Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T06:05:19.297Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Emotional valence and semantic relatedness differentially influence false recognition in mild cognitive impairment, Alzheimer’s disease, and healthy elderly

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 March 2009

KATJA BRUECKNER*
Affiliation:
Epilepsy Center Hamburg, Protestant Hospital Alsterdorf, Hamburg, Germany
STEFFEN MORITZ
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry & Psychotherapy, University Hospital Hamburg Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany
*
*Correspondence and reprint requests to: Katja Brueckner, Epilepsy Center Hamburg, Protestant Hospital Alsterdorf, Bodelschwinghstr. 24, 22337 Hamburg, Germany. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

This study examined whether patients with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) who are at higher risk for later Alzheimer disease (AD) display deficits comparable to patients with diagnosed dementia. We assessed 27 patients with MCI, 36 patients with AD, and 20 healthy older adults with an emotional variant of the Deese–Roediger–McDermott-paradigm. Participants studied four lists that were semantically related to a nonpresented critical theme word. These theme words were either depression-related (i.e., loneliness) or delusion-related (betrayal) or had a positive (holidays) or neutral (window) valence. Despite a normal overall emotional memory and a normal corrected overall false recognition, patients with MCI, as predicted, produced as many false memories as patients with AD. On closer examination, both patient groups showed enhanced false memories to unrelated stimuli and a significant bias to falsely remember stimuli with a positive valence. We conclude that although patients with MCI are not distinguishable from healthy older adults in terms of their overall emotional recognition, positively valenced memories and more specifically false positive memories may represent the signature of a breakdown of emotional memory along the continuum between normal aging and AD. (JINS, 2009, 15, 268–276.)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © INS 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., & Buchanan, T.W. (2005). Amygdala damage impairs emotional memory for gist but not details of complex stimuli. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 512518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Balota, D.A., Cortese, M.J., Duchek, J.M., Adams, D., Roediger, H.L., McDermott, K.B., & Yerys, B.E. (1999). Veridical and false memories in healthy older adults and in dementia of the Alzheimer’s type. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 16, 361384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blessing, A., Martin, M., Wenz, M., & Zöllig, J. (2006). Emotionen und Gedächtnis bei Patienten mit Alzheimer-Demenz. Zeitschrift für Neuropsychologie, 17, 8192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boller, F., El Massioui, F., Devouche, E., Traykov, L., Pomati, S., & Starkstein, S.E. (2002). Processing emotional information in Alzheimer’s disease: Effects on memory performance and neurophysiological correlates. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 14, 104112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bottino, C.M., Castro, C.C., Gomes, R.L., Buchpiguel, C.A., Marchetti, R.L., & Neto, M.R. (2002). Volumetric MRI measurements can differentiate Alzheimer’s disease, mild cognitive impairment, and normal aging. International Psychogeriatrics, 14, 5972.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Budson, A.E., Daffner, K.R., Desikan, R., & Schacter, D.L. (2000). When false recognition is unopposed by true recognition: Gist-based memory distortion in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology, 14, 277287.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Budson, A.E., Desikan, R., Daffner, K.R., & Schacter, D.L. (2001). Perceptual false recognition in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology, 15, 230243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Budson, A.E., Michalska, K.J., Sullivan, A.L., Rentz, D.M., Daffner, K.R., & Schacter, D.L. (2003a). False recognition in Alzheimer disease: Evidence from categorized pictures. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 16, 1627.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Budson, A.E., Sullivan, A.L., Daffner, K.R., & Schacter, D.L. (2003b). Semantic versus phonological false recognition in aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Brain and Cognition, 51, 251261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Budson, A.E., Todman, R.W., Chong, H., Adams, E.H., Kensinger, E.A., Krangel, T.S., & Wright, C.I. (2006). False recognition of emotional word lists in aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 19, 7178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Calabrese, P. (2000). Neuropsychologie der Alzheimer-Demenz. In Calabrese, P. & Förstl, H. (Eds.), Psychopathologie und Neuropsychologie der Demenzen (pp. 3150). Lengerich, Germany: Pabst.Google Scholar
de Leeuw, F.-E., Barkhof, F., & Scheltens, P. (2004). White matter lesions and hippocampal atrophy in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 62, 310312.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deese, J. (1959). On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 1722.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
den Heijer, T., Geerlings, M.I., Hoebeek, F.E., Hofman, A., Koudstaal, P.J., & Breteler, M.M. (2006). Use of hippocampal and amygdalar volumes on magnetic resonance imaging to predict dementia in cognitive intact elderly people. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63, 5762.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denburg, N.L., Buchanan, T.W., Tranel, D., & Adolphs, R. (2003). Evidence for preserved emotional memory in normal older persons. Emotion, 3, 239253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Esiri, M., Lee, V.M., & Trojanowski, J.Q. (2004). The neuropathology of dementia (2nd ed.). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fleming, K., Kim, S.H., Doo, M., Maguire, G., & Potkin, S.D. (2003). Memory for emotional stimuli in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease and Other Dementias, 18, 340342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Folstein, M.F., Folstein, S.E., & McHugh, P.R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatry Research, 12, 189198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenaway, M.C., Lacritz, L.H., Binegar, D., Weiner, M.F., Lipton, A., & Munro Cullum, C. (2006). Patterns of verbal memory performance in mild cognitive impairment, Alzheimer’s disease, and normal aging. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 19, 7984.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamann, S.B., Monarch, E.S., & Goldstein, F.C. (2000). Memory enhancement for emotional stimuli is impaired in early Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology, 14, 8292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Härting, C., Markowitsch, H.J., Neufeld, U., Calabrese, P., Deisinger, K., & Kessler, J. (2000). Wechsler Gedächtnis Test—Revidierte Fassung (WMS-R). Bern, Switzerland: Huber.Google Scholar
