Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-24T09:33:14.909Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Political attitudes vary with detection of androstenone

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 January 2020

Amanda Friesen
Affiliation:
Indiana University–Purdue University Indianapolis
Mike Gruszczynski*
Affiliation:
Indiana University Bloomington
Kevin B. Smith
Affiliation:
University of Nebraska–Lincoln
John R. Alford
Affiliation:
Rice University
*
Correspondence: Mike Gruszczynski, The Media School at Indiana University Bloomington, Bloomington, Indiana. Email: [email protected].
Get access

Abstract

Building on a growing body of research suggesting that political attitudes are part of broader individual and biological orientations, we test whether the detection of the hormone androstenone is predictive of political attitudes. The particular social chemical analyzed in this study is androstenone, a nonandrogenic steroid found in the sweat and saliva of many mammals, including humans. A primary reason for scholarly interest in odor detection is that it varies so dramatically from person to person. Using participants’ self-reported perceptions of androstenone intensity, together with a battery of survey items testing social and political preferences and orientations, this research supports the idea that perceptions of androstenone intensity relate to political orientations—most notably, preferences for social order—lending further support to theories positing the influence of underlying biological traits on sociopolitical attitudes and behaviors.

Type
Article
Copyright
© Association for Politics and the Life Sciences 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aarøe, L., Petersen, M. B., & Arceneaux, K. (2017). The behavioral immune system shapes political intuitions: Why and how individual differences in disgust sensitivity underlie opposition to immigration. American Political Science Review, 111(2), 277294. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0003055416000770CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adorno, T. W., Frenkel-Brunswik , E., Levinson, D. J., & Nevitt, S. R. (1950). The authoritarian personality. W. W. Norton & Co.Google Scholar
Altemeyer, R. (1996). The authoritarian specter. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Balzer, A., & Jacobs, C. M. (2011). Gender and physiological effects in connecting disgust to political preferences. Social Science Quarterly, 92(5), 12971313. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-6237.2011.00819.xGoogle Scholar
Bittner, A., & Goodyear-Grant , E. (2017). Sex isn’t gender: Reforming concepts and measurements in the study of public opinionPolitical Behavior39(4), 10191041. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-017-9391-yCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Booth, A., Granger, D. A., Mazur, A., & Kivlighan, K. T. (2006). Testosterone and social behaviorSocial Forces85(1), 167191. https://doi.org/10.1353/sof.2006.0116CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bremner, E. A. (2003). The prevalence of androstenone anosmiaChemical Senses28(5), 423432. https://doi.org/10.1093/chemse/28.5.423CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castiello, U. (2006). Cross-modal interactions between olfaction and vision when grasping. Chemical Senses, 31(7), 665671. https://doi.org/10.1093/chemse/bjl007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowley, J. J., Johnson, A. L., & Brooksbank, B. W. L. (1997). The effect of two odorous compounds on performance in an assessment-of-people test. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 2(2), 159172. https://doi.org/10.1016/0306-4530(77)90021-XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Damasio, A. R., & Damasio, H. (1994). Cortical systems for retrieval of concrete knowledge: The convergence zone framework. In Koch, C. and Davis, J. L. (Eds.), Large-scale neuronal theories of the brain (pp. 6174). MIT Press.Google Scholar
Feldman, S., & Stenner, K. (1997). Perceived threat and authoritarianismPolitical Psychology18(4), 741770. https://doi.org/10.1677/joe.0.1370167CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Filsinger, E. E., Braun, J. J., Monte, W. C., & Linder, D. E. (1984). Human (Homo sapiens) responses to the pig (Sus scrofa) sex pheromone 5 alpha-androst-16-en-3-one. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 98(2), 219222. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.98.2.219CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fiore, A. M., Yah, X., & Yoh, E. (2000). Effects of a product display and environmental fragrancing on approach responses and pleasurable experiences. Psychology & Marketing, 17(1), 2754. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1520-6793(200001)17:1<27::AID-MAR3>3.0.CO;2-C3.0.CO;2-C>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friesen, A., & Ksiazkiewicz, A. (2015). Do political attitudes and religiosity share a genetic path? Political Behavior37(4), 791818. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-014-9291-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friesen, A., Smith, K. B., & Hibbing, J. R. (2017). Physiological arousal and self-reported valence for erotica images correlate with sexual policy preferencesInternational Journal of Public Opinion Research, 29(3), 449470. https://doi.org/10.1093/ijpor/edw008Google Scholar
Gloor, P., & Gubeman, A. H. (1997). The temporal lobe and limbic system. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 157(11), 15971598.Google Scholar
Gonzalez, F. J., Smith, K. B., & Hibbing, J. R. (2015). No longer “beyond our scope”: The biological and non-conscious underpinnings of public opinion. In Berinsky, A. J. (Ed.), New directions in public opinion (2nd ed., pp. 186204). Routledge.Google Scholar
