Hostname: page-component-5f745c7db-szhh2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-01-06T21:26:12.357Z Has data issue: true hasContentIssue false

Repeated Challenge Studies: A Comparison of Union-Intersection Testing with Linear Modeling

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2025

Richard A. Levine*
Affiliation:
University of California, Davis
Pamela A. Ohman
Affiliation:
Cornell University
*
Requests for reprints should be sent to Richard A. Levine, Division of Statistics, University of California, Davis CA 95616.

Abstract

Challenge studies are often implemented for assessing whether a subject is sensitive to a certain agent or allergen. In particular, researchers test groups of subjects to determine if there really exists a causal relationship between some agent of interest and a response. To answer such a question, we need to detect the presence of the phenomenon in just one individual. Typically, however, there are a large number of unknown risk factors associated with the response and a potentially small population prevalence. Hence, standard statistical techniques, by averaging the treatment effect across the group, may miss a significant response of a single individual and lead to inconclusive results. We develop an alternative approach based on union-intersection testing that will allow a practitioner to correctly examine observations on an individual apart from the other subjects and test the hypothesis of interest: Does the phenomenon exist in the population? More specifically, we show how this technique adjusts for the multiple number of tests encountered when analyzing data for each individual subject separately. Furthermore, we demonstrate power calculations for the determination of sample size prior to performing the study. The performance of the union-intersection approach in comparison to linear models and semiparametric techniques is considered through sample size calculations and simulations. The union-intersection testing methodology out performs the Kolmogorov tests. However, the nested linear model performs as well if not better than the union-intersection tests. To illustrate the ideas presented in the paper, we provide an application in which we analyze psychological data collected by way of a challenge study design.

Type
Original Paper
Copyright
Copyright © 1997 The Psychometric Society

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

1

Research supported by the Office of Naval Research Graduate Fellowship and NIH Training Grant No. ES07261

2

Research supported by NSF Training Grant No. DMS956682

We would like to thank Professor George Casella and Susan Alber for many helpful discussions and advice during the course of this research and for comments on drafts of this manuscript. We would also like to express gratitude to Lorna Bayer and Professor Barbara Strupp for providing the data on dopamine exposure in rats. Finally, we would like to thank three referees and the editor for very helpful and insightful remarks that lead to a much improved version of this paper.

References

Aptech Systems (1992). GAUSS: The GAUSS system version 3.0, Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
Bayer, L. E., Snow, K., Strupp, B. J. (1994). Effect of SKF 81297 in control and lead exposed rats: Evidence that D1 dopaminergic activity modulates attention. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 20, 153153.Google Scholar
Behar, D., Rapoport, J. L., Adams, A. J., Berg, C. J., Cornblath, M. (1984). Sugar challenge testing with children considered “sugar reactive”. Nutrition and Behavior, 1, 277288.Google Scholar
Casella, G., Berger, R. L. (1990). Statistical inference, Pacific Grove, CA: Wadsworth.Google Scholar
Conover, W. J. (1980). Practical nonparametric statistics 2nd ed.,, New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Feingold, B. F. (1975). Hyperkinesis and learning disabilities linked to artificial food flavors and colors. American Journal of Nursing, 75, 797803.Google ScholarPubMed
Kinderman, A. J., Ramage, J. G. (1976). Computer generation of normal random numbers. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 71, 893896.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Metcalf, D. D., Sampson, H. A. (1990). Workshop on experimental methodology for clinical studies of adverse reactions to foods and food additives. Journal of Allergy and Clinical Immunology, 86(3), 421442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milich, R., Wolraich, M., Lindgren, S. (1986). Sugar and hyperactivity: A critical review of empirical findings. Clinical Psychology Review, 6, 493513.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neter, J., Wasserman, W., Kutner, M. H. (1990). Applied linear statistical models 3rd ed.,, Homewood, IL: Irwin.Google Scholar
Pearson, D. J. (1985). Food allergy, hypersensitivity and intolerance. Journal of the Royal College of Physicians of London, 19(3), 154162.Google ScholarPubMed
Pollock, I., Warner, J. O. (1990). Effect of artificial food colours on childhood behaviour. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 65, 7477.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Press, S. J. (1982). Applied multivariate analysis: Using Bayesian and frequentist methods if Inference 2nd ed.,, Melbourne, FL: Krieger Publishing.Google Scholar
Prinz, R. J. (1985). Diet-behavior research with children: Methodological and substantive issues. Advances in Learning and Behavioral Disabilities, 4, 181199.Google Scholar
Rosén, L. A., Booth, S. R., Bender, M. E., McGrath, M. L., Sorrell, S., Drabman, R. S. (1988). Effects of sugar (sucrose) on children's behavior. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56(4), 583589.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roshon, M. S., Hagen, R. L. (1989). Sugar consumption, locomotion, task orientation, and learning in preschool children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 17(3), 349357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowe, K. S. (1988). Synthetic food colourings and “hyperactivity”: A double-blind crossover study. Australian Paediatric Journal, 24, 143147.Google ScholarPubMed
Roy, S. N. (1957). Some aspects of multivariate analysis, New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Sampson, H. A. (1992). Food hypersensitivity: Manifestations, diagnosis, and natural history. Food Technology, 46, 141144.Google Scholar
Schardt, D. (1994). Food sensitivity nothing to sneeze at. Nutrition Action Healthletter, 21, 1214.Google Scholar
Searle, S. R. (1971). Linear models, New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Searle, S. R., Casella, G., McCulloch, C. E. (1994). Variance components, New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Van-Dusseldorp, M. (1989). Diet and hyperactivity: A review. Voeding (The Hauge), 50(1), 28.Google Scholar
Warner, J. O. (1987). Artificial food additive intolerance: Fact or fiction?. In Brostroff, J., Challacombe, S. J. (Eds.), Food intolerance (pp. 133147). Sussex, UK: Bailliere Tindall.Google Scholar