Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-28T10:19:09.306Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Language and Aging

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 November 2008

Extract

In the late 1970s Gillian Cohen surveyed research on the effects of aging on language and concluded that “geriatric psycholinguistics is virtually an unexplored territory” (1979:412). In the decade since she reached this conclusion, psycholinguists, sociolinguists, speech-language pathologists, and cognitive scientists have begun to explore this territory and to map a wide range of age-related changes to language.

Type
Language in Society
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1989

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

UNANNOTATED BIBLIOGRAPHY

Baddeley, A. D. 1986. Working memory. Oxford: Claredon Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Bayles, K. A. et al. , 1989. Differentiating Alzheimer's patients from the normal elderly and stroke patients with aphasia. Journal of speech and hearing disorders. 54.1.7487.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blanken, G. et al. , 1987. Spontaneous speech in senile dementia and aphasia: Implications for a neurolinguistic model of language production. Cognition. 27.3.247274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boden, D. and Bielby, D. D.. 1983. The past as resource. Human development. 26.6.308319.Google Scholar
Borod, J. C., Goodglass, H., and Kaplan, E.. 1980. Normative data on the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination, Parietal Lobe Battery, and the Boston Naming Test. Journal of clinical neuropsychology. 2. 209215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowers, G. H. (ed.) In Press. The psychology of learning and motivation. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bowles, N. L. and Poon, L. W.. 1985. Aging and the retrieval of words in semantic memory. Journal of gerontology. 40.1.7177.Google Scholar
Bowles, N. L., Obler, L. K., and Albert, M. L.. 1987. Naming errors in healthy aging and dementia of the Alzheimer type. Cortex. 23.3.519524CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caporael, L. 1981. The paralanguage of caregiving: Baby talk to the institutionalized aged. Journal of personality and social psychology. 40.5.876884.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caporael, L., Lucaszewski, M. P., and Culbertson, G. H.. 1983. Secondary babytalk: Judgments of institutionalized elderly and their caregivers. Journal of personality and social psychology. 44.4.746754.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, G. 1981. Inferential reasoning in old age. Cognition. 9.5972.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coupland, N. and Coupland, J.. In press. Language and later life: The diachrony and decrement predicament. In Giles, H. and Robinson, P. (eds.) Handbook of language and social psychology. London: Wiley.Google Scholar
Coupland, N., Grainger, K., and Coupland, J.. 1988. Politeness in context: Intergenerational issues. Language in society. 17.2.253262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coupland, N. et al. In press. Accommodating troubles talk: The management of elderly self-disclosure. In McGregor, G. and White, R. S. (eds.) Reception and response: Hearer creativity and the analysis of spoken texts. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Emery, O. B. 1988. Pseudodementia: A theoretical and empirical discussion. Cleveland, OH: The Western Reserve Geriatric Education Center.Google Scholar
Feier, C. D. and Gerstman, L. J.. 1980. Sentence comprehension abilities throughout the adult life span. Journal of gerontology. 35.5.727728.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Franklyn-Stokes, A. et al. , 1988. Information seeking across the life span. Journal of social psychology. 128.3.419421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giles, H. and Robinson, P. (eds.) In Press. Handbook of language and social psychology. London: Wiley.Google Scholar
Giles, H., Coupland, N., and Wieman, J. (eds.) In Press. Communication, health, and the elderly: Proceedings of the Fulbright colloquium. Manchester, England: University of Manchester Press.Google Scholar
Gleason, J. et al. , 1980. Narrative strategies of aphasic and normal-speaking subjects. Journal of speech and hearing research. 23.2.370382.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gruneberg, M. M., Morris, P., and Sykes, R. N. (eds.) 1988. Practical aspects of memory: Current research and issues. 2 Vols. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Howe, M. L. and Brainerd, C. J. (eds.) 1988. Cognitive development in adulthood. New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kemper, S. 1987a. Life-span changes in syntactic complexity. Journal of gerontology. 42.323328.Google Scholar
Kemper, S. 1988. Geriatric psycholinguistics: Syntactic limitations of oral and written language. In Light, L. and Burke, D. (eds.) Language, memory and aging. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 5876.Google Scholar
Light, L. and Anderson, P. A.. 1985. Working memory capacity, age and memory for discourse. Journal of gerontology. 40.6.737747.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Light, L. and Capps, J. L.. 1986. Comprehension of pronouns in younger and older adults. Developmental psychology. 22.4.580585.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Light, L. and Burke, D. (eds.) 1988. Language, memory, and aging. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGregor, G. and White, R. S. (eds.) In Press. Reception and response: Hearer creativity and the analysis of spoken texts. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Nicholas, M. et al. 1985. Lexical retrieval in healthy aging. Cortex. 21.595606.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Obler, L. 1980. Narrative discourse style in the elderly. In Obler, L. and Albert, M. L. (eds.) Language and communication in the elderly. Lexington, MA: Heath and Company. 7590.Google Scholar
Obler, L. and Albert, M. L. (eds.) Language and communication in the elderly. Lexington, MA: Heath and Company.Google Scholar
Stewart, M. A. and Ryan, E. B.. 1982. Attitudes toward younger and older adult speakers: Effects of varying speech rates. Journal of language and social psychology. 1.2.91109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ulatowska, H. K. (ed.) 1985. The aging brain: Communication in the elderly. San Diego, CA: College-Hill.Google Scholar
Ulatowska, H. K. et al. 1986. Disruption of reference in aging. Brain and language. 28.2441.Google Scholar
Wingfield, A. and Stine, E. L.. 1986. Organizational strategies in immediate recall of rapid speech by young and elderly adults. Experimental aging research. 12.1.7983.Google Scholar
Wingfield, A. et al. , 1985. Speed of processing in normal aging: Effects of speech rate, linguistic structure, and processing time. Journal of gerontology. 40.5.579585.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zelinski, E. 1988. Integrating information from discourse: Do older adults show deficits? In Light, L. and Burke, D. (eds.) Language, memory, and aging. Cambridge: Cambrige University Press. 117132.Google Scholar