Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dk4vv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T05:47:48.784Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Neuroimaging of working memory dysfunction and the dilemma with brain reorganization hypotheses

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 June 2008

FRANK G. HILLARY*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, Pennsylvania
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Frank G. Hillary, Assistant Professor, Department of Psychology, 223 Bruce V. Moore Building, University Park, PA 16802. E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

There is a growing literature examining working memory deficits using functional imaging and there has been great convergence in the findings, to date, but interpretations have varied. Investigators consistently observed recruitment of neural resources in clinical samples, with some examiners attributing these findings to neural inefficiency and others attributing differences to neural compensation and/or brain reorganization. It is the goal of this paper to address the current interpretation of altered brain activation in clinical imaging studies of working memory dysfunction with specific emphasis on findings in prefrontal cortex (PFC). Throughout this review, the methods used to examine brain reorganization associated with working memory dysfunction are critiqued with the goal of understanding how study design has influenced data interpretation. It is proposed that much of what has been considered “aberrant” neural activity is not indicative of neural compensation, as it has been typically defined, and does not represent brain reorganization. Instead, recruitment of neural resources in PFC can be explained by a natural, and largely overlooked, role of cognitive control in accommodating neural dysfunction secondary to brain injury and disease. This paper provides predictions based on this proposition and a critique of the current methods available for testing these predictions. (JINS, 2008, 14, 526–534.)

Type
Critical Review
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2008

An Alternative to Brain Reorganization Hypotheses

Functional imaging techniques afford the unique opportunity to examine the relationships between basic brain changes and the behavioral deficits associated with brain injury and disease. There is a growing functional imaging literature examining the effects of neurological insult on working memory (WM), or the ability to maintain and/or manipulate small amounts of information for brief periods of time. In the functional imaging literature examining WM dysfunction, there has been increasing reference to “neural compensation” and “brain reorganization” to describe the altered (and almost universally increased) neural activation observed when comparing clinical samples to healthy adults. Specifically, the term “compensation” has been used to describe either transient or permanent alterations in neural activity, which operates to facilitate performance. Therefore, in this paper, the term “compensation” will be used to indicate a positive relationship between task performance and brain activation. The term “brain reorganization” has typically been used to indicate that the neural networks associated with WM have been permanently altered because of neurological insult, so that insult induces a “rewiring” of WM networks. Therefore, the primary distinction between “compensation” and “brain reorganization” is that the former may be transient whereas the latter represents permanent changes in the neural networks involved in WM. Both of these terms, however, are used to describe increased neural activity that facilitates performance in clinical samples. Investigators have thus concluded that altered neural activity on tasks of WM in clinical samples is explicitly caused by the injury and represents: (1) recruitment of neural resources to bolster WM performance, and/or (2) permanent brain changes in the neural networks responsible for WM. These interpretations are challenged in this paper by first addressing the argument that clinical and control groups have been equated for task performance and, second, by proposing that a native support mechanism, cognitive control, may account for the findings to date.

In brief, cognitive control has been used to describe the neural resources that provide a framework for assisting in task processing by maintaining superordinate goals and the means to achieve those goals (Miller & Cohen, Reference Miller and Cohen2001). The neural substrate of cognitive control processes has been hypothesized to include anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and prefrontal cortex (PFC), with ACC being dedicated to conflict monitoring (Botvinick et al., Reference Botvinick, Nystrom, Fissell, Carter and Cohen1999; van Veen et al., Reference van Veen, Cohen, Botvinick, Stenger and Carter2001), including semantic and retrieval conflict (Maril et al., Reference Maril, Wagner and Schacter2001; van Veen & Carter, Reference van Veen and Carter2005) and PFC operating to process task demands and response preparation (see Cohen et al., Reference Cohen, Botvinick and Carter2000). Cognitive control is theorized to provide “top-down” supervisory control during the development of subroutines, which operate to facilitate task performance and has been conceptualized as an emergent property of WM (for review see Baddeley & Dela Sala, Reference Baddeley and Della Sala1996; Courtney, Reference Courtney2004; Gruber & Goschke, Reference Gruber and Goschke2004). Imaging studies of WM in humans have been particularly important in demonstrating the role of PFC in providing control resources for the maintenance, selection, and manipulation of information in working toward a goal state (D'Esposito et al., Reference D'Esposito, Aguirre, Zarahn, Ballard, Shin and Lease1998; Smith & Jonides, Reference Smith and Jonides1999; Wagner, Reference Wagner1999; Wagner et al., Reference Wagner, Maril, Bjork and Schacter2001).

In the clinical imaging studies investigating WM, an interpretation that has been proposed elsewhere (Hillary et al., Reference Hillary, Genova, Chiaravalloti and DeLuca2006) and is extended here, is that the increased PFC involvement on tasks of WM represents neither “compensation” operating to bolster task performance nor “brain reorganization.” Instead, it is proposed that the increased neural activity in PFC commonly observed in clinical studies of WM is associated with poorer performance and largely represents transient fluctuations in the recruitment and reassignment of attentional resources, or cognitive control (Hillary et al., Reference Hillary, Genova, Chiaravalloti and DeLuca2006).

For the purposes of this paper, the current review will be limited to studies examining traumatic brain injury (TBI) and certain brain diseases such as multiple sclerosis (MS) and human immunodeficiency virus (HIV). The primary reason for focusing on a subset of neurological disorders is that, in cases of injury and disease, there is typically an identifiable time period, prior to which the neural system was considered “normal” in its functioning. This approach allows one to examine how a previously healthy neural system adapts to neurological insult. Such a goal is significantly more challenging in disorders such as schizophrenia, for example, where there exist neurodevelopmental and even chronic pharmacologic influences on the neural environment making interpretations about the “normal” role of PFC and cognitive control tenuous. Moreover, in other clinical samples including cerebrovascular accident and cortical and subcortical degenerative processes, the confluence of normal aging and pathophysiology simultaneously influence the expression of neural plasticity. Therefore, the literature discussed here is gathered from studies of TBI, HIV, MS, chronic fatigue syndrome (CFS), and alcohol abuse.