Gower, D. B., & Ruparelia, B. A. (1993). Olfaction in humans with special reference to odorous 16-androstenes: Their occurrence, perception and possible social, psychological and sexual impact. Journal of Endocrinology, 137(2), 167187. https://doi.org/10.1677/joe.0.1370167CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graham, J., Haidt, J., & Nosek, B. A. (2009). Liberals and conservatives rely on different sets of moral foundations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(5), 10291046. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015141CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gustavson, A. R., Dawson, M. E., & Bonett, D. G. (1987). Androstenol, a putative human pheromone, affects human (Homo sapiens) male choice performance. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 101(2) 210212. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.101.2.210CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haidt, J., & Graham, J. (2007). When morality opposes justice: Conservatives have moral intuitions that liberals may not recognize. Social Justice Research, 20(1), 98116. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11211-007-0034-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hatemi, P., & McDermott, R. (2012). Policing the perimeter: Disgust and purity in democratic debate. PS: Political Science & Politics, 45(4), 675687. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1049096512000686Google Scholar
Havlicek, J., Murray, A. K., Saxton, T. K., & Roberts, S. C. (2010). Current issues in the study of androstenes in human chemosignaling. Vitamins & Hormones, 83, 4781. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0083-6729(10)83003-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herz, R. S. (2014). Verbal priming and taste sensitivity make moral transgressions gross. Behavioral Neuroscience, 128(1), 2028. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035468CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hetherington, M. J., & Weiler, J. D. (2009). Authoritarianism and polarization in American politics. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hibbing, J. R., Smith, K. B., & Alford, J. R. (2013). Predisposed: Liberals, conservatives, and the biology of political differences. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hummer, T. A., & McClintock, M. K. (2009). Putative human pheromone androstadienone attunes the mind specifically to emotional information. Hormones and Behavior, 55(4), 548559. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.01.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Inbar, Y., Pizarro, D. A., & Bloom, P. (2009) Conservatives are more easily disgusted than liberals. Cognition & Emotion, 23(4), 714725. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930802110007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Insel, T. R., & Young, L. J. (2001). The neurobiology of attachment. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(2), 129136. https://doi.org/10.1038/35053579CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacob, S., Garcia, S., Hayreh, D., & McClintock, M. K. (2002). Psychological effects of musky compounds: Comparison of androstadienone with androstenol and muscone. Hormones and Behavior, 42(3), 274283. https://doi.org/10.1006/hbeh.2002.1826CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacob, T. J., Wang, L., Jaffer, S., & McPhee, S. (2005). Changes in the odor quality of androstadienone during exposure-induced sensitizationChemical Senses31(1), 38. https://doi.org/10.1093/chemse/bji073CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller, A. (2011). Genes and smells. The Biochemist, 33(6), 1013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, A., Zhuang, H., Chi, Q., Vosshall, L. B., & Matsunami, H. (2007). Genetic variation in a human odorant receptor alters odour perception. Nature, 449(7161), 468472. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature06162CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirk-Smith , M. D., Booth, D. A., & Van Der Starre, H. (1980). Effects of androstenone on choice of location in others’ presence. In Olfaction and Taste VII: Proceedings of the Seventh International Symposium on Olfaction and Taste and of the Fourth Congress of the European Chemoreception Research Organization (pp. 397400). London: IRL Press.Google Scholar
Kline, J. P., Schwartz, G. E., & Dikman, Z. V. (2007). Interpersonal defensiveness and diminished perceptual acuity for the odor of a putative pheromone: Androstenone. Biological Psychology, 74(3), 405413. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.10.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knaapila, A., Tuorila, H., Silventoinen, K., Wright, M. J., Kyvik, K. O., Cherkas, L. F., Keskitalo, K., Hansen, J., Martin, N. G., Spector, T. D., Kaprio, J., & Perola, M. (2008). Genetic and environmental contributions to perceived intensity and pleasantness of androstenone odor: An international twin study. Chemosensory Perception, 1(1), 3442. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12078-007-9005-xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kosfeld, M., Heinrichs, M., Zak, P. J., Fischbacher, U., & Fehr, E. (2005). Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature, 435(7042), 673676. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature03701CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lodge, M., & Taber, C. S. (2005). The automaticity of affect for political leaders, groups, and issues: An experimental test of the hot cognition hypothesis. Political Psychology, 26(3), 455482. https://doi.org/10.2307/3792606CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lundström, J. N., & Olsson, M. J. (2005). Subthreshold amounts of social odorant affect mood, but not behavior, in heterosexual women when tested by a male, but not a female, experimenter. Biological Psychology, 70(3), 197204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lundström, J. N., Olsson, M. J., Schaal, B., & Hummel, T. (2006). A putative social chemosignal elicits faster cortical responses than perceptually similar odorants. NeuroImage, 30(4), 13401346. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.10.040CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDermott, R., Johnson, D., Cowden, J., & Rosen, S. (2007). Testosterone and aggression in a simulated crisis gameAnnals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science614, 1533.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDermott, R., Tingley, D., & Hatemi, P.K. (2014). Assortative mating on ideology could operate through olfactory cues. American Journal of Political Science, 58(4), 9971005. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajps.12133CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menashe, I., Man, O., Lancet, D., & Gilad, Y. (2003). Different noses for different people. Nature Genetics, 34(2), 143144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mombaerts, P. (1999). Molecular biology of odorant receptors in vertebrates. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 22, 487509. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.22.1.487CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morrin, M., & Ratneshwar, S. (2000). The impact of ambient scent on evaluation, attention, and memory for familiar and unfamiliar brands . Journal of Business Research, 49(2), 157165. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0148-2963(99)00006-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neuberg, S. L., Kenrick, D. T., & Schaller, M. (2011). Human threat management systems: Self-protection and disease avoidance. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(4), 10421051. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.08.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neville, K. R., & Haberly, L. B. (2003). Olfactory cortex. In Shepherd, G. M. (Ed.), The synaptic organization of the brain (5th ed., pp. 415454). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Oxley, D. R., Smith, K. B., Alford, J. R., Hibbing, M. V., Miller, J. L., Scalora, M., … Hibbing, J. R. (2008). Political attitudes vary with physiological traits. Science, 321, 16671670.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pause, B. M. (2004). Are androgen steroids acting as pheromones in humans? Physiology & Behavior, 83(1), 2129. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.07.019CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pause, B. M., Ferstl, R., & Fehm-Wolfsdorf , G. (1998). Personality and olfactory sensitivity. Journal of Research in Personality, 32(4), 510518. https://doi.org/10.1006/jrpe.1998.2228CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saxton, T., Lyndon, A., Little, A., & Roberts, S. (2008). Evidence that androstadienone, a putative human chemosignal, modulates women’s attributions of men’s attractiveness. Hormones and Behavior, 54(5), 597601. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.06.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schaal, B., & Porter, R. H. (1991). “Microsmatic Humans” Revisited: The Generation and Perception of Chemical Signals. In P. J. B. Slater, J. S. Rosenblatt, C. Beer, & M. Milinski (Eds.), Advances in the Study of Behavior (Vol. 20, pp. 135199). https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-3454(08)60321-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sherwood, L. (2010). Human physiology: From cells to systems (7th ed.). Cengage Learning.Google Scholar
Smith, K. B., Oxley, D. R, Hibbing, M. V., Alford, J. R., & Hibbing, J. R. (2011a). Disgust sensitivity and the neurophysiology of left-right political orientations. PLOS ONE, 6(10), e25552. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0025552CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, K. B., Oxley, D. R., Hibbing, M. V., Alford, J. R., & Hibbing, J. R. (2011b). Linking genetics and political attitudes: Reconceptualizing political ideologyPolitical Psychology32(3), 369397. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2010.00821.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stenner, K. (2005). The authoritarian dynamic. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stockhorst, U., & Pietrowsky, R. (2004). Olfactory perception, communication, and the nose-to-brain pathway. Physiology & Behavior, 83(1), 311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tobin, V. A., Hashimoto, H., Wacker, D. W., Tankayanagi, Y., Langnaese, K., Caquineau, C., Noack, J.Landgraf, R.Onaka, T.Leng, G.Meddle, S. L.Engelmann, M., & Ludwig, M. (2010). An intrinsic vasopressin system in the olfactory bulb is involved in social recognition. Nature, 464(7287), 413417. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature08826CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tybur, J. M., Lieberman, D., & Griskevicius, V. (2009). Microbes, mating, and morality: Individual differences in three functional domains of disgust. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(1), 103122. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015474CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tybur, J. M., Merriman, L. A., Hooper, A. E. C., McDonald, M. M., & Navarrete, C. D. (2010). Extending the behavioral immune system to political psychology: Are political conservatism and disgust sensitivity really related? Evolutionary Psychology, 8(4), 147470491000800. https://doi.org/10.1177/147470491000800406CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vigil, J. (2008). Facial expression processing varies with political affiliation. http://precedings.nature.com/documents/2414/version/1.Google Scholar
Vigil, J. M. (2010). Political leanings vary with facial expression processing and psychosocial functioning. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 13(5), 547558. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430209356930CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, M. W., Deppe, K. D., Jacobs, C. M., Friesen, A., Smith, K. B., & Hibbing, J. R. (2015). Beyond survey self-reports: Using physiology to tap political orientationsInternational Journal of Public Opinion Research27(3), 303317. https://doi.org/10.1093/ijpor/edu036CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, L., Chen, L., & Jacob, T. (2004) Evidence for peripheral plasticity in human odour response. Journal of Physiology, 554(1), 236244. https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.2003.054726CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, G. D., & Patterson, J. R. (1968). A new measure of conservatismBritish Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology7(4), 264269. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8260.1968.tb00568.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woodward, J., & Allman, J. (2007). Moral intuition: Its neural substrates and normative significance. Journal of Physiology-Paris , 101(4), 179202. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jphysparis.2007.12.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wysocki, C. J., & Beauchamp, G. K. (1984) Ability to smell androstenone is genetically determined. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 81(15), 48994902. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.81.15.4899CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhou, W., & Chen, D. (2009). Sociochemosensory and emotional functions: Behavioral evidence for shared mechanisms. Psychological Science, 20(9), 11181124. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02413.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed