storkel, Reference Storkel2004), a further avenue of study may be to investigate whether sequential bilinguals with and without DLD are using similar strategies to use shorter words at the expense of phonological neighbourhood, as no effect of length was noted. Exploratory analysis sought to account for the potential influence of average word length per participant on phonological neighbourhood values, resulting in an overfitted model. It is notable, however, that the interaction effect between word category and group remained significant while controlling for mean length. As such, while results cannot shed definitive support for a deficit in phonological storage mechanisms, as postulated by Gathercole and Baddeley (Reference Gathercole and Baddeley1990), usage patterns of both groups reflect the characteristic that verbs tend to be shorter than nouns in English (Black & Chiat, Reference Black and Chiat2003).

Generally, no main effect of group emerged based on semantic, lexical, and phonological properties in single words. Looking to theoretical implications, these outcomes cannot clarify the presence of impoverished lexical entries in the case of bilinguals with DLD, as posited by Kail et al. (Reference Kail, Hale, Leonard and Nippold1984). While speech produced by affected bilinguals reflected a reliance on highly imageable, concrete and frequent items that are low in AoA ratings, performance of TD bilinguals also conforms to these conditions. It is possible that differences may not be discernible at the single-word level, as both groups may be relying on similar strategies for early stages of L2 acquisition. This possibility is discussed in greater detail in the following sub-section. Additionally, while these results cannot shed definitive support for a deficit in phonological storage mechanisms, as postulated by Gathercole and Baddeley (Reference Gathercole and Baddeley1990), it is curious that TD sequential bilinguals produce similar patterns of use, though to a lesser magnitude than their affected peers. Elaboration on the possibility of children with DLD harbouring impoverished lexical entries (Kail et al., Reference Kail, Hale, Leonard and Nippold1984) and/or deficits in phonological storage (Gathercole & Baddeley, Reference Gathercole and Baddeley1990), however, can only be speculative and requires greater study, particularly in the area of phonological properties.

Similarities between Sequential Bilinguals with and without DLD

Several possible explanations may clarify the lack of group differences between TD sequential bilinguals and those with DLD. Firstly, the impairments arising within the classification of DLD are highly heterogeneous, with different children demonstrating varying impairments (Bishop, Reference Bishop2017). While word-learning deficits tend to be initial indicators of potential impairment (Sheng & McGregor, Reference Sheng and McGregor2010), certain children may have word knowledge deficits in the area of semantics, while others may indicate a deficit within the phonological buffer, resulting in noise at the group-level in word property analysis. Future studies may benefit from examining word properties using single-case statistical analysis (Crawford & Garthwaite, Reference Crawford and Garthwaite2002). Moreover, at the group-level, varying impairments encompassed by DLD may appear more pronounced in more complex language facets relying on word-learning abilities, like morphosyntax, rather than emerging at the single-word level. This may be particularly applicable to the current sample, as groups differed across measures of MLU but not in relation to lexical diversity (see Table 2).

Secondly, the bilingual experience of the current sample varies in terms of individual L2 exposure and the influence of both age and exposure to L2 may be highly prominent. While previous studies of Degani et al. (Reference Degani, Kreiser and Novogrodsky2019) and Marini et al. (Reference Marini, Sperindè, Ruta, Savegnago and Avanzini2019) noted significant contrasts between TD and affected bilingual groups across controlled tasks, such as picture-naming, phonological memory capacity and vocabulary comprehension, participants were between the ages of 7 and 10 and 9-14 years respectively with greater exposure to L2. The current study examined the spontaneous speech of younger bilinguals engaging in early stages of L2 acquisition, which may mask group differences between affected and TD groups. Exposure to L2, while balanced across groups, varied across individual children with a range between 11 and 49 months of total L2 exposure. Overall, this culminates in half of the sample being exposed to under two years of L2 English. It has been proposed that surface level similarities between multilingual children and children with DLD may disappear following a minimum of two years of L2 exposure, or longer, depending on a child’s own needs and conditions (Garraffa et al., Reference Garraffa, Vender, Sorace and Guasti2019; Marinis & Chondrogianni, Reference Marinis and Chondrogianni2010). It may be the case that both groups were at similar stages in early L2 acquisition and that gaps in subtle areas, such as the word-level, may emerge beyond the two-year cut-off, once TD bilinguals have sufficient exposure to make sizeable leaps in acquisition. In relation to the aim of the current study, however, a waiting period of two or more years would not satisfy language-based needs, as this creates a challenge for applying early intervention techniques. Sequential bilinguals may require a separate, more sensitive approach to diagnosis that emerges at an earlier point than two years post-exposure.