Goals of this Paper

This paper will focus on a subset of functional imaging studies in order to achieve two goals, both organized around the study of brain reorganization hypotheses. The first goal of this review is to summarize the findings and interpretations in the current clinical WM literature. It is a primary aim in this first part to offer an alternative explanation to the prevailing view that changes in neural activity in clinical samples represent neural compensation and/or brain reorganization. Secondly, it is a goal to develop testable predictions for examining the nature of PFC recruitment in WM dysfunction and to outline the methodological difficulties encountered when using functional imaging to test these predictions.

Integrating the Current Findings: Determining the Meaning of PFC Recruitment

Accuracy, reaction time, and activation

In order to understand how data in the clinical WM literature have been interpreted, a brief background on performance measurement is required.

Two of the most consistent methodological constraints in functional imaging work are to keep the study participant “busy” during data acquisition and guarantee that participants maintain high rates of task accuracy (the latter being critically important if response scenarios are dichotomous). Failure to comply with these mandates results in: (1) data that misrepresent the cognitive function examined because “activation” includes excessive off-task averaging, and (2) data that are difficult to interpret because of chance performance; the examiner cannot guarantee that the subject was occupied with the task during the experiment. In doing so, task accuracy is artificially restricted and reaction time (RT) better characterizes the variance in task performance; yet in clinical imaging studies, to date, RT has been under-examined.

Imaging data have little meaning without a behavioral reference point, and what requires emphasis is the very long history dedicated to understanding the relationship between accuracy, RT, and task load manipulations (see Donders, Reference Donders1969; Sternberg, Reference Sternberg1969; Treisman & Souther, Reference Treisman and Souther1985). There are important differences in the types of information provided by measures of accuracy and RT and by examining accuracy in isolation, there is critical variability in behavioral performance that has been under-examined in clinical functional imaging studies of WM. For example, RT has been shown to be more sensitive than response accuracy in predicting PFC involvement during tasks of speeded processing and WM; RT has been shown repeatedly in healthy adults to maintain a positive relationship with the extent of neural activation in PFC (Bergerbest et al., Reference Bergerbest, Ghahremani and Gabrieli2004; Durston et al., Reference Durston, Davidson, Thomas, Worden, Tottenham, Martinez, Watts, Ulug and Casey2003; Rypma et al., Reference Rypma, Berger and D'Esposito2002; Rypma & D'Esposito, Reference Rypma and D'Esposito2000).

At least one clinical imaging study of WM has incorporated information about RT noting a relationship between RT and neural recruitment, even when the rate of accuracy was comparable between groups (Chang et al., Reference Chang, Speck, Miller, Braun, Jovicich, Koch, Itti and Ernst2001). Chang et al. (Reference Chang, Speck, Miller, Braun, Jovicich, Koch, Itti and Ernst2001), examined individuals with HIV demonstrating that, compared to accuracy, RT maintained a greater correlation with neural recruitment (e.g., % signal change, extent of activation) in PFC. These findings are important for the current review primarily because of the assumption often presented by examiners of WM dysfunction: if there are no between-group differences in task accuracy, the differences in task-related brain activation indicate permanent changes in the representative neural network. This method for equating behavioral performance between groups is problematic, because it may be insensitive to very fundamental between-group differences in information processing speed/efficiency.

Current Findings in Clinical Imaging of WM

Nearly all imaging studies of WM dysfunction have noted some difference in the brain “activation” between clinical samples and healthy adults. When the results of these studies are considered together, the primary findings are remarkably consistent (see Table 1 for a summary). With few exceptions, WM is associated with increased neural activity, or more elaborate neural networks (i.e., “dispersion”), in clinical samples, and a primary site of additional neural recruitment has been ventrolateral and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC and DLPFC).

Table 1a. Studies revealing increased PFC and/or ACC activity as a primary finding

Note

* data apply to encoding portion of recognition task only,

** in mild but not severely impaired MS.

Summary of the 33 studies reviewed, with Table 1a reporting a primary finding of increased PFC (either VLPFC, DLPFC, or both) and/or ACC involvement in the clinical sample. Table 1b consists of those studies where a primary finding was not increased PFC activity.

Abbreviations: ACC = anterior cingulated cortex, CBF = cerebral blood flow, CFS = chronic fatigue syndrome, CISSMS = clinically isolated syndrome suggestive of MS, DAI = diffusive, axonal injury, DMS = delayed match to sample task, DLPFC = dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, ETOH = alcohol abuse, F = frontal, fMRI = functional magnetic resonance imaging, IFG = inferior frontal gyrus, PVSAT = paced visual serial addition task, TBI = traumatic brain injury, T = temporal, PCC = posterior cingulated cortex, PCS = post concussion syndrome, VLPFC = ventrolateral prefrontal cortex.

Table 1b. Studies where increased PFC and/or ACC activity not found

Note

Summary of the 33 studies reviewed, with Table 1a reporting a primary finding of increased PFC (either VLPFC, DLPFC, or both) and/or ACC involvement in the clinical sample. Table 1b consists of those studies where a primary finding was not increased PFC activity.

Abbreviations: ACC = anterior cingulated cortex, CBF = cerebral blood flow, CFS = chronic fatigue syndrome, CISSMS = clinically isolated syndrome suggestive of MS, DAI = diffusive, axonal injury, DMS = delayed match to sample task, DLPFC = dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, ETOH = alcohol abuse, F = frontal, fMRI = functional magnetic resonance imaging, IFG = inferior frontal gyrus, PVSAT = paced visual serial addition task, TBI = traumatic brain injury, T = temporal, PCC = posterior cingulated cortex, PCS = post concussion syndrome, VLPFC = ventrolateral prefrontal cortex.

