Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T17:08:26.128Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The role of prior language context on bilingual spoken word processing: Evidence from the visual world task*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 March 2015

JULIE MERCIER
Affiliation:
McGill University, Montreal
IRINA PIVNEVA
Affiliation:
McGill University, Montreal
DEBRA TITONE*
Affiliation:
McGill University, Montreal
*
Address for correspondence: Debra Titone, Department of Psychology, McGill University, 1205 Dr. Penfield Avenue, Montreal, QC, H3A 1B1, Canada[email protected]

Abstract

We investigated whether speaking in one language affects cross- and within-language activation when subsequently switching to a task performed in the same or different language. English–French bilinguals (L1 English, n = 29; L1 French, n = 28) were randomly assigned to a prior language context condition consisting of a spontaneous production task in English (the no-switch group) or in French (the switch group). Participants then performed an English spoken language comprehension task using the visual world method. The key result was that the switch group showed less evidence of cross-language competition than the no-switch group, consistent with the notion of an active inhibition of a prior language in the switch group. These data suggest that proficient bilinguals can globally suppress a non-target language, whether it is L1 or L2, though doing so requires cognitive resources that may be diverted from other demands, such as controlling within-language competition.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

This research was supported by a Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC) Discovery Award. The authors gratefully acknowledge additional support from a Canada Graduate Scholarships (CGS) Doctoral Award from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR) (Mercier), a Frederick Banting and Charles Best CGS, Master's Award from the CIHR (Pivneva), FQRNT doctoral award (Pivneva), an NSERC award (Pivneva), and the Canada Research Chairs Program (Titone). The authors are also grateful to Stacey Wilson for her help with recruitment and testing. We gratefully acknowledge the anonymous reviewers who provided important feedback that helped us to revise a previous version of this manuscript.