Jellinger, K.A. (2003). Neuropathology of Alzheimer’s disease and clinical relevance. In Iqbal, K. & Winblad, B. (Eds.), Alzheimer’s disease and related disorders: Research advances (pp. 153169). Bucharest, Romania: “Ana Aslan” International Academy of Aging.Google Scholar
Kazui, H., Mori, E., Hashimoto, M., Hirono, N., Imamura, T., Tanimukai, S., & Hanihara, T. (2000). Impact of emotion on memory. Controlled study of the influence of emotionally charged material on declarative memory in Alzheimer’s disease. British Journal of Psychiatry, 177, 343347.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kensinger, E.A., Anderson, A., Growdon, J.H., & Corkin, S. (2004). Effects of Alzheimer disease on memory for verbal emotional information. Neuropsychologia, 42, 791800.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kensinger, E.A., Brierley, B., Medford, N., Growdon, J.H., & Corkin, S. (2002). Effects of normal aging and Alzheimer’s disease on emotional memory. Emotion, 2, 118134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kensinger, E.A. & Corkin, S. (2004). The effect of emotional content and aging on false memories. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 4, 19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lehrl, S. (1995). Mehrfachwahl-Wortschatz-Intelligenz test MWT-B. Balingen, Germany: Perimed-Spitta.Google Scholar
Medford, N., Phillips, M.L., Brierley, B., Brammer, M., Bullmore, E.T., & David, A.S. (2005). Emotional memory: Separating content and context. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 138, 247258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mega, M.S., Small, G.W., Xu, M.L., Felix, J., Manese, M., Tran, N.P., Dailey, J.I., Ercoli, L.M., Bookheimer, S.Y., & Toga, A.W. (2002). Hippocampal atrophy in persons with age-associated memory impairment: Volumetry within a common space. Psychosomatic Medicine, 64, 487492.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mizuno, K., Wakai, M., Takeda, A., & Sobue, G. (2000). Medial temporal atrophy and memory impairment in early stage of Alzheimer’s disease: An MRI volumetric and memory assessment study. Journal of Neurological Sciences, 172, 1824.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moayeri, S.E., Cahill, L., Jin, Y., & Potkin, S.G. (2000). Relative sparing of emotionally influenced memory in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroreport, 11, 653655.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mori, E., Ikeda, M., Hirono, N., Kitagaki, H., Imamura, T., & Shimomura, T. (1999). Amygdalar volume and emotional memory in Alzheimer’s disease. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 216222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moritz, S., Glascher, J., & Brassen, S. (2005). Investigation of mood-congruent false and true memory recognition in depression. Depression and Anxiety, 21, 917.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ochsner, K.N. (2000). Are affective events richly “remembered” or simply familiar? The experience and process of recognizing feelings past. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 129, 242261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, R.C., Parisi, J.E., & Dickson, D.W. (2000). Neuropathologic substrate of mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiology of Aging, 21, 198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petersen, R.C., Stevens, J.C., Ganguli, M., Tangalos, E.G., Cummings, J.L., & DeKosky, S.T. (2001). Practice parameter: Early detection of dementia: Mild cognitive impairment (an evidence-based review). Report of the Quality Standards Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology. Neurology, 56, 11331142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pierce, B.H., Sullivan, A.L., Schacter, D.L., & Budson, A.E. (2005). Comparing source-based and gist-based false recognition in aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology, 19, 411419.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Price, J.L. & Morris, J.C. (1999). Tangles and plaques in nondemented aging and “preclinical” Alzheimer’s disease. Annals of Neurology, 45, 358368.3.0.CO;2-X>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reitan, R.M. (1992). Trail Making Test. Manual for administration and scoring. South Tucson, AZ: Reitan Neuropsychology Laboratory.Google Scholar
Roediger, H.L. & McDermott, K.B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 21, 803814.Google Scholar
Schacter, D.L., Verfaellie, M., Anes, M.D., & Racine, C. (1998). When true recognition suppresses false recognition: Evidence from amnesic patients. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 668679.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schreiber, Y.A., Ackl, N., Sonntag, A., & Zihl, J. (2005). Charakterisierung kognitiver Einbußen von Patienten mit “Mild Cognitive Impairment” (MCI) in der CERAD-Screeningbatteries. Zeitschrift für Neuropsychologie, 16, 139149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sheikh, J.I. & Yesavage, J.A. (1986). Geriatric depression scale (GDS): Recent evidence and development of a shorter version. Clinical Gerontologist, 5, 165173.Google Scholar
Thalmann, B., Monsch, A.U., Schneitter, M., Bernasconi, F., Aebi, C., Camachova-Davet, Z., & Staehelin, H.B. (2000). The CERAD neuropsychological assessment battery (CERAD-NAB)—A minimal dataset as a common tool for German-speaking Europe. Neurobiology of Aging, 21, 30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, J.M., Balota, D.A., & Sergent-Marshall, S.D. (2001). Semantic, phonological, and hybrid veridical and false memories in healthy adults and dementia of the Alzheimer’s type. Neuropsychology, 15, 254267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Welsh, K., Butters, N., Hughes, J., Mohs, R., & Heymann, A. (1991). Detection of abnormal memory decline in mild cases of Alzheimer’s disease using CERAD neuropsychological measures. Archives of Neurology, 49, 278281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
World Health Organization (Ed.). (1993). The ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioural disorders diagnostic criteria for research. Bern, Switzerland: Huber.Google Scholar
World Health Organization (Ed.). (2000). Internationale Klassifikation psychischer Störungen: ICD-10. Kapitel V (F). Klinisch-diagnostische Leitlinien. Bern, Switzerland: Huber.Google Scholar