Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gbm5v Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T19:31:11.933Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Global/local scoring modifications for the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure: Relation to unilateral temporal lobe epilepsy patients

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 May 2006

REGINA MCCONLEY
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham, Alabama
ROY MARTIN
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham, Alabama
JAMES BAÑOS
Affiliation:
James Baños, Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham, Alabama
PAUL BLANTON
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham, Alabama
EDWARD FAUGHT
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham, Alabama

Abstract

Observation of figural reproductions of right temporal lobe epilepsy (RTLE) patients have revealed overall configuration errors, whereas in left temporal lobe epilepsy (LTLE) patients, the global aspect of performance is generally preserved. This study compared performance of individuals with temporal lobe epilepsy (LTLE, n = 83; RTLE, n = 63) on a global/local modification of the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (RCFT). In Phase I, neuropsychologists (n = 6) and neuropsychology fellows (n = 3) completed a survey identifying the global and local aspects of the RCFT. Questionnaire responses were used to categorize a list of global and local items (five global, five local) for re-scoring the protocols of TLE patients during study Phase II. Results showed that the RTLE and LTLE groups were not differentiated according to the global or local indices F(1,141) = .385; p = not significant. There were lower local scores for both groups in the copy F(1,142) = 5.23; p = .024, immediate F(1,142) = 445.26; p < .001, and delay trials F(1,142) = 427.82; p < .001, indicating less retention of local information over time. Results suggest general declines in figural memory for local stimulus properties in both unilateral TLE groups. However, this relationship was weakened after controlling for global and local item verbalizability. (JINS, 2006, 12, 383–390.)

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2006 The International Neuropsychological Society

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Abrahams, S., Pickering, A., Polkey, C.E., & Morris, R.G. (1997). Spatial memory deficits in patients with unilateral damage to the right hippocampal formation. Neuropsychologia, 35, 1124.Google Scholar
Barr, W.B. (1997). Examining the right temporal lobe's role in nonverbal memory. Brain and Cognition, 35, 2641.Google Scholar
Barr, W.B., Chelune, G.J., Hermann, B.P., Loring, D.W., Perrine, K., Strauss, E., Trenarry, M.R., & Westerveld, M. (1997). The use of figural reproduction tests as measures of nonverbal memory in epilepsy surgery candidates. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 3, 435443.Google Scholar
Baxendale, S.A., Thompson, P.J., & Van Paesschen, W. (1998). A test of spatial memory and it's clinical utility in the pre-surgical investigation of temporal lobe epilepsy patients. Neuropsychologia, 36, 591602.Google Scholar
Binder, L. (1982). Constructional strategies on complex figure drawings after unilateral brain damage. Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 4, 5158.Google Scholar
Boles, D.B. (1984). Global versus local processing: Is there a hemispheric dichotomy? Neuropsychologia, 22, 445455.Google Scholar
Breier, J.I., Plenger, P.M., Castillo, R., Fuchs, K., Wheless, J.W., Thomas, A.B., Brookshire, B.L., Willmore, L.J., & Papanicolaou, A. (1996). Effects of temporal lobe epilepsy on spatial and figural aspects of memory for a complex geometric figure. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 2, 535540.Google Scholar
Chervinsky, A.B., Mitrushina, M., & Satz, P. (1992). Comparison of four methods of scoring the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Drawing Test on four age groups of normal elderly. Brain Dysfunction, 5, 267287.Google Scholar
Fedio, P. & Mirsky, A.F. (1969). Selective intellectual deficits in children with temporal lobe or centrencephalic epilepsy. Neuropsychologia, 7, 287300.Google Scholar
Kim, H., Yi, S., Son, E.I., & Kim, J. (2003). Material-specific memory in temporal lobe epilepsy: Effects of seizure laterality and language dominance. Neuropsychology, 17, 5968.Google Scholar
Lee, T.M.C., Yip, J.T.H., & Jones-Gotman, M. (2002). Memory deficits after resection from the left or right anterior temporal lobe in humans: A meta-analytic review. Epilepsia, 43, 283291.Google Scholar
Lezak, M. (2004). Neuropsychological Assessment (3rd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
Loring, D.W., Lee, G.P., & Meador, K.J. (1988). Revising the Rey-Osterrieth: Rating right hemisphere recall. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 3, 239247.Google Scholar
Loughead, J., Hammarlund, S., Glosser, D., & Sperling, M. (2000). The developmental scoring system for the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure does not differentiate right versus left temporal lobe epilepsy patients [Abstract]. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 15, 711.Google Scholar
Martin, M. (1979). Hemispheric specialization for local and global processing. Neuropsychologia, 17, 3340.Google Scholar
Meyers, J.E. & Meyers, K.R. (1995). Rey Complex Figure Test and Recognition Trial. Professional Manual. Odessa: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Milner, B. (Ed.). (1975). Hemispheric Specialization and Interaction. Massachusetts: Cambridge.
Pelo, D.K., Henderson, V.W., Murdock, G.A., McCleary, C.A., & Buckwalter, J.G. (1998). The association between naming ability and memory for visual reproduction. Paper presented at International Neuropsychological Society, Honolulu, HI.
Piguet, O., Saling, M.M., O'Shea, M.F., Berkovic, S.F., & Bladin, P.F. (1994). Rey figure distortions reflect nonverbal recall differences between right and left foci in unilateral temporal lobe epilepsy. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 9, 451460.Google Scholar
Poreh, A. & Shye, S. (1998). Examination of the global and local features of the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure using faceted smallest space analysis. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 12, 453467.Google Scholar
Robertson, L.C. & Lamb, M.R. (1991). Neuropsychological contributions to theories of part/whole organization. Cognitive Psychology, 23, 299230.Google Scholar
Sawrie, S.M., Martin, R.C., Gilliam, F., Knowlton, R., Faught, E., & Kuzniecky, R. (2001). Verbal retention lateralizes patients with unilateral temporal lobe epilepsy and bilateral hippocampal atrophy. Epilepsia, 42, 651659.Google Scholar
Schwartz, T.J., Peters, C.E., & Stein, A. (2000). Qualitative scoring of the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure proves useful in evaluating epilepsy patients. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 15, 712.Google Scholar
Seidenberg, M., Griffith, R., Sabsevitz, D., Moran, M., Haltiner, A., Bell, B., Swanson, S., Hammeke, T., & Hermann, B. (2002). Recognition and identification of famous faces in patients with unilateral temporal lobe epilepsy. Neuropsychologia, 40, 446456.Google Scholar
Sergent, J. (1982). The cerebral balance of power: Confrontation or cooperation? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8, 253272.Google Scholar
Spiers, H.J., Burgess, N., Maguire, E.A., Baxendale, S.A., Hartley, T., Thompson, P.J., & O'Keefe, J. (2001). Unilateral temporal lobectomy patients show lateralized topographical and episodic memory deficits in a virtual town. Brain, 124, 24762489.Google Scholar
Spreen, O. & Strauss, L. (1998). A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests. New York: Oxford University Press.
Stern, R.A., Singer, E.A., Duke, L.M., Singer, N.G., Morey, C.E., & Daughtrey, E.W. (1994). The Boston Qualitative Scoring System for the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure: Description and interrater reliability. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 8, 309322.Google Scholar
Troyer, A.K. & Wishart, H.A. (1997). A comparison of qualitative scoring systems for the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 11, 381390.Google Scholar
Waber, D.P. & Holmes, J.M. (1986). Assessing children's memory productions of the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 8, 563589.Google Scholar