Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-p9bg8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T16:29:17.445Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Recognition of Facial Emotion and Affective Prosody in Children at High Risk of Criminal Behavior

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 November 2018

Lisette van Zonneveld*
Affiliation:
Clinical Neurodevelopmental Sciences, Leiden University, the Netherlands Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition, Leiden, the Netherlands
Leo de Sonneville
Affiliation:
Clinical Neurodevelopmental Sciences, Leiden University, the Netherlands Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition, Leiden, the Netherlands
Stephanie van Goozen
Affiliation:
Clinical Neurodevelopmental Sciences, Leiden University, the Netherlands School of Psychology, Cardiff University, Cardiff, United Kingdom
Hanna Swaab
Affiliation:
Clinical Neurodevelopmental Sciences, Leiden University, the Netherlands Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition, Leiden, the Netherlands
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Lisette van Zonneveld, Faculty of Social and Behavioural Sciences, PO Box 9555, 2300 RB Leiden, the Netherlands. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Objective: Emotion recognition is an important aspect of emotion processing, which is needed for appropriate social behavior and normal socialization. Previous studies in adults with antisocial personality disorder or psychopathy, in those convicted of criminal behavior, or in children with conduct disorder show impairments in negative emotion recognition. The present study investigated affective facial and prosody recognition in a sample of children at high risk of developing future criminal behavior. Methods: Participants were 8- to 12-year-old children at high risk of developing criminal behavior (N=219, 83.1% boys) and typically developing controls (N=43, 72.1% boys). The high-risk children were recruited through an ongoing early intervention project of the city of Amsterdam, that focuses on the underage siblings or children of delinquents, and those failing to attend school. Facial and vocal recognition of happy, sad, angry, and fear was measured with the Facial Emotion Recognition (FER) test and the prosody test of the Amsterdam Neuropsychological Tasks (ANT), respectively. Results: The high-risk group was significantly worse in facial affect recognition and had particular problems with fear and sadness recognition. No hostile attribution bias was found. The high-risk group did not differ from controls in affective prosody recognition but needed significantly more time to recognize emotions. Conclusions: The emotion-specific deficits found in forensic and clinical populations are already present in a sample of children at high risk of developing future criminal behavior. These findings help us understand a possible underlying mechanism of antisocial behavior that could provide directions for tailored interventions. (JINS, 2019, 25, 57–64)

Type
Regular Research
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2018 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Achenbach, T.M. & Rescorla, L.A. (2001). Manual for the ASEBA School-Age Forms & Profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, and Families.Google Scholar
Blair, R. (2005). Applying a cognitive neuroscience perspective to the disorder of psychopathy. Development and Psychopathology, 17(3), 865891. doi:10.1017/S0954579405050418 Google Scholar
Blair, R.J.R. (2003). Neurobiological basis of psychopathy. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 182(1), 57. doi:10.1192/bjp.182.1.5 Google Scholar
Blair, R.J.R., Budhani, S., Colledge, E., & Scott, S. (2005). Deafness to fear in boys with psychopathic tendencies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(3), 327336. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00356.x Google Scholar
Blair, R.J.R., & Coles, M. (2000). Expression recognition and behavioural problems in early adolescence. Cognitive Development, 15(4), 421434. doi:10.1016/S0885-2014(01)00039-9 Google Scholar
Blair, R.J.R., Colledge, E., Murray, L., & Mitchell, D.G.V. (2001). A selective impairment in the processing of sad and fearful expressions in children with psychopathic tendencies. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 29(6), 491498. doi:10.1023/a:1012225108281 Google Scholar
Bowen, K.L., Morgan, J.E., Moore, S.C., & van Goozen, S.H. (2014). Young offenders’ emotion recognition dysfunction across emotion intensities: explaining variation using psychopathic traits, conduct disorder and offense severity. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 36(1), 6073.Google Scholar
Campbell, J.M. (1998). Internal and external validity of seven Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children – Third edition short forms in a sample of inpatients. Psychological Assessment, 10, 431434.Google Scholar
Carr, M., & Lutjemeier, J. (2005). The relation of facial affect recognition and empathy to delinquency in youth offenders. Adolescence, 40(159), 601619.Google Scholar
