Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dzt6s Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T16:05:36.915Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Perceptual organization and visual immediate memory in children with specific language impairment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 June 2006

NATACHA AKSHOOMOFF
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, University of California, San Diego, California
JOAN STILES
Affiliation:
Department of Cognitive Science, School of Medicine, University of California, San Diego, California
BEVERLY WULFECK
Affiliation:
School of Speech, Language, and Hearing Sciences, San Diego State University, San Diego, California Center for Research on Language, University of California, San Diego

Abstract

Children with specific language impairment (LI) have deficits on some nonverbal tasks, but it is not clear if these are related to specific visuospatial deficits or to more general deficits in processing strategies. Children with LI were given two visuospatial tasks that we have shown to be sensitive to strategy use as well as specific processing deficits. In Study 1, children with LI (N = 29, ages 6 to 12 years) performed significantly worse than typically developing children (N = 26) on the Hierarchical Forms Memory task. In Study 2, children with LI (N = 15; ages 9 to 12 years) performed significantly worse than typically developing children (N = 40) on the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure task. Children with LI were less accurate and tended to use a fairly piecemeal (immature) strategy when copying the figure and were less likely to draw the core rectangle in a more integrated fashion during the immediate memory condition. These results suggest children with LI have subtle deficits on visuospatial tasks that may be more indicative of limitations associated with processing load and planning than of specific visuospatial processing deficits (JINS, 2006, 12, 465–474.)

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2006 The International Neuropsychological Society

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Akshoomoff, N.A., Feroleto, C.C., Doyle, R.E., & Stiles, J. (2002). The impact of early unilateral brain injury on perceptual organization and visual memory. Neuropsychologia, 40, 539561.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Akshoomoff, N.A. & Stiles, J. (1995a). Developmental trends in visuospatial analysis and planning: I. Copying a complex figure. Neuropsychology, 9, 364377.Google Scholar
Akshoomoff, N.A. & Stiles, J. (1995b). Developmental trends in visuospatial analysis and planning: II. Memory for a complex figure. Neuropsychology, 9, 378389.Google Scholar
Akshoomoff, N.A. & Stiles, J. (2003). Children's performance on the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure and the development of spatial analysis. In J. Knight & E. Kaplan (Eds.), The handbook of Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure usage: Clinical and research applications (pp. 393409). Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Beery, K.E. (1997). The Beery-Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration (VMI) Administration, scoring, and teaching manual (4th ed., revised). Parsippany, NJ: Modern Curriculum Press.
Delis, D.C., Robertson, L.C., & Efron, R. (1986). Hemispheric specialization of memory for visual hierarchical stimuli. Neuropsychologia, 24, 205214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dukette, D. & Stiles, J. (2001). The effects of stimulus density on children's analysis of hierarchical patterns. Developmental Science, 4, 233251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hick, R., Botting, N., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2005). Cognitive abilities in children with specific language impairment: Consideration of visuo-spatial skills. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 40, 137149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnston, J.R. & Ramstad, V. (1983). Cognitive development in pre-adolescent language impaired children. British Journal of Disorders of Communication, 18, 4955.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnston, J.R. & Weismer, S.E. (1983). Mental rotation abilities in language-disordered children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 26, 397403.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kinchla, R.A. & Wolfe, J.M. (1979). The order of visual processing: ‘top down’, ‘bottom-up’, or ‘middle-out’. Perception and Psychophysics, 25, 225231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirkwood, M.W., Weiler, M.D., Bernstein, J.H., Forbes, P.W., & Waber, D.P. (2001). Sources of poor performance on the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test among children with learning difficulties: A dynamic assessment approach. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 15, 345356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L.B. (1998). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 353383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Osterrieth, P.A. (1944). Le test de copie d'une figure complexe. Contribution à l'étude de la perception et de la mémoire. Archives de psychologie, 30, 206353.Google Scholar
Reilly, J.S. & Wulfeck, B.B. (2004). Plasticity and development: Language in atypical children. Brain and Language, 88, 163257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Restrepo, M.A., Swisher, L., Plante, E., & Vance, R. (1992). Relations among verbal and nonverbal cognitive skills in normal language and specifically language-impaired children. Journal of Communication Disorders, 25, 205219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schul, R., Stiles, J., Wulfeck, B., & Townsend, J. (2004). How ‘generalized’ is the ‘slowed processing’ in SLI? The case of visuospatial attentional orienting. Neuropsychologia, 42, 661671.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Semel, E., Wiig, E.H., & Secord, W. (1987). CELF-R: Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals–Revised. New York: Psychological Corporation.
Stern, R.A., Singer, E.A., Duke, L.M., Singer, N.G., Morey, C.E., Daughtrey, E.W., & Kaplan, E. (1994). The Boston Qualitative Scoring System for the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure: Description and interrater reliability. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 8, 309322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stiles, J., Bates, E., Thal, D., Trauner, D., & Reilly, J. (1998). Linguistic, cognitive and affective development in children with pre- and perinatal focal brain injury: A ten-year overview from the San Diego Longitudinal Project. In L.L.C. Rovee-Collier & H. Hayne (Eds.), Advances in infancy research (pp. 131163). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Stiles, J. & Stern, C. (2001). Developmental change in spatial cognitive processing: Complexity effects and block construction performance in preschool children. Journal of Cognition and Development, 2, 157187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stiles, J., Stern, C., Appelbaum, M., Nass, R., Trauner, D., & Hesselink, J. (submitted). Effects of early focal brain injury on memory for visuospatial patterns: Selective deficits of global-local processing.
Swisher, L., Plante, E., & Lowell, S. (1994). Nonlinguistic deficits of children with language disorders complicate the interpretation of their nonverbal IQ scores. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 25, 235240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tada, W. & Stiles, J. (1996). Developmental change in children's analysis of spatial patterns. Developmental Psychology, 32, 951970.Google Scholar
Tallal, P. & Benasich, A.A. (2002). Developmental language learning impairments. Developmental Psychopathology, 14, 559579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomblin, J.B., Records, N.L., Buckwalter, P., Zhang, X., Smith, E., & O'Brien, M. (1997). Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 40, 13151320.Google Scholar
Waber, D.P. & Holmes, J.M. (1985). Assessing children's copy productions of the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 7, 264280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1974). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–Revised. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
Wechsler, D. (1989). Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence–Revised. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (3rd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
Weismer, S.E. (1991). Hypothesis-testing abilities of language-impaired children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 13291338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weismer, S.E. (2002). The role of processing limitations in early identification of specific language impairment. Topics in Language Disorders, 22, 1529.CrossRefGoogle Scholar