References

Abutalebi, J., Annoni, J. M., Seghier, M., Zimine, I., Lee-Jahnke, H., Lazeyras, F., Cappa, S. F., & Khateb, A. (2008). Language control and lexical competition in bilinguals: An event-related fMRI study. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 14961505.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abutalebi, J., & Green, D. W. (2007). Bilingual language production: The neurocognition of language representation and control. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 20, 242275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allopenna, P. D., Magnuson, J. S., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1998). Tracking the time course of spoken word recognition using eye movements: Evidence for continuous mapping models. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 419439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Altmann, G. T. M. (2011). Language can mediate eye movement control within 100 milliseconds, regardless of whether there is anything to move the eyes to. Acta Psychologica, 137, 190200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Altmann, G. T. M., & Kamide, Y. (1999). Incremental interpretation at verbs: Restricting the domain of subsequent reference. Cognition, 73, 247264.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Altmann, G. T. M., & Kamide, Y. (2004). Now you see it, now you don’t: Mediating the mapping between language and the visual world. In Henderson, J. M. & Ferreira, F. (eds.), The interface of language, vision, and action: Eye movements and the visual world, pp. 347386. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Anderson, A. H., Bader, M., Bard, E. G., Boyle, E. H., Doherty, G. M., Garrod, S. C., Isard, S. D., Kowtko, J. C., McAllister, J. M., Miller, J., Sotillo, C. F., Thompson, H. S., & Weinert, R. (1991). The HCRC map task corpus. Language and Speech, 34, 351366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. H. (2008). Analyzing linguistic data: A practical introduction to statistics using R. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 390412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. H., Piepenbrock, R., & Van Rijn, H. (1995). The CELEX lexical database (CD-ROM). Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Linguistic Data Consortium.Google Scholar
Barr, D. J. (2008). Analyzing ‘visual world’ eyetracking data using multilevel logistic regression. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 457474.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, D. M. (2007). Linear mixed model implementation in lme4. Ms., University of Wisconsin–Madison.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2009). Bilingualism: The good, the bad, and the indifferent. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12, 311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Green, D. W., & Gollan, T. H. (2009). Bilingual minds. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 10, 89129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Luk, G. (2008). Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 859873.Google ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Luk, G. (2012). Bilingualism: Consequences for mind and brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 240250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Ryan, J. (2006). Executive control in a modified antisaccade task: Effects of aging and bilingualism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 13411354.Google Scholar
Blumenfeld, H. K., & Marian, V. (2007). Constraints on parallel activation in bilingual spoken language processing: Examining proficiency and lexical status using eye-tracking. Language and Cognitive Processes, 22, 633660.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blumenfeld, H. K., & Marian, V. (2011). Bilingualism influences inhibitory control in auditory comprehension. Cognition, 118, 245257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Braver, T. S. (2012). The variable nature of cognitive control: A dual-mechanisms framework. Trends in Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 106113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, E. K., & Miller, J. E. (1980). The syntax of Scottish English. Final Report to SSRC (UK), Project No. 5152, London.Google Scholar
Canseco-Gonzalez, E., Brehm, L., Brick, C. A., Brown-Schmidt, S., Fischer, K., & Wagner, K. (2010). Carpet or cárcel: The effect of age of acquisition and language mode on bilingual lexical access. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25, 669705.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooper, R. M. (1974). The control of eye fixation by the meaning of spoken language: A new methodology for the real-time investigation of speech perception, memory, and language processing. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 84107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Hernández, M., Costa-Faidella, J., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2009). On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition, 113, 135149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costa, A., Hernández, M., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2008). Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task. Cognition, 106, 5986.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costa, A., & Santesteban, M. (2004). Lexical access in bilingual speech production: Evidence from language switching in highly proficient bilinguals and L2 learners. Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 491511.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Santesteban, M., & Ivanova, I. (2006). How do highly proficient bilinguals control their lexicalization process? Inhibitory and language-specific selection mechanisms are both functional. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 10571074.Google ScholarPubMed
Cutler, A., Weber, A., & Otake, T. (2006). Asymmetric mapping from phonetic to lexical representations in second-language listening. Journal of Phonetics, 34, 269284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dahan, D., & Gaskell, G. (2007). The temporal dynamics of ambiguity resolution: Evidence from spoken-word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 483501.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dahan, D., Magnuson, J. S., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2001). Time-course of frequency effects in spoken-word recognition: Evidence from eye movements. Cognitive Psychology, 42, 317367.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dahan, D., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2004). Continuous mapping from sound to meaning in spoken-language comprehension: Immediate effects of verb-based thematic constraints. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 498513.Google ScholarPubMed
De Groot, A. M. B., & Christoffels, I. K. (2006). Language control in bilinguals: Monolingual tasks and simultaneous interpreting. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 9, 189201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Groot, A. M. B., & Nas, G. L. (1991). Lexical representation of cognates and noncognates in compound bilinguals. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 90123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dijkstra, T., De Bruijn, E., Schriefers, H., & Brinke, S. T. (2000). More on interlingual homograph recognition: Language intermixing versus explicitness of instruction. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 3, 6978.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duyck, W., Van Assche, E., Drieghe, D., & Hartsuiker, R. (2007). Visual word recognition by bilinguals in a sentence context: Evidence for nonselective lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 663679.Google Scholar
Elston-Güttler, K. E., & Gunter, T. C. (2008). Fine-tuned: Phonology and semantics affect first- to second-language zooming in. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 180196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elston-Güttler, K. E., Gunter, T. C., & Kotz, S. A. (2005). Zooming into L2: Global language context and adjustment affect processing of interlingual homographs in sentences. Cognitive Brain Research, 25, 5770.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischer, B. (1992). Saccadic reaction time: Implications for reading, dyslexia and visual cognition. In Rayner (ed.), pp. 31–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
FitzPatrick, I., & Indefrey, P. (2010). Lexical competition in nonnative speech comprehension. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 1165–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerfen, C., Tam, J., McClain, R. R., Linck, J. A., & Kroll, J. F. (2015). Evidence for inhibition in native language production during immersion in the second language. Ms., Pensylvania State University.Google Scholar
Gollan, T. H., & Ferreira, V. S. (2007). Natural language switching: Expanding the role of inhibitory control. Poster presented at the 48th Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society.Google Scholar
Grainger, J., & Beauvillain, C. (1987). Language blocking and lexical access in bilinguals. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 39, 295319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, D. W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, 6781.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, D. W. (2011). Language control in different contexts: The behavioral ecology of bilingual speakers. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00103. Published online by Frontiers, May 19, 2011.Google Scholar
Grosjean, F. (2001). The bilingual's language modes. In Nicol, J. L. (ed.), One mind, two languages: Bilingual language processing, pp. 122. Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Grosjean, F. (2008). Studying bilinguals. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guo, T., Liu, H., Misra, M., & Kroll, J. F. (2011). Local and global inhibition in bilingual word production: fMRI evidence from Chinese–English bilinguals. NeuroImage, 56, 23002309.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hallett, P. E. (1978). Primary and secondary saccades to goals defined by instructions. Vision Research, 18, 12791296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hanulová, J., Davidson, D. J., & Indefrey, P. (2011). Where does the delay in L2 picture naming come from? Psycholinguistic and neurocognitive evidence on second language word production. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26, 902934.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henderson, J. M. (1992). Visual attention and eye movement control during reading and scene perception. In Rayner (ed.), pp. 260–283.Google Scholar
Henderson, J. M., Pollatsek, A., & Rayner, K. (1989). Covert visual attention and extrafoveal information use during object identification. Perception & Psychophysics, 45, 196208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hilchey, M. D., & Klein, R. M. (2011). Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 18, 625658.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huettig, F., Rommers, J., & Meyer, A. S. (2011). Using the visual world paradigm to study language processing: A review and critical evaluation. Acta Psychologica, 137, 151171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackson, G. M., Swainson, R., Cunnington, R., & Jackson, S. R. (2001). ERP correlates of executive control during repeated language switching. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 4, 169178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ju, M., & Luce, P. A. (2004). Falling on sensitive ears: Constraints on bilingual lexical activation. Psychological Science, 15, 314318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koch, I., Gade, M., Schuch, S., & Philipp, A. (2010). The role of inhibition in task switching: A review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kovelman, I., Baker, S. A., & Petitto, L. A. (2008). Bilingual and monolingual brains compared: A functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of syntactic processing and a possible “neural signature” of bilingualism. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 153169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F. (2008). Juggling two languages in one mind. Psychological Science Agenda: Science Briefs (American Psychological Association) 22, http://www.apa.org/science/about/psa/2008/01/kroll.aspx.Google Scholar
Kroll, J. F., & Gollan, T. H. (2014). Speech planning in two languages: What bilinguals tell us about language production. In Ferreira, V., Goldrick, M. & Miozzo, M. (eds.), The Oxford handbook of language production, pp. 165181. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Levy, B. J., McVeigh, N. D., Marful, A., & Anderson, M. C. (2007). Inhibiting your native language: The role of retrieval-induced forgetting during second language acquisition. Psychological Science, 18, 2934.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Libben, M. R., & Titone, D. A. (2009). Bilingual lexical access in context: Evidence from eye movements during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 381390.Google ScholarPubMed
Linck, J. A., Kroll, J. F., & Sunderman, G. (2009). Losing access to the native language while immersed in a second language: Evidence for the role of inhibition in second language learning. Psychological Science, 20, 15071515.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, X., Banich, M. T., Jacobson, B. L., & Tanabe, J. L. (2004). Common and distinct neural substrates of attentional control in an integrated Simon and spatial Stroop task as assessed by event-related fMRI. NeuroImage, 22, 10971106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luce, P., & Pisoni, D. (1998). Recognizing spoken words: The neighborhood activation model. Ear and Hearing, 19, 136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Macafee, C. (1983). Glasgow. Amsterdam & Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macaulay, R. (1985). The narrative skills of a Scottish coal miner. Amsterdam & Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Magnuson, J. S., Dixon, J. A., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Aslin, R. N. (2007). The dynamics of lexical competition during spoken word recognition. Cognitive Science, 31, 133156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marian, V., Blumenfeld, K. H., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2007). The Language Proficiency and Experience Questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing language profiles in bilinguals and multilinguals. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 940967.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marian, V., & Spivey, M. J. (2003a). Competing activation in bilingual language processing: Within- and between-language competition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 6, 97115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marian, V., & Spivey, M. J. (2003b). Comparing bilingual and monolingual processing of competing lexical items. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 173193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marian, V., Spivey, M. J., & Hirsh, J. (2003). Shared and separate systems in bilingual language processing: Converging evidence. Brain and Language, 86, 7082.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marslen-Wilson, W. D., & Welsh, A. (1978). Processing interactions and lexical access during word recognition in continuous speech. Cognitive Psychology, 10, 2963.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matin, E., Shao, K., & Boff, K. (1993). Saccadic overhead: Information processing time with and without saccades. Perception & Psychophysics, 53, 372380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McClelland, J. L., & Elman, J. L. (1986). The TRACE model of speech perception. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McQueen, J. M., & Cutler, A. (2001). Spoken word access processes: An introduction. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16, 469490.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mercier, J., Pivneva, I., & Titone, D. (2014). Individual differences in inhibitory control relate to bilingual spoken word processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17, 89117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meuter, R. F. I., & Allport, A. (1999). Bilingual language switching in naming: Asymmetrical costs of language selection. Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 2540.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mishra, R. K., Hilchey, M. D., Singh, N., & Klein, R. M. (2012). On the time course of exogenous orienting in bilinguals: Higher proficiency in a second language is associated with more rapid attentional disengagement. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 15021510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Misra, M., Guo, T., Bobb, S. C., & Kroll, J. F. (2012). When bilinguals choose a single word to speak: Electrophysiological evidence for global inhibition in bilingual word production. Journal of Memory and Language, 67, 224237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
New, B., Pallier, C., Brysbaert, M., Ferrand, L. (2004). Lexique 2: A new French lexical database. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 516524.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norris, D. (1994). Shortlist: A connectionist model of continuous speech recognition. Cognition, 52, 189234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paap, K. R., & Greenberg, Z. I. (2013). There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66, 232258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Philipp, A., Gade, M., & Koch, I. (2007). Inhibitory processes in language switching: Evidence from switching language-defined response sets. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19, 395416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phillips, N. A., Klein, D., Mercier, J., & De Boysson, C. (2006). ERP measures of auditory word repetition and translation priming in bilinguals. Brain Research, 1125, 116131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Philipp, A., & Koch, I. (2009). Inhibition in language switching: What is inhibited when switching between languages in naming tasks? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 11871195.Google ScholarPubMed
Pivneva, I., Mercier, J., & Titone, D. (2014). Inhibitory control modulates cross-language lexical activation during L2 reading: Evidence from eye movements. Ms., McGill University.Google Scholar
Pivneva, I., Palmer, C., & Titone, D. (2012). Inhibitory control and L2 proficiency modulate bilingual language production: Evidence from spontaneous monologue and dialogue speech. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pivneva, I., & Titone, D. (2014). What the eyes tell us about bilingual language production and its link to inhibitory control. Ms., McGill University.Google Scholar
R Development Core Team (2012). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. [http://www.R-project.org/].Google Scholar
Rayner, K. (ed.) (1992). Eye movements and visual cognition: Scene perception and reading. New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rodriguez-Fornells, A., Rotte, M., Heinze, H. J., Nösselt, T., & Münte, T. F. (2002). Brain potential and functional MRI evidence for how to handle two languages with one brain. Nature, 415, 10261029.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Runnqvist, E., Strijkers, K., Sadat, J., & Costa, A. (2011). On the temporal and functional origin of L2 disadvantages in speech production: A critical review. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saslow, M. G. (1967). Latency for saccadic eye movement. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 57, 10301033.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shook, A., & Marian, V. (2012). Bimodal bilinguals co-activate both languages during spoken comprehension. Cognition, 124, 314324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simon, J. R., & Rudell, A. P. (1967). Auditory S–R compatibility: The effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. Journal of Applied Psychology, 51, 300304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spivey, M. J., & Marian, V. (1999). Cross talk between native and second languages: Partial activation of an irrelevant lexicon. Psychological Science, 10, 281284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 18, 643662.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tanenhaus, M. K. (2007). Spoken language comprehension: Insights from eye movements. In Gaskell, M. G., (ed.), Oxford handbook of psycholinguistics, pp. 309326. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tanenhaus, M. K., Spivey-Knowlton, M., Eberhard, K., & Sedivy, J. C. (1995). Integration of visual and linguistic information is spoken-language comprehension. Science, 268, 16321634.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thierry, G., & Wu, Y. J. (2007). Brain potentials reveal unconscious translation during foreign-language comprehension. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 1253012535.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomas, M. S. C., & Allport, A. (2000). Language switching costs in bilingual visual word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 43, 4466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Titone, D., Mercier, J., Sudarshan, A., Pivneva, I., & Baum, S. (2014). Spoken word processing in bilingual older adults. Ms., McGill University.Google Scholar
Weber, A., & Cutler, A. (2004). Lexical competition in non-native spoken-word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wu, Y. J., & Thierry, G. (2010). Investigating bilingual processing: The neglected role of language processing contexts. Frontiers in Psychology, doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00178. Published online by Frontiers, November 1, 2010.Google Scholar
Yee, E., & Sedivy, J. C. (2006). Eye movements to pictures reveal transient semantic activation during spoken word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 114.Google ScholarPubMed
Supplementary material: PDF

Mercier supplementary material

Tables S1-S4

Download Mercier supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 457.4 KB