Thirdly, the examination of word properties to differentiate between affected and TD bilinguals may prove fruitful in more controlled tasks, such as picture-naming, non-word repetition or cross-linguistic lexical tasks (Degani et al., Reference Degani, Kreiser and Novogrodsky2019; Haman & Pomiechowska, Reference Haman and Pomiechowska2015; Vender et al., Reference Vender, Garraffa, Sorace and Guasti2016). While there are naturalistic benefits to the analysis of spontaneous speech, outcomes of the current study are insufficient to comment on the possibilities of DLD resulting in impoverished lexical entries and/or deficits in phonological working memory. In this case, a wider range of testing materials may be necessary to appraise the language development of sequential bilinguals, prioritising experimental control and the consideration of factors pertinent to the bilingual experience, such as length and degree of exposure to L2.

Strengths and Limitations

Some limitations within the current study require address. Firstly, values for semantic and lexical properties were not obtained from databases specific to child language norms but were compiled using adult speakers (Brysbaert et al., Reference Brysbaert, Warriner and Kuperman2014; Kuperman et al., Reference Kuperman, Stadthagen-Gonzalez and Brysbaert2012; Stadthagen-Gonzalez & Davis, Reference Stadthagen-Gonzalez and Davis2006). Specifically, measures of frequency may not adequately represent child language use, as frequency ratings obtained had standard deviations exceeding mean values, reflecting a high degree of variance. While using monolingually-normed AoA values can enable some comparisons between monolinguals and bilinguals, this carried particular limitations in the current study as interpretations are constrained for application to the sequential bilingual context alone. Additionally, while analysis comprised the separation of nouns and verbs, the comprehensive databases used in the current study do not enable word category distinctions between ratings and it was not possible to classify word input into the N-Watch programme. However, the effect of certain entries returning a norm value reflecting both noun and verb usage is unclear, as results speak to the validity of this approach, such as the distinctions overall between nouns and verbs in terms of semantic, lexical and phonological variables.

Moreover, due to the focus of the study on sequential bilinguals with diverse L1 backgrounds, certain bilingual factors such as typological distance between L1 and L2 were excluded from the analysis. Additional factors, such as the number of speakers with whom children engage in L2 and the language environment of L2 acquisition may account for a certain degree of variance in their respective bilingual experience. Despite certain limitations, however, the present study approached a growing global issue in failures to cater to sequential bilingual children. Accurate identification of DLD in L2 is paramount in bilingual research, given the lack of representation of the bilingual experience in certain clinical measures and the growing populations of L2 speakers with varying L1 backgrounds. Moreover, this study combined two different approaches to word-learning impairments in DLD – namely, the posited presence of impoverished lexical entries (Kail et al., Reference Kail, Hale, Leonard and Nippold1984) and the possibility of a phonological storage deficit (Gathercole & Baddeley, Reference Gathercole and Baddeley1990). In doing so, the scope of analysis encompassed a comprehensive range of variables spanning semantic, lexical and phonological aspects of word knowledge.

Conclusions

The primary goal of this research was to ascertain possible markers of DLD across semantic, lexical, and phonological word properties in the spontaneous speech of sequential bilinguals to facilitate diagnosis in the bilingual context. Results suggest that sequential bilinguals with and without DLD in early stages of L2 acquisition demonstrate similar word-learning strategies, leading to vocabularies which are comparable in terms of word imageability, concreteness, frequency, age of acquisition, and length. Analysis of the word properties of monolinguals with and without DLD alongside those of sequential bilinguals is needed to appropriately characterise the word-learning strategies of sequential bilinguals with and without DLD and determine how these may differ from monolingual counterparts. Further clarification on potential differences between sequential bilinguals with and without DLD is needed, particularly in relation to possible phonological markers, as group effects did not emerge across any individual marker, but the disparity between verb and noun phonological neighbourhoods was slightly different between groups. Continued efforts for diagnosis within the sequential bilingual population require prioritisation, particularly in early L2 acquisition as early intervention alternatives may not be readily available to newcomer families (Paradis et al., Reference Paradis, Schneider and Sorenson-Duncan2013). As the analysis of word properties may be masked by similar early-learning strategies within the first two years of L2 exposure and given the heterogeneity of impairments within DLD, future studies should comprise a blend of tests representing language development and possible phonological and morphosyntactic markers in early L2 acquisition to further the goal of misdiagnosis reduction in the sequential bilingual population.

