Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-24T10:35:16.008Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The roles of disgust and harm perception in political attitude moralization

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 November 2020

Daniel C. Wisneski*
Affiliation:
Saint Peter’s University
Brittany E. Hanson
Affiliation:
Saint Peter’s University
G. Scott Morgan
Affiliation:
Drew University
*
Correspondence: Daniel C. Wisneski, Department of Psychology, Saint Peter’s University, Jersey City, New Jersey, USA; email: [email protected]
Get access

Abstract

What causes people to see their political attitudes in a moral light? One answer is that attitude moralization results from associating one’s attitude stance with feelings of disgust. To test the possibility that disgust moralizes, the current study used a high-powered preregistered design looking at within-person change in moral conviction paired with an experimental manipulation of disgust or anger (versus control). Results from the preregistered analyses found that we successfully induced anger but not disgust; however, our manipulation had no effect on moral conviction. Additional exploratory analyses investigating whether emotion and harm predicted increases in moral conviction over time found that neither disgust, anger, nor sadness had an effect on moralization, whereas perceptions of harm did predict moralization. Our findings are discussed in terms of their implications for current theory and research into attitude moralization.

Type
Article
Copyright
© Association for Politics and the Life Sciences 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Avramova, Y. R., & Inbar, Y. (2013). Emotion and moral judgment. Cognitive Science, 4(2), 169178. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1216Google ScholarPubMed
Brandt, M. J., Wisneski, D. C., & Skitka, L. J. (2015). Moralization and the 2012 U.S. presidential election campaign. Journal of Social and Political Psychology, 3(2), 211237. http://dx.doi.org/10.23668/psycharchives.1694CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chozick, A. (2016, February 7). In Flint, Hillary Clinton prods Congress and calls response to crisis “immoral.” New York Times. https://www.nytimes.com/2016/02/08/us/politics/in-flint-hillary-clinton-prods-congress-and-calls-response-to-crisis-immoral.htmlGoogle Scholar
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feinberg, M., Kovacheff, C., Teper, R., & Inbar, Y. (2019). Understanding the process of moralization: How eating meat becomes a moral issue. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 117(1), 5072. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000149CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harmon-Jones, C., Bastian, B., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2016). The discrete emotions questionnaire: A new tool for measuring state self-reported emotions. PLOS ONE, 11(8), e0159915.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lakens, D. (2013). Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: A practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAs. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(863), 112. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00863CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landy, J. F., & Goodwin, G. P. (2015). Does incidental disgust amplify moral judgment? A meta-analytic review of experimental evidence. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(4), 518536. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691615583128CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, L. C., Murphy, R., & Buss, A. H. (1981). Consciousness of body: Private and public. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41(2), 397406. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.41.2.397CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Molenaar, P. C. (2004). A manifesto on psychology as idiographic science: Bringing the person back into scientific psychology, this time forever. Measurement, 2(4), 201218. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15366359mea0204_1Google Scholar
Morgan, G. S., Skitka, L. J., & Wisneski, D. C. (2010). Moral and religious convictions and intentions to vote in the 2008 presidential election. Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy, 10(1), 307320. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1530-2415.2010.01204.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mueller, A. B., & Skitka, L. J. (2018). Liars, damned liars, and zealots: The effect of moral mandates on transgressive advocacy acceptance. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 9(6), 711718. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550617720272CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998–2010). Mplus user’s guide (6th ed.). Muthén & Muthén.Google Scholar