Despite the striking consistencies in the findings summarized in Table 1, interpretations of these findings vary, with investigators commonly arriving at one of two general conclusions. In studies of more severely impaired samples, where cognitive deficits are conspicuous, and therefore evident when measuring task accuracy alone, a negative relationship between performance and activation has been observed. These findings have been believed to represent “neural inefficiency” and increased activation in PFC and other regions has been linked to diminished performance (Chang et al., Reference Chang, Tomasi, Yakupov, Lozar, Arnold, Caparelli and Ernst2004; Chiaravalloti et al., Reference Chiaravalloti, Hillary, Ricker, Christodoulou, Kalnin, Liu, Steffener and DeLuca2005; Christodoulou et al., Reference Christodoulou, DeLuca, Ricker, Madigan, Bly, Lange, Kalnin, Liu, Steffener, Diamond and Ni2001; Hillary et al., Reference Hillary, Chiaravalloti, Ricker, Steffener, Bly, Lange, Liu, Kalnin and DeLuca2003; Perlstein et al., Reference Perlstein, Cole, Demery, Seignourel, Dixit, Larson and Briggs2004). In cases of mild brain dysfunction, where task accuracy alone may not differentiate the clinical and control groups, examiners have interpreted the recruitment of neural resources as “compensatory” and operating to bolster performance (Audoin et al., Reference Audoin, Ibarrola, Ranjeva, Confort-Gouny, Malikova, Ali-Cherif, Pelletier and Cozzone2003; Lange et al., Reference Lange, Steffener, Cook, Bly, Christodoulou, Liu, Deluca and Natelson2005; Maruishi et al., Reference Maruishi, Miyatani, Nakao and Muranaka2007; McAllister et al., Reference McAllister, Saykin, Flashman, Sparling, Johnson, Guerin, Mamourian, Weaver and Yanofsky1999, Reference McAllister, Sparling, Flashman, Guerin, Mamourian and Saykin2001; Penner et al., Reference Penner, Rausch, Kappos, Opwis and Radü2003; Staffen et al., Reference Staffen, Mair, Zauner, Unterrainer, Niederhofer, Kutzelnigg, Ritter, Golaszewski, Iglseder and Ladurner2002) or, alternatively, as reflecting more permanent brain changes or “brain reorganization” (Audoin et al., Reference Audoin, Au Duong, Ranjeva, Ibarrola, Malikova, Confort-Gouny, Soulier, Viout, Ali-Chérif, Pelletier and Cozzone2005; Chang et al., Reference Chang, Tomasi, Yakupov, Lozar, Arnold, Caparelli and Ernst2004; Forn et al., Reference Forn, Barros-Loscertales, Escudero, Belloch, Campos, Parcet and Avila2006; Forn et al., Reference Forn, Barros-Loscertales, Escudero, Belloch, Campos, Parcet and Avila2007; Mainero et al., Reference Mainero, Caramia, Pozzilli, Pisani, Pestalozza, Borriello, Bozzao and Pantano2004; Mainero et al., Reference Mainero, Pantano, Caramia and Pozzilli2006; Pantano et al., Reference Pantano, Mainero and Caramia2006). These last two interpretations (compensation and brain reorganization) have been based on the observation that between group differences in task accuracy are negligible (e.g., between group t-tests were non-significant).

For example, by selecting individuals diagnosed with MS who did not exhibit obvious cognitive deficits (Forn et al., Reference Forn, Barros-Loscertales, Escudero, Belloch, Campos, Parcet and Avila2006, Reference Forn, Barros-Loscertales, Escudero, Belloch, Campos, Parcet and Avila2007), investigators have attempted to control the performance-activation confounds endemic to clinical imaging studies (see Price & Friston, Reference Price and Friston1999; Price & Friston, Reference Price and Friston2002). The investigators interpreted increased PFC activation during WM performance in this MS sample as “reflective of the existence of neural reorganizative processes” (Forn et al., Reference Forn, Barros-Loscertales, Escudero, Belloch, Campos, Parcet and Avila2007). However, for reasons emphasized earlier, it remains quite possible in this study and others comparing accuracy alone that subtle, but consequential, processing speed differences were present.

What is at issue here is the “dilemma” that is central to this paper: if the areas believed to represent genuine reorganization in any given neural network directly fluctuate with changes in task performance, and these areas overlap neuroanatomically with support areas observed in healthy adults, it is difficult to conclude that group differences represent brain reorganization. Support for a brain reorganization hypothesis in PFC requires that the “recruited” brain region is not a load-dependent support mechanism operating identically as it would in healthy adults. Making this determination is very difficult in the WM literature, because it requires methodological precision capable of separating “baseline” from “performance-dependent” neural functioning in PFC, one of the most highly flexible and interconnected substrates in the brain. A transient support mechanism such as cognitive control would operate to bolster attentional resources, would be evident predominantly during the creation of subroutines that later facilitate task performance and would likely diminish as performance increases. Because of this, dissociating the effects of normal plasticity from pathology-induced brain reorganization could be achieved by observing changes in the neural network as one practices a task (see Kelly & Garavan, Reference Kelly and Garavan2005) or via longitudinal designs (discussed in greater detail later).

The conflicting explanations for what appears to be quite similar PFC recruitment across distinct clinical samples evokes an important question regarding how neural systems change in response to injury and disease. Is it possible that recruitment of neural resources in mild forms of neurological impairment represents compensation and/or reorganization operating to facilitate performance, yet in cases of more severe cognitive deficit, recruitment of nearly identical neural networks is associated with neural inefficiency and correlated with diminished performance? An explanation with greater parsimony for the convergent findings to date is that the regions consistently recruited across clinical samples represent a native support mechanism(s) (Hillary et al., Reference Hillary, Genova, Chiaravalloti and DeLuca2006). This explanation remains at odds, however, with studies proposing that neural activity in PFC is facilitating performance and what must ultimately be determined in order to clarify this issue is the basic relationship between activation and performance in PFC.