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), 155.Google Scholar
Corden, B., Critchley, H.D., Skuse, D., & Dolan, R.J. (2006). Fear recognition ability predicts differences in social cognitive and neural functioning in men. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(6), 889897. doi:10.1162/jocn.2006.18.6.889 Google Scholar
Dadds, M.R., Perry, Y., Hawes, D.J., Merz, S., Riddell, A.C., Haines, D.J., Solak, E., & Abeygunawardane, A.I. (2006). Attention to the eyes and fear-recognition deficits in child psychopathy. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 189(3), 280281.Google Scholar
Dawel, A., O’Kearney, R., McKone, E., & Palermo, R. (2012). Not just fear and sadness: Meta-analytic evidence of pervasive emotion recognition deficits for facial and vocal expressions in psychopathy. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(10), 22882304. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.08.006 Google Scholar
De Castro, B.O., Veerman, J.W., Koops, W., Bosch, J.D., & Monshouwer, H.J. (2002). Hostile attribution of intent and aggressive behavior: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 73(3), 916934. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00447 Google Scholar
De Sonneville, L.M.J. (1999). Amsterdam Neuropsychological Tasks: A computer-aided assessment program. Computers in Psychology, 6, 187203.Google Scholar
De Sonneville, L.M.J. (2014). Handboek ANT: Amsterdamse Neuropsychologische Taken. Amsterdam: Boom Test Uitgevers.Google Scholar
Dennis, M., Francis, D.J., Cirino, P.T., Schachar, R., Barnes, M.A., & Fletcher, J.M. (2009). Why IQ is not a covariate in cognitive studies of neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15(3), 331343.Google Scholar
Dishion, T.J., French, D.C., & Patterson, G.R. (1995). The development and ecology of antisocial behavior. In D. Cicchetti & D.J. Cohen (Eds.), Wiley series on personality processes. Developmental psychopathology, Vol. 2. Risk, disorder, and adaptation (pp. 421471). Oxford, England: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Dodge, K.A. (2006). Trans al science in action: Hostile attributional style and the development of aggressive behavior problems. Development and Psychopathology, 18(3), 791814. doi:10.1017/S0954579406060391 Google Scholar
Fairchild, G., Stobbe, Y., van Goozen, S.H.M., Calder, A.J., & Goodyer, I.M. (2010). Facial expression recognition, fear conditioning, and startle modulation in female subjects with conduct disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 68(3), 272279. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.02.019 Google Scholar
Fairchild, G., Van Goozen, S.H.M., Calder, A.J., Stollery, S.J., & Goodyer, I.M. (2009). Deficits in facial expression recognition in male adolescents with early-onset or adolescence-onset conduct disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(5), 627636. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.02020.x Google Scholar
Farrington, D., Piquero, A.R., & Jennings, W.G. (2013). Offending From Childhood to Late Middle Age: Recent Results From the Cambridge Study in Delinquent Development. New York: Springer Science & Business Media.Google Scholar
Hubble, K., Bowen, K.L., Moore, S.C., & van Goozen, S.H.M. (2015). Improving negative emotion recognition in young offenders reduces subsequent crime. PLoS One, 10(6), e0132035. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0132035 Google Scholar
Izard, C., Fine, S., Schultz, D., Mostow, A., Ackerman, B., & Youngstrom, E. (2001). Emotion knowledge as a predictor of social behavior and academic competence in children at risk. Psychological Science, 12(1), 1823. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00304 Google Scholar
Kort, W., Schittekatte, M., Dekker, P.H., Verhaeghe, P., Compaan, E.L., Bosmans, M., & Vermeir, G. (2005). Handleiding en verantwoording WISC-IIINL . London: Pearson Assessment.Google Scholar
Leist, T., & Dadds, M.R. (2009). Adolescents’ ability to read different emotional faces relates to their history of maltreatment and type of psychopathology. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 14(2), 237250. doi:10.1177/1359104508100887 Google Scholar
Loeber, R., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1998). Development of juvenile aggression and violence – Some common misconceptions and controversies. American Psychologist, 53(2), 242259. doi:10.1037//0003-066x.53.2.242 Google Scholar
Marsh, A.A., Adams, R.B. Jr., & Kleck, R.E. (2005). Why do fear and anger look the way they do? Form and social function in facial expressions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(1), 7386. doi:10.1177/0146167204271306 Google Scholar
Marsh, A.A., & Blair, R.J.R. (2008). Deficits in facial affect recognition among antisocial populations: A meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 32(3), 454465.Google Scholar
Martin-Key, N.A., Graf, E.W., Adams, W.J., & Fairchild, G. (2018). Facial emotion recognition and eye movement behaviour in conduct disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 59(3), 247257. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12795.Google Scholar
