Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T04:17:13.811Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Effect of Preschool Working Memory, Language, and Narrative Abilities on Inferential Comprehension at School-Age in Children with Spina Bifida Myelomeningocele and Typically Developing Children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 February 2013

Meredith Pike
Affiliation:
Deparment of Psychology, University of Guelph, Guelph, Ontario
Paul Swank
Affiliation:
Children's Learning Institute, Department of Pediatrics, University of Texas Health-Science Center-Houston, Houston, Texas
Heather Taylor
Affiliation:
Children's Learning Institute, Department of Pediatrics, University of Texas Health-Science Center-Houston, Houston, Texas
Susan Landry
Affiliation:
Children's Learning Institute, Department of Pediatrics, University of Texas Health-Science Center-Houston, Houston, Texas
Marcia A. Barnes*
Affiliation:
Children's Learning Institute, Department of Pediatrics, University of Texas Health-Science Center-Houston, Houston, Texas
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Marcia A. Barnes, The University of Texas Medical School at Houston, Department of Pediatrics, 7000 Fannin, Suite 2431, Houston, TX 77030. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Children with spina bifida myelomeningocele (SBM) are more likely to display a pattern of good-decoding/poor comprehension than their neurologically intact peers. The goals of the current study were to (1) examine the cognitive origins of one of the component skills of comprehension, bridging inferences, from a developmental perspective and (2) to test the effects of those relations on reading comprehension achievement. Data from a sample of children with SBM and a control group (n = 78) who participated in a longitudinal study were taken from age 36-month and 9.5-year time points. A multiple mediation model provided evidence that three preschool cognitive abilities (working memory/inhibitory control, oral comprehension, narrative recall), could partially explain the relation between group and bridging inference skill. A second mediation model supported that each of the 36-month abilities had an indirect effect on reading comprehension through bridging inference skill. Findings contribute to an understanding of both typical and atypical comprehension development, blending theories from the developmental, cognitive, and neuropsychological literature. (JINS, 2013, 19, 1–10)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2013

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Barnes, M.A., Dennis, M. (1992). Reading in children and adolescents after early onset hydrocephalus and in normally developing age peers: Phonological analysis, word recognition, word comprehension, and passage comprehension skills. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 17, 445465. doi:10.1093/jpepsy/17.4.445CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnes, M.A., Dennis, M. (1998). Discourse after early-onset hydrocephalus: Core deficits in children of average intelligence. Brain and Language, 61, 309334. doi:10.1006/brln.1998.1843CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnes, M.A., Dennis, M., Hetherington, R. (2004). Reading and writing skills in young adults with spina bifida and hydrocephalus. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10, 655663. doi:10.1017/S1355617704105055CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnes, M.A., Faulkner, H., Wilkinson, M., Dennis, M. (2004). Meaning construction and integration in children with hydrocephalus. Brain and Language, 89, 4756. doi:10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00295-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnes, M.A., Huber, J., Johnston, A.M., Dennis, M. (2007). A model of comprehension in spina bifida meningomyelocele: Meaning activation, integration, and revision. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13, 854864. doi:10.1037/0708-5591.49.2.125CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron, R.M., Kenny, D.A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 11731182. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cain, K., Oakhill, J. (Eds.) (2007). Children's Comprehension Problems in Oral and Written Language. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Cain, K., Oakhill, J., Bryant, P. (2004). Children's reading comprehension ability: Concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal ability, and component skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96(1), 3142. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.96.1.31CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Catts, H., Adlof, S., Ellis-Weismer, S. (2006). Language deficits in poor comprehenders: A case for the simple view of reading. Journal of Speech-Language-Hearing Research, 49, 278293. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2006/023)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Catts, H., Fey, M., Zhang, X., Tomblin, J.B. (1999). Language basis of reading and reading disabilities: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3, 331361. doi:10.1207/s1532799xssr0304_2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clifton, C.J., Duffy, S.A. (2001). Sentence and text comprehension: Roles of linguistic structure. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 167196. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.167CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christopher, M.E., Miyake, A., Keenan, J.M., Pennington, B., DeFries, J.C., Olson, R. (2012). Predicting word reading and comprehension with executive function and speed measures across development: A latent variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 16. doi:10.1037/a0027375Google ScholarPubMed