Acknowledgments

This work was funded by the Erasmus Mundus Joint Master Degree scholarship provided by the European Commission. Many thanks to the faculty members of the EMCL+ programme 2018-2020 for their support and the students of the EMCL+ programme for their frequent and insightful discussions. The authors also wish to thank the anonymous reviewer and the Action Editor of the Journal of Child Language for their guidance, comments, and assessments

Competing interests

The author(s) declare none.

Appendices

Appendix A Mean values of word properties across L1 groups

Abbreviations: Imag = Imageability, Con = Concreteness, Freq= Frequency, AoA = Age of Acquisition, PN = Phonological Neighbourhood, LenP = Word Length in Phonemes

Appendix B Parameters, & Coefficients for Linear Mixed Models

Abbreviations: Imag = Imageability, Con = Concreteness, Freq= Frequency, AoA = Age of Acquisition.

Appendix C Model Parameters for Generalised Linear Mixed Models

Abbreviations: PN = Phonological Neighbourhood, LenP = Word Length in Phonemes, MLen = Mean Length in Phonemes per participant

References

Anderson, C. J., Verkuilen, J., & Johnson, T. (2012). Applied Generalized Linear Mixed Models: Continuous and Discrete Data. Springer.Google Scholar
Arslan, S., Broc, L., Mathy, F., & Olive, T. (2020). Reduced deficits observed in children and adolescents with developmental language disorder using proper nonverbalizable span tasks. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 96. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2019.103522CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baayen, R. H., Gulikers, L., Piepenbrock, R., & Centre for Lexical Information, M. P. I. voor P. (1995). The Celex lexical database. Linguistic Data Consortium.Google Scholar
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 148. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bedore, L. M., & Peña, E. D. (2008). Assessment of Bilingual Children for Identification of Language Impairment: Current Findings and Implications for Practice. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 11(1), 129. https://doi.org/10.2167/beb392.0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bird, H., Howard, D., & Franklin, S. (2003). Verbs and nouns: The importance of being imageable. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 16(2), 113149. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0911-6044(02)00016-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M. (2017). Why is it so hard to reach agreement on terminology? The case of developmental language disorder (DLD). International Journal of Language, & Communication Disorders, 52(6), 671680. https://doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12335CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Black, M., & Chiat, S. (2003). Noun-verb dissociations: A multi-faceted phenomenon. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 16(2–3), 231250. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0911-6044(02)00017-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brackenbury, T., & Pye, C. (2005). Semantic deficits in children with language impairments: Issues for clinical assessment. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 36(1), 516. https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2005/002)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brysbaert, M., Warriner, A. B., & Kuperman, V. (2014). Concreteness ratings for 40 thousand generally known English word lemmas. Behavior Research Methods, 46. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-013-0403-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burgemeister, B. B., Blum, L. H., & Lorge, I. (1954). Columbia Mental Maturity Scale. World Book Company.Google Scholar
Chondrogianni, V., & Marinis, T. (2011). Differential effects of internal and external factors on the development of vocabulary, tense morphology and morpho-syntax in successive bilingual children. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 1(3), 318346. https://doi.org/10.1075/lab.1.3.05choCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coady, J. A., & Evans, J. L. (2008). Uses and interpretations of non-word repetition tasks in children with and without specific language impairment (SLI). International Journal of Language, & Communication Disorders, 43(1), 140. https://doi.org/:10.1080/13682820601116485CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colombo, L., & Burani, C. (2002). The Influence of Age of Acquisition, Root Frequency, and Context Availability in Processing Nouns and Verbs. Brain and Language, 81(1), 398411. https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.2001.2533CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crawford, J. R., & Garthwaite, P. H. (2002). Investigation of the single case in neuropsychology: Confidence limits on the abnormality of test scores and test score differences. Neuropsychologia, 40, 11961208. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0028-3932(01)00224-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cronin, V. S. (2002). The syntagmatic-paradigmatic shift and reading development. Journal of Child Language, 29, 189204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, C. J. (2005). N-WATCH: A Program for Deriving Neighborhood Size and Other Psycholinguistic Statistics. Behaviour, Research, Methods, Instruments and Computers, 37, 6570.