Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gvvz8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-29T01:20:01.789Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The roles of language use and vocabulary size in the emergence of word-combining in children with complex neurodevelopmental disabilities

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 May 2020

Susan FOSTER-COHEN*
Affiliation:
University of Canterbury, New Zealand Institute of Language, Brain and Behaviour The Champion Centre, Christchurch, New Zealand
Anne van BYSTERVELDT
Affiliation:
University of Canterbury, New Zealand Institute of Language, Brain and Behaviour
Viktoria PAPP
Affiliation:
University of Canterbury, New Zealand Institute of Language, Brain and Behaviour
*
Address for correspondence: Susan Foster-Cohen, New Zealand Institute of Language, Brain and Behaviour, University of Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand. Email: [email protected]

Abstract

Parent report data on 82 preschool children with complex neurodevelopmental disabilities including Down syndrome, dyspraxia, autism, and global developmental delay suggests communicative language use must reach a threshold level before vocabulary size becomes the best predictor of word combining. Using the Language Use Inventory and the MacArthur-Bates CDI (with sign vocabulary option), statistical modelling using regression trees and random forests suggests that, despite high linear correlations between variables, (1) pragmatic ability, particularly children's emerging ability to talk about things, themselves and others is a significantly better predictor of the earliest word combining than vocabulary size; and (2) vocabulary size becomes a better predictor of later word combining, once this pragmatic base has been established.

Type
Brief Research Report
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

We are grateful to the parents who made this research possible; to the staff of The Champion Centre who understood its importance; and to the action editor and reviewers at the Journal of Child Language for incisive feedback that improved the quality of the work.

References

Bates, E., Bretherton, I., & Snyder, L. (1988). From first words to grammar. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bates, E., & Goodman, J. C. (1997). On the inseparability of grammar and the lexicon: Evidence from acquisition, aphasia and real-time processing. Language and cognitive Processes, 12(5-6), 507584.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. (2000). The intentionality model of word learning: How to learn a word, any word. In Golinkoff, R., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Bloom, L., Smith, L. B., Woodward, A. L., Akhtar, N., Tomasello, M. & Hollich, G. (Eds.), Becoming a word learner: A debate on lexical acquisition (pp. 1950). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braginsky, M., Yurovsky, D., Marchman, V. A, & Frank, M. C. (2015). Developmental Changes in the Relationship Between Grammar and the Lexicon. Paper presented at the CogSci. 2015. langcog.stanford.eduGoogle Scholar
Buckley, S. (1993). Language development in children with Down syndrome-Reasons for optimism. Down syndrome research and practice, 1(1), 39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buckley, S. (2003). Literacy and Language. In Rondal, J. A. & Buckley, S. (Ed.), Speech and Language Intervention in Down Syndrome (pp. 132153). London: Whurr Publishers Ltd.Google Scholar
Crais, E. (2001). Developmental Milestones for Speech, Language and Communication. In Roush, J. (Ed.), Screening for Hearing Loss and Otitis Media in Children (p. 96). San Diego, CA. : Singular.Google Scholar
Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A, Thal, D. J., Dale, P. S., & Reznick, J. S. (2007). MacArthur-Bates communicative development inventories: User's guide and technical manual: Baltimore, MD: Brookes.Google Scholar
Foster-Cohen, S.H., & van Bysterveldt, A. K. (2016). Assessing the communication development of children with language delay through parent multi-questionnaire reporting. Speech, Language and Hearing, 1-8. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/2050571X.2015.1108067CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harrison, P.L., & Oakland, T. (2000) ABAS, adaptive behavior assessment system : manual. San Antonio, Tex.: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Haynes, W. O, Moran, M. J, & Pindzola, R. H. (2006). Communication disorders in the classroom: An introduction for professionals in school settings: Jones & Bartlett Learning.Google Scholar
Hothorn, T., Hornik, K., & Zeileis, A. (2006). Unbiased recursive partitioning: A conditional inference framework. Journal of Computational and Graphical statistics, 15(3), 651674.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marchman, V. A, & Bates, E. (1994). Continuity in lexical and morphological development: A test of the critical mass hypothesis. Journal of child language, 21(2), 339366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moyle, M. J., Weismer, S. E., Evans, J. L., & Lindstrom, M. J. (2007). Longitudinal relationships between lexical and grammatical development in typical and late-talking children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50(2), 508528.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newport, E. L. (1990). Maturational constraints on language learning. Cognitive science, 14(1), 1128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Neill, D. K. (2009). Language Use Inventory, An assessment of young children's pragmatic language development for 18- to 47-month-old children [manual]. Waterloo, Ontario, Canada: Knwoledge in Development.Google Scholar
Paterson, S. J, Parish-Morris, J., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2016). Considering development in developmental disorders. Journal of Cognition and Development, 17(4), 568583.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patterson, J. L. (1998). Expressive vocabulary development and word combinations of Spanish-English bilingual toddlers. American journal of speech-language pathology, 7(4), 4656.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paul, R., & Norbury, C. (2012). Language disorders from infancy through adolescence: Listening, speaking, reading, writing, and communicating: Elsevier Health Sciences.Google Scholar
Pesco, D., & O'Neill, D. K. (2012). Predicting later language outcomes from the Language Use Inventory. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55(2), 421434.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peters, A. M. (1977). Language learning strategies: does the whole equal the sum of the parts? Language, 560573.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
R Core Team. (2014). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. 2013.Google Scholar
Ramírez, N. F., Lieberman, A. M., & Mayberry, R. I. (2013). The initial stages of first-language acquisition begun in adolescence: When late looks early. Journal of child language, 40(2), 391414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reese, E., & Read, S. (2000). Predictive validity of the New Zealand MacArthur communicative development inventory: Words and sentences. Journal of Child Language, 27(02), 255266.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shore, C. M. (1994). Individual differences in language development (Vol. 7): Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Singer Harris, N. G, Bellugi, U., Bates, E., Jones, W., & Rossen, M. (1997). Contrasting profiles of language development in children with Williams and Down syndromes. Developmental Neuropsychology, 13(3), 345370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stolt, S., Matomäki, J., Haataja, L., Lapinleimu, H., Lehtonen, L., & Group, PIPARI Study. (2013). The emergence of grammar in very-low-birth-weight Finnish children at two years of age. Journal of child language, 40(2), 336357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thal, D., & Bates, E. (1990). Continuity and variation in early language development. Individual differences in infancy: Reliability, stability, prediction, 359-383.Google Scholar
Tomaschek, F., Hendrix, P., & Baayen, R. H. (2018). Strategies for addressing collinearity in multivariate linguistic data. Journal of Phonetics, 71, 249267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language: A usage-based approach to child language acquisition. Cambridge (MA).Google Scholar