If the recruitment of PFC resources observed in clinical samples is a natural support mechanism (e.g., cognitive control), one would also expect that this “natural” mechanism can be reliably elicited in healthy adults. In fact, examiners have repeatedly documented, that as WM load increases and performance diminishes, there exist parallel increases in PFC activation (Braver et al., Reference Braver, Cohen, Nystrom, Jonides, Smith and Noll1997; D'Esposito et al., Reference D'Esposito, Postle, Ballard and Lease1999; Manoach et al., Reference Manoach, Schlaug, Siewert, Darby, Bly, Benfield, Edelman and Warach1997; Mostofsky et al., Reference Mostofsky, Schafer, Abrams, Goldberg, Flower, Boyce, Courtney, Calhoun, Kraut, Denckla and Pekar2003; Rypma et al., Reference Rypma, Prabhakaran, Desmond, Glover and Gabrieli1999). Work by Perlstein et al. (Reference Perlstein, Cole, Demery, Seignourel, Dixit, Larson and Briggs2004) comparing individuals with mild TBI and healthy adults is consistent with these reports: the investigators observed attendant changes in PFC in direct response to changes in task load irrespective of group membership. Moreover, investigations of healthy adults have demonstrated that as performance improves and a task routine is formalized, activation in PFC decreases (presumably because of diminished demand on cognitive control resources) (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Qin, Stenger and Carter2004; Qin et al., Reference Qin, Carter, Silk, Stenger, Fissell, Goode and Anderson2004; Sohn et al., Reference Sohn, Goode, Stenger, Carter and Anderson2003; Sohn et al., Reference Sohn, Goode, Stenger, Jung, Carter and Anderson2005). One important caveat to this discussion regarding practice, learning, and activation in healthy adults is that, whereas PFC activation may diminish, the role of the ACC may increase as performance improves (Fincham & Anderson, Reference Fincham and Anderson2006). Consistent with this, in a study directly examining cognitive control in individuals with TBI, Scheibel and colleagues (Reference Scheibel, Newsome, Steinberg, Pearson, Rauch, Mao, Troyanskaya, Sharma and Levin2007) revealed that poorer performance in individuals diagnosed with TBI was associated with less ACC involvement. Thus, if findings in clinical samples represent a natural support mechanism similar to what is observed in healthy adults, we would anticipate that PFC as opposed to ACC should be differentially recruited during tasks of WM. In many of the studies summarized here this is the case (see Table 1).

If the PFC recruitment observed in clinical WM studies does represent brain reorganization, what remains unclear is why the “reorganization” is so consistent given the distinct pathophysiology, disease/injury severity, and lesion constellation across clinical samples. The lack of specificity again points to a common neural support mechanism evoked during periods of cerebral challenge. If recruitment of PFC in these clinical studies is a common neural support mechanism, then it should be more closely tied to behavioral performance than the magnitude or site of pathophysiology. There is evidence that this could be the case. In one of the few MS studies to integrate information about the degree of neuropathology with functional imaging results, work by Mainero and colleagues (Reference Mainero, Caramia, Pozzilli, Pisani, Pestalozza, Borriello, Bozzao and Pantano2004) demonstrated that lesion load, regardless of its neuroanatomical origin, was correlated with right DLFC recruitment. These findings are very similar to work using pMRS and fMRI in conjunction to examine HIV; metabolic abnormalities found in several brain regions, including right frontal white matter, midfrontal gray matter, and basal ganglia, were correlated with PFC recruitment during a WM task (Chang et al., Reference Chang, Tomasi, Yakupov, Lozar, Arnold, Caparelli and Ernst2004). As noted above, in MS and moderate and severe TBI, subjects with the poorest performance have been shown to exhibit the greatest right DLPFC recruitment (Chiaravalloti et al., Reference Chiaravalloti, Hillary, Ricker, Christodoulou, Kalnin, Liu, Steffener and DeLuca2005; Christodoulou et al., Reference Christodoulou, DeLuca, Ricker, Madigan, Bly, Lange, Kalnin, Liu, Steffener, Diamond and Ni2001; Hillary et al., Reference Hillary, Chiaravalloti, Ricker, Steffener, Bly, Lange, Liu, Kalnin and DeLuca2003) and increased right DLPFC activity in TBI has been tied to monotonic increases in task load (see Perlstein et al., Reference Perlstein, Cole, Demery, Seignourel, Dixit, Larson and Briggs2004). Thus, increased PFC involvement in these studies seems to be directly influenced by task performance but it is essentially unaffected by the etiology of neurological insult or even the specific neuroanatomical substrate that is affected. Moreover, WM requires the involvement of multiple neural substrates including the ACC, parietal areas, and cerebellum, yet these areas have not been common sites of neural recruitment; regardless of the task or clinical sample, the most consistent finding has been increased PFC activity.

Taken together, there appears to be greater evidence for a general mechanism in PFC (e.g., cognitive control) that provides transient support during periods of cerebral challenge, as opposed to permanent, injury-specific changes in the functional neural networks representing WM functioning (i.e., brain reorganization). In other words, the increased activation in PFC observed in the studies summarized here may be no different from the transient waxing and waning demand on PFC resources occurring day-to-day or even moment-to-moment in healthy adults as task demands continuous change.

Testing Brain Reorganization Hypotheses

It has been proposed thus far that there are at least three mechanisms that could explain the role of PFC recruitment in WM dysfunction in the literature reviewed here. The first, brain reorganization, supposes that additional PFC recruitment reflects underlying changes in the brain structure and/or changes in the functional network associated with WM tasks. This change is presumably permanent and the recruited neural resources maintain a positive relationship between activation and performance. A second mechanism, neural compensation, as it has been defined in this literature, operates similarly to brain reorganization, (it has a positive performance/activation relationship), but, unlike brain reorganization, it does not imply permanent changes in the neural network. The difference between these two explanations is subtle and linked almost exclusively to the presumed permanence of these changes. A third explanation is that the “aberrant” PFC recruitment is neither abnormal nor permanent; it is the same process that is observed in healthy adults, albeit at a lower threshold. For example, in work by Sweet and colleagues (Reference Sweet, Rao, Primeau, Durgerian and Cohen2006), healthy adults recruited nearly identical right PFC resources as the MS sample but only after increases in task load. The third explanation thus offers that PFC recruitment is a native support mechanism. Based on these three explanations for the recruitment of PFC resources in WM, I propose several testable predictions:

Prediction 1

If the recruitment of PFC resources represents either brain reorganization or neural compensation, then with practice of a task, PFC activation should remain the same or increase as task facility increases.

Prediction 2

If the recruitment of PFC resources represents either brain reorganization or neural compensation, the most impaired subjects in any clinical group should demonstrate the least involvement of PFC resources.

Prediction 3

If the recruitment of PFC resources represents brain reorganization, then recruitment of neural resources should be initiated at the outset of recovery and should increase over the course of recovery as task performance improves.