Mellentin, A.I., Dervisevic, A., Stenager, E., Pilegaard, M., & Kirk, U. (2015). Seeing enemies? A systematic review of anger bias in the perception of facial expressions among anger-prone and aggressive populations. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 25(Part B), 373383. doi:10.1016/j.avb.2015.09.001 Google Scholar
Montagne, B., van Honk, J., Kessels, R.P.C., Frigerio, E., Burt, M., van Zandvoort, M.J.E., de Haan, E.H.F. (2005). Reduced efficiency in recognising fear in subjects scoring high on psychopathic personality characteristics. Personality and Individual Differences, 38(1), 511. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2004.02.008 Google Scholar
Nelson, D.A., & Coyne, S.M. (2009). Children’s intent attributions and feelings of distress: Associations with maternal and paternal parenting practices. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37(2), 223237. doi:10.1007/s10802-008-9271-3 Google Scholar
Oerlemans, A.M., Van der Meer, J.M., Van Steijn, D.J., De Ruiter, S.W., De Bruijn, Y.G., De Sonneville, L.M., Buitelaar, J.K., & Rommelse, N.N. (2014). Recognition of facial emotion and affective prosody in children with ASD (+ ADHD) and their unaffected siblings. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 23(5), 257271.Google Scholar
Schönenberg, M., & Jusyte, A. (2014). Investigation of the hostile attribution bias toward ambiguous facial cues in antisocial violent offenders. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 264(1), 6169. doi:10.1007/s00406-013-0440-1 Google Scholar
Schwenck, C., Gensthaler, A., Romanos, M., Freitag, C.M., Schneider, W., & Taurines, R. (2014). Emotion recognition in girls with conduct problems. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 23(1), 1322. doi:10.1007/s00787-013-0416-8 Google Scholar
Stevens, D., Charman, T., & Blair, R.J.R. (2001). Recognition of emotion in facial expressions and vocal tones in children with psychopathic tendencies. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 162(2), 201211. doi:10.1080/00221320109597961 Google Scholar
Sully, K., Sonuga-Barke, E.J.S., & Fairchild, G. (2015). The familial basis of facial emotion recognition deficits in adolescents with conduct disorder and their unaffected relatives. Psychological Medicine, 45(9), 19651975. doi:10.1017/S0033291714003080 Google Scholar
Székely, E., Tiemeier, H., Jaddoe, V.W.V., Hofman, A., Verhulst, F.C., & Herba, C.M. (2014). Associations of internalizing and externalizing problems with facial expression recognition in preschoolers: The Generation R Study. Social Development, 23(3), 611630. doi:10.1111/sode.12070 Google Scholar
Van Goozen, S.H., & Fairchild, G. (2008). How can the study of biological processes help design new interventions for children with severe antisocial behavior? Development and Psychopathology, 20(03), 941973. doi:10.1017/S095457940800045X Google Scholar
Van Goozen, S.H.M. (2015). The role of early emotion impairments in the development of persistent antisocial behavior. Child Development Perspectives, 9(4), 206210.Google Scholar
Van Rijn, S., Aleman, A., Swaab, H., Krijn, T., Vingerhoets, G.U.Y., & Kahn, R.S. (2007). What is said versus how it is said: Comprehension of affective prosody in men with Klinefelter (47,XXY) syndrome. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13(6), 10651070. doi:10.1017/S1355617707071044 Google Scholar
Van Rijn, S., Aleman, A., Van Diessen, E., Berckmoes, C., Vingerhoets, G., & Kahn, R.S. (2005). What is said or how it is said makes a difference: Role of the right fronto-parietal operculum in emotional prosody as revealed by repetitive TMS. European Journal of Neuroscience, 21(11), 31953200. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04130.x Google Scholar
Van Zonneveld, L., Platje, E., de Sonneville, L., van Goozen, S., & Swaab, H. (2017). Affective empathy, cognitive empathy and social attention in children at high risk of criminal behaviour. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(8), 913921. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12724 Google Scholar
Verhulst, F.C., Van der Ende, J., & Koot, H.M. (1996). Handleiding voor de CBCL/ 4-18 [Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist]. Rotterdam : Afdeling Kinder- en Jeugdpsychiatrie, Sophia Kinderziekenhuis, Erasmus MC.Google Scholar
Verhulst, F.C., Van der Ende, J., & Koot, H.M. (1997). Handleiding voor de Teachers’ Report Form [Manual for the Teachers’ Report Form]. Rotterdam, The Netherlands: Afdeling Kinder- en Jeugdpsychiatrie, Sophia Kinderziekenhuis, Erasmus MC.Google Scholar
Walker, D.W., & Leister, C. (1994). Recognition of facial affect cues by adolescents with emotional and behavioral disorders. Behavioral Disorders, 19(4), 269276. doi:10.1177/019874299401900408 Google Scholar
Woodworth, M., & Waschbusch, D. (2008). Emotional processing in children with conduct problems and callous/unemotional traits. Child: Care, Health and Development, 34(2), 234244. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2214.2007.00792.x Google Scholar