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Degani, T., Kreiser, V., & Novogrodsky, R. (2019). The joint effects of bilingualism, DLD and item frequency on children’s lexical-retrieval performance. International Journal of Language, & Communication Disorders, 54(3), 485498. https://doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12454CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dotan, D., & Friedmann, N. (2015). Steps towards understanding the phonological output buffer and its role in the production of numbers, morphemes, and function words. Cortex, 63, 317351. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.08.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Engel de Abreu, P. M., & Gathercole, S. E. (2012). Executive and Phonological Processes in second langauge acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104, 974986. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028390CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friedmann, N., Biran, M., & Dotan, D. (2013). Lexical retrieval and its breakdown in aphasia and developmental language impairment. In The Cambridge Handbook of Biolinguistics (pp. 350374). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garraffa, M., Vender, M., Sorace, A., & Guasti, M. T. (2019). Is it possible to differentiate multilingual children and children with Developmental Language Disorder? Languages, Society, & Policy, 18. https://doi.org/10.17863/CAM.37928Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1990). Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection? Journal of Memory and Language Journal of Memory and Language, 29(3), 336360. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(90)90004-JCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gentner, D. (2006). Why verbs are hard to learn. In Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. (Eds.), Action meets word: How children learn verbs (pp. 544564). Oxford.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ghyselinck, M., Lewis, M. B., & Brysbaert, M. (2004). Age of acquisition and the cumulative-frequency hypothesis: A review of the literature and a new multi-task investigation. Acta Psychologica, 115(1), 4367. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2003.11.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, T. A., Peña, E. D., & Bedore, L. M. (2014). The relation between language experience and receptive-expressive semantic gaps in bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 17(1), 90110. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2012.743960CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., Cera, C., & Sandoval, T. C. (2008). More use almost always a means a smaller frequency effect: Aging, bilingualism, and the weaker links hypothesis. Journal of Memory and Language, 58(3), 787814. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.07.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grimm, A., & Schulz, P. (2014). Specific Language Impairment and Early Second Language Acquisition: The Risk of Over- and Underdiagnosis. Child Ind Res Child Indicators Research : The Official Journal of the International Society for Child Indicators, 7(4), 821841. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12187-013-9230-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grosjean, F. (1997). The bilingual individual. Interpreting, 2(1–2), 163187. https://doi.org/10.1075/intp.2.1-2.07groCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haman, E., & Pomiechowska, B. (2015). Designing Cross-linguistic Lexical Tasks (CLTs) for bilingual preschool children. Multingual Matters. https://www.academia.edu/8632957/Designing_Cross_linguistic_Lexical_Tasks_CLTs_for_bilingual_preschool_childrenCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howell, S. R., & Becker, S. (2001). Modelling Language Acquisition: Grammar from the Lexicon? eScholarship, University of California. https://escholarship.org/uc/item/9tg9p5fzGoogle Scholar
Kail, R., Hale, C. A., Leonard, L. B., & Nippold, M. A. (1984). Lexical storage and retrieval in language-impaired children. Applied Psycholinguistics Applied Psycholinguistics, 5(1), 3749. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400004823CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuperman, V., Stadthagen-Gonzalez, H., & Brysbaert, M. (2012). Age-of-acquisition ratings for 30,000 English words. Behavior Research Methods, 44(4), 978990. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-012-0210-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leonard, L. B. (2014). Children with specific language impairment. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levie, R., Ben-Zvi, G., & Ravid, D. (2017). Morpho-lexical development in language impaired and typically developing Hebrew-speaking children from two SES backgrounds. Reading and Writing, 30(5), 10351064. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-016-9711-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lüdecke, D. (2021). sjPlot: Data visualisation for statistics in social science. (version 2.8.7) [Computer software]. https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/sjPlot/citation.htmlGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2003). The CHILDES Project: Tools for analyzing talk. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Mainela-Arnold, E., Evans, J. L., & Coady, J. A. (2010). Explaining lexical-semantic deficits in specific language impairment: The role of phonological similarity, phonological working memory, and lexical competition. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53(6), 17421756. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2010/08-0198)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marini, A., Sperindè, P., Ruta, I., Savegnago, C., & Avanzini, F. (2019). Linguistic Skills in Bilingual Children With Developmental Language Disorders: A Pilot Study. Frontiers in Psychology, 10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00493CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marinis, T., & Chondrogianni, V. (2010). Production of tense marking in successive bilingual children: When do they converge with their monolingual peers? International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 12(1), 1928. https://doi.org/10.3109/17549500903434125CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGregor, K. K., & Appel, A. (2002). On the relation between mental representation and naming in a child with specific language impairment. Clinical Linguistics, & Phonetics Clinical Linguistics, & Phonetics, 16(1), 120. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699200110085034CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGregor, K. K., Oleson, J., Bahnsen, A., & Duff, D. (2013). Children with developmental language impairment have vocabulary deficits characterized by limited breadth and depth. International Journal of Language, & Communication Disorders, 48(3), 307319. https://doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McMurray, B., Klein-Packard, J., McMurray, B., & Tomblin, J. B. (2019). A real-time mechanism underlying lexical deficits in developmental language disorder: Between-word inhibition. Cognition, 191. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2019.06.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montgomery, J. W., Gillam, R. B., Evans, J. L., Schwartz, S., & Fargo, J. D. (2019). A comparison of the storage-only deficit and joint mechanism deficit hypotheses of the verbal working memory storage capacity limitation of children with developmental language disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 62(10), 38083825. https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_JSLHR-L-19-0071CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nash, M., & Donaldson, M. L. (2005). Word Learning in Children With Vocabulary Deficits. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48(2), 439458. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2005/030)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Owen, A. J., & Leonard, L. B. (2002). Lexical Diversity in the Spontaneous Speech of Children With Specific Language Impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45(5), 927937. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2002/075)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paradis, J. (2010). The interface between bilingual development and specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31(2), 227252. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716409990373CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., Crago, M., Genesee, F., & Rice, M. (2003). French-English bilingual children with SLI: How do they compare with their monolingual peers? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46(1), 113127. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2003/009)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paradis, J., Emmerzael, K., & Sorenson Duncan, T. (2010). Assessment of English language learners: Using parent report on first language development. Journal of Communication Disorders, 43(6), 474497. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2010.01.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paradis, J., Schneider, P., & Sorenson-Duncan, T. (2013). Discriminating Children With Language Impairment Among English-Language Learners From Diverse First-Language Backgrounds. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56(3), 971981. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2012/12-0050)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team. (2018). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R foundation for statistical computing. https://www.R-project.org/Google Scholar
R Core Team. (2019). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R foundation for statistical computing. https://www.R-project.org/Google Scholar
Rice, M. L., & Bode, J. V. (1993). GAPS in the verb lexicons of children with specific language impairment. First Language, 13 (37, Pt 1), 113131. https://doi.org/10.1177/014272379301303707CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rofes, A., de Aguiar, V., Ficek, B., Wendt, H., Webster, K., & Tsapkini, K. (2019). The Role of Word Properties in Performance on Fluency Tasks in People with Primary Progressive Aphasia. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 68(4), 15211534. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-180990CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rofes, A., Zakariás, L., Ceder, K., Lind, M., Johansson, M. B., de Aguiar, V., Bjekić, J., Fyndanis, V., Gavarró, A., Simonsen, H. G., Sacristán, C. H., Kambanaros, M., Kraljević, J. K., Martínez-Ferreiro, S., Mavis, İ., Orellana, C. M., Sör, I., Lukács, Á., Tunçer, M., … Howard, D. (2018). Imageability ratings across languages. Behavior Research Methods, 50(3), 11871197. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-017-0936-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sadoski, M., Goetz, E. T., & Fritz, J. B. (2016). A Causal Model of Sentence Recall: Effects of Familiarity, Concreteness, Comprehensibility, and Interestingness: Journal of Reading Behavior. https://doi.org/10.1080/10862969309547799CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanz-Torrent, M., Serrat, E., Andreu, L., & Serra, M. (2008). Verb morphology in Catalan and Spanish in children with Specific Language Impairment: A developmental study. CLINICAL LINGUISTICS AND PHONETICS, 22(6), 459474. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699200801892959CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schad, D. J., Vasishth, S., Hohenstein, S., & Kliegl, R. (2020). How to capitalize on a priori contrasts in linear (mixed) models: A tutorial. Journal of Memory and Language, 110, 104038. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2019.104038CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seiger-Gardner, L., & Schwartz, R. G. (2008). Lexical access in children with and without specific language impairment: A cross-modal picture-word interference study. JLCD International Journal of Language, & Communication Disorders, 43(5), 528551. https://doi.org/10.1080/13682820701768581Google ScholarPubMed
Sheng, L., & McGregor, K. K. (2010). Lexical-semantic organization in children with specific language impairment. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53(1), 146159. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0160).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skipp, A., Windfuhr, K. L., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2002). Children’s grammatical categories of verb and noun: A comparative look at children with specific language impairment (SLI) and normal language (NL). JLCD International Journal of Language, & Communication Disorders, 37(3), 253271. https://doi.org/10.1080/13682820110119214Google Scholar
Spoelman, M., & Bol, G. W. (2012). The use of subject-verb agreement and verb argument structure in monolingual and bilingual children with specific language impairment. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 26(4), 357379. https://doi.org/10.3109/02699206.2011.637658CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stadthagen-Gonzalez, H., & Davis, C. J. (2006). The Bristol norms for age of acquisition, imageability, and familiarity. Behavior Research Methods, 38(4), 598605. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193891CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Storkel, H. L. (2004). Do children acquire dense neighborhoods? An investigation of similarity neighborhoods in lexical acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 201221. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716404001109CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thordardottir, E. T., & Weismer, S. E. (2001). High-frequency verbs and verb diversity in the spontaneous speech of school-age children with specific language impairment. International Journal of Language, & Communication Disorders, 36(2), 221244. https://doi.org/10.1080/13682820152410872CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vender, M., Garraffa, M., Sorace, A., & Guasti, M. T. (2016). How early L2 children perform on Italian clinical markers of SLI: A study of clitic production and nonword repetition. Clin. Linguist. Phon. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 30(2), 150169. https://doi.org/10.3109/02699206.2015.1120346CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vitevich, M. S., & Sommers, M. S. (2003). The facilitative influence of phonological similarity and neighborhood frequency in speech production in younger and older adults. Memory & Cognition, 31(4), 491504. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196091CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitworth, A., Webster, J., & Howard, D. (2014). Cognitive neuropsychological approach to assessment and intervention in aphasia: A clinician’s guide. Psychology Press. https://researchers.mq.edu.au/en/publications/cognitive-neuropsychological-approach-to-assessment-and-interventCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wickham, H. (2016). ggplot2: Elegant graphics for data analysis. Springer-Verlag. https://ggplot2.tidyverse.orgCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wickham, H., Averick, M., Bryan, J., Chang, W., McGovern, L. D., François, R., Grolemund, G., Hayes, A., Henry, L., Hester, J., Kuhn, M., Pedersen, T., Miller, E., Bache, S., Müller, K., Ooms, J., Robinson, D., Seidel, D., Spinu, V., … Yutani, H. (2019). Welcome to the tidyverse. Journal of Open Source Software, 4(43).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winter, B. (2013). Linear models and linear mixed effects models in R with linguistic applications. ArXiv:1308.5499 [Cs]. http://arxiv.org/abs/1308.5499Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. L1 Background of Participants

Figure 1

Table 2. Participant Characteristics across TD and DLD Groups

Figure 2

Table 3. Production of Grammatical Category across Groups

Figure 3

Table 4. Mean and Standard Deviations of Word Properties

Figure 4

Figure 1. Boxplots of Imageability and Concreteness ratings across group and word category

Figure 5

Figure 2. Boxplots of Frequency and Age of Acquisition ratings across group and word category

Figure 6

Figure 3. Boxplots of Phonological Length and Phonological Neighbourhood ratings across group and word category

Figure 7

Figure 4. Interaction plot of Phonological Neighbourhood ratings across group and word category

Figure 8

Appendix A Mean values of word properties across L1 groups

Figure 9

Appendix B Parameters, & Coefficients for Linear Mixed Models

Figure 10

Appendix C Model Parameters for Generalised Linear Mixed Models