The following two sections focus on methodological considerations for examining WM dysfunction and testing the predictions offered here.

Direct Examination of Performance-activation Relationships

In order to examine the predictions offered here, it is critical that one understands the basic relationship between activation and performance. This can be achieved by manipulating task load and directly examining the interaction between alterations in the task and the associated neural activity (as in Perlstein et al., Reference Perlstein, Cole, Demery, Seignourel, Dixit, Larson and Briggs2004), or by using behavior (e.g., accuracy, RT) as a regressor during analysis. These methods permit the separation of baseline neural network from components in the neural network that modulate task performance. Additionally, such manipulations allow one to determine if there are between group differences in how an identical neural substrate is moderating performance (Perlstein et al., Reference Perlstein, Cole, Demery, Seignourel, Dixit, Larson and Briggs2004). Thus, manipulation of task load and direct examination of task/performance relationships provide the opportunity to assess the nature of between group differences (as opposed to simply reconfirming that the groups are different).

As noted throughout this paper, much of the work to date infers that the resources recruited in PFC operate to facilitate WM performance. That is, increased involvement of PFC allows individuals sustaining neurological insult to perform comparably when compared to healthy adults. With the exception of the work in normal aging (Davis et al., Reference Davis, Dennis, Daselaar, Fleck and Cabeza2007; Reuter-Lorenz et al., Reference Reuter-Lorenz, Jonides, Smith, Hartley, Miller, Marshuetz and Koeppe2000; Rypma & D'Esposito, Reference Rypma and D'Esposito2000), however, there are virtually no WM studies to date, in healthy or clinical samples, that have demonstrated this relationship. Unfortunately, less than 1/3 of the studies reviewed here directly examined the relationship between WM performance and neural activity. If PFC recruitment in WM dysfunction does facilitate performance, we would anticipate that, in clinical samples, practicing a task would increase PFC involvement (consistent with Prediction #1). The effects of practice on neural activity have not been directly examined in clinical studies of WM dysfunction. As noted, in healthy adults it has been demonstrated that when engaged in tasks of attention, speeded processing, and WM increased task facility results in diminished, and not increased, PFC involvement (for review see Kelly & Garavan, Reference Kelly and Garavan2005). If PFC recruitment in WM dysfunction is reflecting the need for greater attentional resources (i.e., cognitive control) as the task is more slowly processed, we would anticipate that demand on these resources would diminish as one practices the task (contrary to Prediction #1).

A separate consideration is that the influence of PFC involvement in any clinical sample may also be deduced by examining within-group variance in the BOLD response (Prediction #2). This is an important consideration, given the significant methodological hurdles involved in making between group comparisons in clinical samples. For example, according to Prediction #2, if PFC recruitment does represent brain reorganization or neural compensation, then subjects demonstrating the greatest cognitive impairment should show the least PFC involvement. Very few studies reviewed in Table 1 engage in this type of analysis and in studies where the investigators specifically provide data for the most cognitively impaired subjects (e.g., Audoin et al., Reference Audoin, Au Duong, Ranjeva, Ibarrola, Malikova, Confort-Gouny, Soulier, Viout, Ali-Chérif, Pelletier and Cozzone2005; Chiaravalloti et al., Reference Chiaravalloti, Hillary, Ricker, Christodoulou, Kalnin, Liu, Steffener and DeLuca2005) the opposite was shown to be true (i.e., the most impaired subjects demonstrated the greatest PFC and parietal cortex involvement). Larger sample sizes in future studies of clinical WM will permit this type of within-group analysis.

Verification of Brain Reorganization using Longitudinal Designs

If the PFC recruitment observed in this literature does represent permanent brain changes in the neural network (i.e., brain reorganization), we would anticipate that, over the course of recovery, increased involvement of PFC would operate to facilitate performance (Prediction #3). Examining the same subject during the recovery period offers the opportunity to document the consistency and permanence of the brain changes following diagnosis.

An important advantage of using longitudinal designs is that within-subject comparisons eliminate many of the methodological problems inherent in between-subjects designs, including the daunting task of guaranteeing identical performance between the clinical and comparison groups. Longitudinal designs also permit data interpretation with fewer statistical comparisons (e.g., second level, or group analysis is often unnecessary). Given the enormous number of statistical comparisons used to determine what is and what is not “active” in each of the group representative images; without appropriate statistical control, between-group comparisons may reflect activation differences that are statistical artifact (Price et al., Reference Price, Crinion and Friston2006). Therefore, in clinical studies, longitudinal designs eliminate the effects of within-group heterogeneity on statistical comparisons and provide the opportunity to examine within-subject change over time.

In addition to reducing the likelihood of obtaining spurious findings and simplifying data interpretation, longitudinal designs may help to characterize the nature of the differences that have been observed in clinical samples. It has been proposed here that transient, innate mechanisms such as cognitive control can account for many of the findings in PFC. However, this explanation is based on the current cross-sectional data that provide a “snap-shot” at one time point during recovery. What remains to be determined is the interplay between natural support mechanisms and injury/disease induced changes in WM networks over the course of recovery. This relationship is likely to be very fluid and, therefore, quite challenging to disentangle. Ultimately, cognitive control may play an important role in determining where and how plasticity is expressed in WM (Kelly & Garavan, Reference Kelly and Garavan2005) and within-subject designs permit the opportunity to examine alterations in the neural network that are transient versus those that are permanent.

Finally, longitudinal methods may be useful for clarifying inconsistent or nuanced findings in the literature. For example, it has occasionally been observed that neural activity in parts of a relevant network actually decreases in cases of neurologic insult. The current review has focused almost solely on the increased activation typically observed in PFC and support regions for WM and processing speed. Several studies, however, have shown decreased activation in the baseline neural network when comparing clinical samples and healthy adults (Chang et al., Reference Chang, Speck, Miller, Braun, Jovicich, Koch, Itti and Ernst2001; Wishart et al., Reference Wishart, Saykin, McDonald, Mamourian, Flashman, Schuschu, Ryan, Fadul and Kasper2004). One hypothesis to be tested is that the failure to “activate” similar baseline networks may be indicative of neural disconnection, requiring an increased role of cognitive control resources. Separately, in the case of very severe pathology (e.g., late stages of Alzheimer's disease), neural recruitment may be impossible because of significant gray matter loss and the eradication of viable neural networks. In cross sectional work, therefore, imaging results may be highly dependent upon the time elapsing between diagnosis and MRI data collection. Longitudinal designs thus allow the opportunity to examine the evolution of neural change/recovery, including potentially competing effects such as neural disconnection and the natural recruitment of cognitive control resources. Ultimately, future studies must emphasize longitudinal designs not only to definitively test brain reorganization hypotheses (such as the predictions offered here), but to address important questions regarding the time course, magnitude, and nature of the altered brain activation in clinical samples.