Olatunji, B. O., Williams, N. L., Tolin, D. F., Sawchuck, C. N., Abramowitz, J. S., Lohr, J. M., et al. (2007). The disgust scale: Item analysis, factor structure, and suggestions for refinement. Psychological Assessment, 19, 281297. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.19.3.281CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pomerantz, E. M., Chaiken, S., & Tordesillas, R. S. (1995). Attitude strength and resistance processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(3), 408419. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.69.3.408CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rhee, J. J., Schein, C., & Bastian, B. (2019). The what, how, and why of moralization: A review of current definitions, methods, and evidence in moralization research. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 13(12), e12511. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12511CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rozin, P. (1999). The process of moralization. Psychological Science, 10(3), 218221. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00139CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rozin, P., Markwith, M., & Stoess, C. (1997). Moralization and becoming a vegetarian: The transformation of preferences into values and the recruitment of disgust. Psychological Science, 8(2), 6773. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00685.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rozin, P., & Singh, L. (1999). The moralization of cigarette smoking in the United States. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 8(3), 321337. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327663jcp0803_07CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryan, T. J. (2014). Reconsidering moral issues in politics. Journal of Politics, 76(2), 380397. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022381613001357CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schaller, M. (2006). Parasites, behavioral defenses, and the social psychological mechanisms through which cultures are evoked. Psychological Inquiry, 17, 96101. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327965pli1702_2Google Scholar
Schnall, S., Haidt, J., Clore, G. L., & Jordan, A. H. (2008). Disgust as embodied moral judgment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(8), 10961109. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167208317771CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schein, C., & Gray, K. (2018). The theory of dyadic morality: Reinventing moral judgment by redefining harm. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 22(1), 3270. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F1088868317698288CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skitka, L. J. (2014). The psychological foundations of moral conviction. In Wright, J., & Sarkissian, H. (Eds.), Advances in experimental moral psychology (pp. 148166). Bloomsbury Academic Press.Google Scholar
Skitka, L. J., & Bauman, C. W. (2008). Moral conviction and political engagement. Political Psychology, 29(1), 2954. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2007.00611.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skitka, L. J., Bauman, C. W., & Lytle, B. L. (2009). Limits on legitimacy: Moral and religious convictions as constraints on deference to authority. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(4), 567578. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0015998CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skitka, L. J., Hanson, B. E., Morgan, G. S., & Wisneski, D. C. (in press). The psychology of moral conviction. Annual Review of Psychology.Google Scholar
Skitka, L. J., Hanson, B. E., & Wisneski, D. C. (2017). Utopian hopes or dystopian fears? Understanding the motivational underpinnings of morally motivated political engagement. Personality and Social Psychological Bulletin, 43, 177190. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167216678858Google ScholarPubMed
Skitka, L. J., & Morgan, G. S. (2014). The social and political implications of moral conviction. Political Psychology, 35, 95110. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12166CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skitka, L. J., Wisneski, D. C., & Brandt, M. J. (2018). Attitude moralization: Probably not intuitive or rooted in perceptions of harm. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27(1), 913. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721417727861CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Small, D. A., Lerner, J. S., & Fischhoff, B. (2006). Emotion priming and attributions for terrorism: Americans’ reactions in a national field experiment. Political Psychology, 27, 289298. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2006.00007.xGoogle Scholar
Soave, R. (2019, July 30). Democrats Forget the Flint Water Crisis Was Caused by a Bold New Infrastructure Plan. Reason. https://reason.com/2019/07/30/flint-water-crisis-democrat-debate-infrastructure/Google Scholar
van Zomeren, M., Postmes, T., & Spears, R. (2012). On conviction’s collective consequences: Integrating moral conviction with the social identity model of collective action. British Journal of Social Psychology, 51(1), 5271. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309.2010.02000.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
West, S. G., Taylor, A. B., & Wu, W. (2012). Model fit and model selection in structural equation modeling. In Hoyle, R. H. (Ed.), Handbook of structural equation modeling (pp. 209231). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Wisneski, D. C., & Skitka, L. J. (2017). Moralization through moral shock: Exploring the emotional antecedents to moral conviction. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 43, 139150. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167216676479CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zaal, M. P., Laar, C. V., Ståhl, T., Ellemers, N., & Derks, B. (2011). By any means necessary: The effects of regulatory focus and moral conviction on hostile and benevolent forms of collective action. British Journal of Social Psychology, 50(4), 670689. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309.2011.02069.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Supplementary material: File

Wisneski et al. supplementary material

Wisneski et al. supplementary material

Download Wisneski et al. supplementary material(File)
File 29.4 KB