Davis, B.J., Johnston, A.M., Barnes, M.A., Desrochers, A. (2007). Bridging inferences in children from grades three to eight. Poster presented at annual convention of Canadian Psychological Association, Ottawa, Ontario.Google Scholar
Dearing, E., Hamilton, L.C. (2006). Contemporary advances and classic advice for analyzing mediating and moderating variables. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 71, 88104. doi:10.1111/j.1540-5834.2006.00406.xGoogle Scholar
De Beni, R., Palladino, P. (2000). Intrusion errors in working memory tasks: Are they related to reading comprehension ability? Learning and Individual Differences, 12(2), 131143. doi:10.1016/S1041-6080(01)00033-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Beni, R., Palladino, P., Pazzaglia, F., Cornoldi, C. (1998). Increases in intrusion errors and working memory deficit of poor comprehenders The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51, 305320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dennis, M., Jacennik, B., Barnes, M.A. (1994). The content of narrative discourse in children and adolescents after early-onset hydrocephalus and in normally developing age peers. Brain and Language, 46, 129165. doi:10.1006/brln.1994.1008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dennis, M., Landry, S.H., Barnes, M., Fletcher, J.M. (2006). A model of neurocognitive function in spina bifida over the life span. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12, 285296. doi:10.1017/S1355617706060371CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diamond, A., Prevor, M.B., Callendar, G., Druin, D.P. (1997). Prefrontal cognitive deficits in children treated early and continuously for PKU. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 62, iiv. doi:10.2307/1166208CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ewing-Cobbs, L., Prasad, M.R., Landry, S.H., Kramer, L., DeLeon, R. (2004). Executive functions following traumatic brain injury in young children: A preliminary analysis. Developmental Neuropsychology, 26, 487512. doi:10.1207/s15326942dn2601_7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fletcher, J.M., Dennis, M., Northup, H., Barnes, M.A., Hannay, H.J., Landry, S.H., Francis, D.J. (2004). Spina bifida: Genes, brain, and development. In L.M. Glidden (Ed.), Handbook of research on mental retardation (Vol. 28, pp. 63117). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. doi:10.1016/S0074-7750(04)29003-6Google Scholar
Gernsbacher, M.A. (1990). Language comprehension as structure building. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gernsbacher, M.A. (1993). Less skilled readers have less efficient suppression mechanisms. Psychological Science, 4, 294298. doi:10.1111/j.1467- 9280.1993.tb00567.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gernsbacher, M.A., Faust, M.E. (1991). The mechanism of suppression: A component of general comprehension skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 245262. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.17.2.245Google ScholarPubMed
Hayes, A.F. (2009). Beyond Baron and Kenny: Statistical mediation analysis in the new millennium. Communication Monographs, 76, 408420. doi:10.1080/03637750903310360CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, A. F. (2011). Macros and code for SPSS and SAS. Retrieved from http://www.afhayes.com/spss-sas-and-mplus-macros-and-code.htmlGoogle Scholar
Hulme, C., Snowling, M.J. (2011). Children's reading comprehension difficulties: Nature, causes, and treatments. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 139142. doi:10.1177/0963721411408673CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendeou, P., Van den Broek, P., White, M.J., Lynch, J.S. (2009). Predicting reading comprehension in early elementary school: The independent contributions of oral language and decoding skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 765778. doi:10.1037/a0015956CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kintsch, W. (1988). The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: A construction-integration model. Psychological Review, 95, 163182. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.95.2.163CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kintsch, W. (1994). Text comprehension, memory, and learning. American Psychologist, 49, 294303. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.49.4.294CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kintsch, W.A. (2005). An overview of top-down and bottom-up effects in comprehension: The CI perspective. Discourse Processes, 39, 125128. doi:10.1207/s15326950dp3902&3_2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lomax-Bream, L.E., Barnes, M.A., Copeland, K., Taylor, H.B., Landry, S.H. (2007). The impact of spina bifida on development across the first three years. Developmental Neuropsychology, 31, 120. doi:10.1207/s15326942dn3101_1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lynch, J.S., van den Broek, P., Kremer, K., Kendeou, P., White, M.J., Lorch, E.P. (2008). The development of narrative comprehension and its relation to other early reading skills. Reading Psychology, 29, 327365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacKinnon, D.P., Krull, J.L., Lockwood, C.M. (2000). Equivalence of the mediation, confounding and suppression effect. Prevention Science, 1, 173181. doi:10.1023/A:1026595011371CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miyake, A., Friedman, N.P., Emerson, M.J., Witzki, A.H., Howerter, A., Wager, T.D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex frontal lobe tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49100. doi:10.1006/cogp.1999.0734CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nation, K., Clarke, P., Marshall, C.M., Durand, M. (2004). Hidden language impairments in children: Parallels between poor reading comprehension and specific language impairment? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 199211. doi:10.1044/1092-4388CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oakhill, J., Cain, K. (2007). Introduction to comprehension development. In K. Cain & J. Oakhill (Eds.), Children's comprehension problems in oral and written language. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Palladino, P., Cornoldi, C., De Beni, R., Pazzaglia, F. (2001). Working memory and updating processes in reading comprehension. Memory and Cognition, 29, 344354. doi:10.3758/BF03194929CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petrides, M., Milner, B. (1982). Deficits on subject-ordered tasks after frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia, 20, 249262. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(82)90100-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pike, M., Barnes, M., Barron, R.W. (2010). The role of illustrations in children's inferential comprehension. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105, 243255. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2009.10.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Preacher, K.J., Hayes, A.F. (2008). Contemporary approaches to assessing mediation in communication research. In A.F. Hayes, M.D. Slater, & L.B. Snyder (Eds.), The Sage sourcebook of advanced data analysis methods for communication research (pp. 1354). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roth, F.P., Speece, D.L., Cooper, D.H. (2002). A longitudinal analysis of the connection between oral language and early reading. The Journal of Educational Research, 95, 259272. doi:10.1080/00220670209596600CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shrout, P., Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: New procedures and recommendations. Psychological Methods, 7, 422445. doi:10.1037/1082-989X.7.4.422CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sobel, M.E. (1982). Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect effects in structural equation models. In S. Leinhardt (Ed.), Sociological methodology, pp. 290312. Washington, DC: American Sociological Association.Google Scholar
Storch, S.A., Whitehurst, G.J. (2002). Oral language and code-related precursors to reading: Evidence from a longitudinal structural model. Developmental Psychology, 38, 934947. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.38.6.934CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, H.L., Howard, C., Sáez, L. (2006). Do different components of working memory underlie different subgroups of reading disabilities? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 39, 252269. doi:10.1177/00222194060390030501CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, H.L., Howard, C.B., Sáez, L. (2007). Reading comprehension and working memory in children with learning disabilities in reading. In K. Cain & J. Oakhill (Eds.), Children's comprehension problems in oral and written language (pp. 157190). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Tannenbaum, K.R., Torgesen, J.K., Wagner, R.K. (2006). Relationships between word knowledge and reading comprehension in third-grade children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10, 381398. doi:10.1207/s1532799xssr1004_3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van den Broek, P. (1997). Discovering the cements of the universe: The development of event comprehension from childhood to adulthood. In P. van den Broek, P. Bauer, & T. Bourg (Eds.), Developmental spans in event comprehension: Bridging fictional and actual events (pp. 321342). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
van den Broek, P.W., White, M.J., Kendeou, P., Carlson, S. (2009). Reading between the lines. Developmental and individual differences in cognitive processes in reading comprehension. In: Wagner, R.K., Schatschneider, C., Phythian-Sence, C. (Eds.), Beyond decoding. The behavioral and biological foundations of reading comprehension, pp. 107123. New York: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Woodcock, R.W., McGrew, K.S., Mather, N. (2001). Woodcock-Johnson III. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.Google Scholar
Zimmerman, I.L., Steiner, V.G., Pond, R.E. (2002). Preschool Language Scale (4th ed.). San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment, Inc.Google Scholar