CONCLUSION

Clinical imaging studies have advanced our understanding of the influence of brain injury and disease on neural networks. It was the goal of this paper to integrate one specific literature and offer an interpretation that accounts for the findings to date. It has been proposed that in functional imaging studies of WM dysfunction, important methodological concerns obscure the meaning of existing findings and that the compensatory and brain reorganization hypotheses presented thus far may not fully capture the convergent data. It has been argued that a natural mechanism, cognitive control, may account for many of the findings in this literature and that this phenomenon must be accounted for in order to begin understanding brain reorganization processes in cases of brain injury and disease.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The author thanks Sheri Berenbaum and Peter Arnett for their important feedback on previous versions of this manuscript. The information in this manuscript and the manuscript itself has never been published either electronically or in print. The author has no financial relationships that could be interpreted as a conflict of interest affecting this manuscript.

References

REFERENCES

Anderson, J.R., Qin, Y., Stenger, V.A., & Carter, C.S. (2004). The relationship of three cortical regions to an information processing model. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 637653.Google Scholar
Audoin, B., Au Duong, M.V., Ranjeva, J.P., Ibarrola, D., Malikova, I., Confort-Gouny, S., Soulier, E., Viout, P., Ali-Chérif, A., Pelletier, J., & Cozzone, P.J. (2005). Magnetic resonance study of the influence of tissue damage and cortical reorganization on PASAT performance at the earliest stage of multiple sclerosis. Human Brain Mapping, 24, 216228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Audoin, B., Ibarrola, D., Ranjeva, J.P., Confort-Gouny, S., Malikova, I., Ali-Cherif, A., Pelletier, J., & Cozzone, P. (2003). Compensatory cortical activation observed by fMRI during a cognitive task at the earliest stage of MS. Human Brain Mapping, 20, 5158.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. & Della Sala, S. (1996). Working memory and executive control. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B, Biological Sciences, 351, 13971403.Google ScholarPubMed
Bergerbest, D., Ghahremani, D.G., & Gabrieli, J.D. (2004). Neural correlates of auditory repetition priming: Reduced fMRI activation in the auditory cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 966977.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bobholz, J.A., Rao, S.M., Lobeck, L., Elsinger, C., Gleason, A., Kanz, J., Durgerian, S., & Maas, E. (2006). fMRI study of episodic memory in relapsing-remitting MS: Correlation with T2 lesion volume. Neurology, 67, 16401645.Google Scholar
Botvinick, M., Nystrom, L.E., Fissell, K., Carter, C.S., & Cohen, J.D. (1999). Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulated cortex. Nature, 402, 179181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braver, T.S., Cohen, J.D., Nystrom, L.E., Jonides, J., Smith, E.E., & Noll, D.C. (1997). A parametric study of prefrontal cortex involvement in human working memory. Neuroimage, 5, 4962.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caseras, X., Mataix-Cols, D., Giampietro, V., Rimes, K.A., Brammer, M., Zelaya, F., Chalder, T., & Godfrey, E.L. (2006). Probing the working memory system in chronic fatigue syndrome: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study using the n-back task. Psychosomatic Medicine, 68, 947955.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chang, L., Speck, O., Miller, E.N., Braun, J., Jovicich, J., Koch, C., Itti, L., & Ernst, T. (2001). Neural correlates of attention and working memory deficits in HIV patients. Neurology, 57, 10011007.Google Scholar
Chang, L., Tomasi, D., Yakupov, R., Lozar, C., Arnold, S., Caparelli, E., & Ernst, T. (2004). Adaptation of the attention network in human immunodeficiency virus brain injury. Annals of Neurology, 56, 259272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, J.K., Johnston, K.M., Frey, S., Petrides, M., Worsley, K., & Ptito, A. (2004). Functional abnormalities in symptomatic concussed athletes: An fMRI study. Neuroimage, 22, 6882.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, S.H., Kareken, D.A., Fastenau, P.S., Trexler, L.E., & Hutchins, G.D. (2003). A study of persistent post-concussion symptoms in mild head trauma using positron emission tomography. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 74, 326332.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chiaravalloti, N., Hillary, F., Ricker, J., Christodoulou, C., Kalnin, A., Liu, W.C., Steffener, L., & DeLuca, J. (2005). Cerebral activation patterns during working memory performance in multiple sclerosis using fMRI. Journal of Clinical & Experimental Neuropsychology, 27, 3354.Google Scholar
Christodoulou, C., DeLuca, J., Ricker, J.H., Madigan, N.K., Bly, B.M., Lange, G., Kalnin, A.J., Liu, W.C., Steffener, J., Diamond, B.J., & Ni, A.C. (2001). Functional magnetic resonance imaging of working memory impairment after traumatic brain injury. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 71, 161168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, J.D., Botvinick, M., & Carter, C.S. (2000). Anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex: Who's in control? Nature Neuroscience, 3, 421423.Google Scholar
Cook, D.B., O'Connor, P.J., Lange, G., & Steffener, J. (2007). Functional neuroimaging correlates of mental fatigue induced by cognition among chronic fatigue syndrome patients and controls. Neuroimage, 36, 108122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Courtney, S.M. (2004). Attention and cognitive control as emergent properties of information representation in working memory. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 4, 501516.Google Scholar
Davis, S.W., Dennis, N.A., Daselaar, S.M., Fleck, M.S., & Cabeza, R. (2007). Que PASA? The posterior anterior shift in aging. Cerebral Cortex, Oct 8 (Epub ahead of print).Google ScholarPubMed
Desmond, J.E., Chen, S.H., DeRosa, E., Pryor, M.R., Pfefferbaum, A., & Sullivan, E.V. (2003). Increased frontocerebellar activation in alcoholics during verbal working memory: An fMRI study. Neuroimage, 19, 15101520.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
D'Esposito, M., Aguirre, G. K., Zarahn, E., Ballard, D., Shin, R. K., & Lease, J. (1998). Functional MRI studies of spatial and nonspatial working memory. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research, 7, 113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
D'Esposito, M., Postle, B.R., Ballard, D., & Lease, J. (1999). Maintenance versus manipulation of information held in working memory: An event-related fMRI study. Brain & Cognition, 41, 6686.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donders, F.C. (1969). On the speed of mental processes. Acta Psychologica, 30, 412431.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Durston, S., Davidson, M.C., Thomas, K.M., Worden, M.S., Tottenham, N., Martinez, A., Watts, R., Ulug, A.M., & Casey, B.J. (2003). Parametric manipulation of conflict and response competition using rapid mixed-trial event-related fMRI. Neuroimage, 20, 21352141.Google Scholar
Ernst, T., Chang, L., & Arnold, S. (2003). Increased glial metabolites predict increased working memory network activation in HIV brain injury. Neuroimage, 19, 16861693.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ernst, T., Chang, L., Jovicich, J., Ames, N., & Arnold, S. (2002). Abnormal brain activation on functional MRI in cognitively asymptomatic HIV patients. Neurology, 59, 13431349.Google Scholar
Fincham, J.M. & Anderson, J.R. (2006). Distinct roles of the anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex in the acquisition and performance of a cognitive skill. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103, 1294112946.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forn, C., Barros-Loscertales, A., Escudero, J., Belloch, V., Campos, S., Parcet, M.A., & Avila, C. (2006). Cortical reorganization during PASAT task in MS patients with preserved working memory functions. Neuroimage, 31, 686691.Google Scholar
Forn, C., Barros-Loscertales, A., Escudero, J., Belloch, V., Campos, S., Parcet, M.A., & Avila, C. (2007). Compensatory activations in patients with multiple sclerosis during preserved performance on the auditory N-back task. Human Brain Mapping, 28, 424430.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gruber, O. & Goschke, T. (2004). Executive control emerging from dynamic interactions between brain systems mediating language, working memory and attentional processes. Acta Psychologica, 115, 105121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hillary, F.G., Chiaravalloti, N.D., Ricker, J.H., Steffener, J., Bly, B.M., Lange, G., Liu, W.C., Kalnin, A.J., & DeLuca, J. (2003). An investigation of working memory rehearsal in multiple sclerosis using fMRI. Journal of Clinical & Experimental Neuropsychology, 25, 965978.Google Scholar
Hillary, F.G., Genova, H.M., Chiaravalloti, N.D., & DeLuca, J. (2006). Prefrontal modulation of working memory performance in brain injury and disease. Human Brain Mapping, 27, 837847.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelly, A.M. & Garavan, H. (2005). Human functional neuroimaging of brain changes associated with practice. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 10891102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lange, G., Steffener, J., Cook, D.B., Bly, B.M., Christodoulou, C., Liu, W.C., Deluca, J., & Natelson, B.H. (2005). Objective evidence of cognitive complaints in Chronic Fatigue Syndrome: A BOLD fMRI study of verbal working memory. Neuroimage, 26, 513524.Google Scholar
Mainero, C., Caramia, F., Pozzilli, C., Pisani, A., Pestalozza, I., Borriello, G., Bozzao, L., & Pantano, P. (2004). fMRI evidence of brain reorganization during attention and memory tasks in multiple sclerosis. Neuroimage, 21, 858867.Google Scholar
Mainero, C., Pantano, P., Caramia, F., & Pozzilli, C. (2006). Brain reorganization during attention and memory tasks in multiple sclerosis: Insights from functional MRI studies. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 245, 9398.Google Scholar
Manoach, D.S., Schlaug, G., Siewert, B., Darby, D.G., Bly, B.M., Benfield, A., Edelman, R.R., & Warach, S. (1997). Prefrontal cortex fMRI signal changes are correlated with working memory load. Neuroreport, 8, 545549.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maril, A., Wagner, A.D., & Schacter, D.L. (2001). On the tip of the tongue: An event-related fMRI study of semantic retrieval failure and cognitive conflict. Neuron, 31, 653660.Google Scholar
Maruishi, M., Miyatani, M., Nakao, T., & Muranaka, H. (2007). Compensatory cortical activation during performance of an attention task by patients with diffuse axonal injury: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 78, 168173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McAllister, T.W., Saykin, A.J., Flashman, L.A., Sparling, M.B., Johnson, S.C., Guerin, S.J., Mamourian, A.C., Weaver, J.B., & Yanofsky, N. (1999). Brain activation during working memory 1 month after mild traumatic brain injury: A functional MRI study. Neurology, 53, 13001308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McAllister, T.W., Sparling, M.B., Flashman, L.A., Guerin, S.J., Mamourian, A.C., & Saykin, A.J. (2001). Differential working memory load effects after mild traumatic brain injury. Neuroimage, 14, 10041012.Google Scholar
Miller, E.K. & Cohen, J.D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 167202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mostofsky, S.H., Schafer, J.G., Abrams, M.T., Goldberg, M.C., Flower, A.A., Boyce, A., Courtney, S.M., Calhoun, V.D., Kraut, M.A., Denckla, M.B., & Pekar, J.J. (2003). fMRI evidence that the neural basis of response inhibition is task-dependent. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research, 17, 419430.Google Scholar
Newsome, M.R., Scheibel, R.S., Steinberg, J.L., Troyanskaya, M., Sharma, R.G., Rauch, R.A., Li, X., & Levin, H.S. (2007). Working memory brain activation following severe traumatic brain injury. Cortex, 43, 95111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pantano, P., Mainero, C., & Caramia, F. (2006). Functional brain reorganization in multiple sclerosis: Evidence from fMRI studies. Journal of Neuroimaging, 16, 104114.Google Scholar
Penner, I.K., Rausch, M., Kappos, L., Opwis, K., & Radü, E.W. (2003). Analysis of impairment related functional architecture in MS patients during performance of different attention tasks. Journal of Neurology, 250, 461472Google Scholar
Perlstein, W.M., Cole, M.A., Demery, J.A., Seignourel, P.J., Dixit, N.K., Larson, M.J., & Briggs, R.W. (2004). Parametric manipulation of working memory load in traumatic brain injury: Behavioral and neural correlates. Journal of the International Neuropsychology Society, 10, 724741.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pfefferbaum, A., Desmond, J.E., Galloway, C., Menon, V., Glover, G.H., & Sullivan, E.V. (2001). Reorganization of frontal systems used by alcoholics for spatial working memory: An fMRI study. Neuroimage, 14 (Pt. 1), 720.Google Scholar
Price, C.J., Crinion, J., & Friston, K. (2006). Design and analysis of fMRI studies with neurologically impaired patients. Journal of Magnetic Resonance Imaging, 23, 816826.Google Scholar
Price, C.J. & Friston, K.J. (1999). Scanning patients with tasks they can perform. Human Brain Mapping, 8, 102108.3.0.CO;2-J>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Price, C.J. & Friston, K. (2002). Functional imaging studies of neuropsychological patients: Applications and limitations. Neurocase, 8, 345354.Google Scholar
Qin, Y., Carter, C.S., Silk, E.M., Stenger, V.A., Fissell, K., Goode, A., & Anderson, J.R. (2004). Predicting the practice effects on the BOLD function of fMRI in a symbolic manipulation task. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States, 100, 56865691.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reuter-Lorenz, P.A., Jonides, J., Smith, E.E., Hartley, A., Miller, A., Marshuetz, C., & Koeppe, R.A. (2000). Age differences in the frontal lateralization of verbal and spatial working memory revealed by PET. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 174187.Google Scholar
Rypma, B., Berger, J.S., & D'Esposito, M. (2002). The influence of working-memory demand and subject performance on prefrontal cortical activity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 721731.Google Scholar
Rypma, B. & D'Esposito, M. (2000). Isolating the neural mechanisms of age-related changes in human working memory. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 509515.Google Scholar
Rypma, B., Prabhakaran, .V, Desmond, J.E., Glover, G.H., & Gabrieli, J.D. (1999). Load-dependent roles of frontal brain regions in the maintenance of working memory. Neuroimage, 9, 216226.Google Scholar
Scheibel, R.S., Newsome, M.R., Steinberg, J.L., Pearson, D.A., Rauch, R.A., Mao, H., Troyanskaya, M., Sharma, R.G., & Levin, H.S. (2007). Altered brain activation during cognitive control in patients with moderate to severe traumatic brain injury. Neurorehabilitation & Neural Repair, 21, 3645.Google Scholar
Schweinsburg, A.D., Schweinsburg, B.C., Cheung, E.H., Brown, G.G., Brown, S.A., & Tapert, S.F. (2005). fMRI response to spatial working memory in adolescents with comorbid marijuana and alcohol use disorders. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 79, 201210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, E.E. & Jonides, J. (1999). Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science, 283, 16571661.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sohn, M.H., Goode, A., Stenger, V.A., Carter, C.S. & Anderson, J.R. (2003). Competition and representation during memory retrieval: Roles of prefrontal cortex and posterior parietal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States, 100, 74127417.Google Scholar
Sohn, M.H., Goode, A., Stenger, V.A., Jung, K.J., Carter, C.S. & Anderson, J.R. (2005). An information processing model of three cortical regions: Evidence in episodic memory retrieval. Neuroimage, 25, 2133.Google Scholar
Staffen, W., Mair, A., Zauner, H., Unterrainer, J., Niederhofer, H., Kutzelnigg, A., Ritter, S., Golaszewski, S., Iglseder, B., & Ladurner, G. (2002). Cognitive function and fMRI in patients with multiple sclerosis: Evidence for compensatory cortical activation during an attention task. Brain, 125, 12751282.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sternberg, S. (1969). The discovery of processing stages: Extension of Donders method. Acta Psychologica, 30, 276315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sweet, L.H., Rao, S.M., Primeau, M., Durgerian, S., & Cohen, R.A. (2006). Functional magnetic resonance imaging response to increased verbal working memory demands among patients with multiple sclerosis. Human Brain Mapping, 27, 2836.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sweet, L.H., Rao, S.M., Primeau, M., Mayer, A.R., & Cohen, R.A. (2004). Functional magnetic resonance imaging of working memory among multiple sclerosis patients. Journal of Neuroimaging, 2, 150157.Google Scholar
Tapert, S.F., Brown, G.G., Baratta, M.V., & Brown, S.A. (2004). fMRI BOLD response to alcohol stimuli in alcohol dependent young women. Addictive Behaviors, 29, 3350.Google Scholar
Treisman, A. & Souther, J. (1985). Search asymmetry: A diagnostic for preattentive processing of separable features. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 114, 285310.Google Scholar
van Veen, V. & Carter, C.S. (2005). Separating semantic conflict and response conflict in the Stroop Task: A functional MRI study. NeuroImage, 27, 497504.Google Scholar
van Veen, V., Cohen, J.D., Botvinick, M.M., Stenger, V.A., & Carter, C.S. (2001). Anterior cingulate cortex, conflict monitoring, and levels of processing. Neuroimage, 14, 13021308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wagner, A.D. (1999). Working memory contributions to human learning and remembering. Neuron, 22, 1922.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wagner, A.D., Maril, A., Bjork, R.A., & Schacter, D.L. (2001). Prefrontal contributions to executive control: fMRI evidence for functional distinctions within lateral prefrontal cortex. Neuroimage, 14, 13371347.Google Scholar
Wishart, H.A., Saykin, A.J., McDonald, B.C., Mamourian, A.C., Flashman, L.A., Schuschu, K.R., Ryan, K.A., Fadul, C.E., & Kasper, L.H. (2004). Brain activation patterns associated with working memory in relapsing-remitting MS. Neurology, 62, 234238.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1a. Studies revealing increased PFC and/or ACC activity as a primary finding

Figure 1

Table 1b. Studies where increased PFC and/or ACC activity not found