Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gbm5v Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T02:01:18.919Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Attentional Shifts Alter Pain Perception in the Chicken

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 January 2023

M J Gentle*
Affiliation:
Roslin Institute (Edinburgh), Roslin, Midlothian EH25 9PS, UK
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.

In humans, psychological manipulations such as hypnosis, behavioural modifications, relaxation training and cognitive behaviour therapy have all been used to reduce pain intensity. One thing these treatments have in common is selective attention. Work on attention-based cognitive coping strategies has shown that they have potentially useful analgesic qualities in pain therapy. In animals, there have been few studies on the effects of attentional shifts on pain perception. There is extensive literature on stress-induced analgesia and it is likely that, in some of the experiments, attention could be an important variable. This paper will present some of our recent work on selective attention and pain perception using the sodium urate model of gouty arthritis. Birds are naturally prone to articular gout and the model we have developed mimics acute gouty attacks in a single joint. Experimental sodium urate arthritis produces a tonically painful inflammation lasting for at least 3h during which time the animals show pain-related behaviours. Changes in motivation can reduce these pain-related behaviours and it has been hypothesized that these motivational changes act by way of altering the attention of the animal away from pain. The motivational changes investigated included nesting, feeding, exploration and social interactions. The degree of pain suppression ranged from marked hypoalgesia to complete analgesia and as such demonstrates a remarkable ability to suppress tonic pain. These shifts in attention not only reduced pain but also significantly reduced peripheral inflammation. These results are discussed in terms of the limited capacity models of attention.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2001 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

References

Brune, K, Bucher, K and Walz, D 1974 The avian microcrystal arthritis I. Simultaneous recording of nociception and temperature effect in the inflamed joint. Agents Actions 4: 2126CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bushnell, M C, Duncan, G H, Dubner, R and He, L F 1984 Activity of trigeminothalamic neurons in the medullary dorsal horn of awake monkeys trained in a thermal discrimination task. Journal of Neurophysiology 52: 170187CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cioffi, D 1991 Beyond attentional strategies: a cognitive-perceptual model of somatic interpretation. Psychological Bulletin 109: 2541CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coderre, T J and Wall, P D 1988 Ankle joint urate arthritis in rats provides a useful tool for the evaluation of analgesic and anti-arthritic agents. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behaviour 29: 461466CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Devine, D P and Spanos, N P 1990 Effectiveness of maximally different cognitive strategies and expectancy in attenuation of reported pain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 58: 472678CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duff, SRI 1985 Dyschondroplasia/osteochondrosis of the femoral trochanter in the fowl. Journal of Comparative Pathology 95: 363371CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duff, SRI and Hocking, P M 1986 Chronic orthopaedic disease in adult male broiler breeding fowls. Research in Veterinary Science 41: 340348CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duff, SRI and Thorp, B H 1985 Abnormal angulation/torsion of the pelvic appendicular skeleton in broiler fowl: morphological and radiological findings. Research in Veterinary Science 39: 313319CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eccleston, C 1995a The attentional control of pain: methodological and theoretical concerns. Pain 63: 310CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eccleston, C 1995b Chronic pain and distraction: an experimental investigation into the role of sustained and shifting attention in the processing of chronic persistent pain. Behaviour Research Therapy 33: 391405CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernandez, E and Turk, D C 1989 The utility of cognitive coping strategies for altering pain perception: a meta-analysis. Pain 38: 123135CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Floersheim, G L, Brune, K and Seiler, K 1973 Colchicine in avian sodium urate and calcium pyrophosphate microcrystal arthritis. Agents Actions 3: 2023CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, W and Watts, J W 1950 Psychosurgery in the Treatment of Mental Disorders and Intractable Pain. Thomas: Springfield, USAGoogle Scholar
Gentle, M J 1997 Sodium urate arthritis: effects on the sensory properties of articular afferents in the chicken. Pain 70: 245251CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gentle, M J and Corr, S A 1995 Endogenous analgesia in the chicken. Neuroscience Letters 201: 211215CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gentle, M J and Hunter, L N 1993 Neurogenic inflammation in the chicken (Gallus gallus var domesticus). Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology 105C: 459462Google Scholar
Gentle, M J and Tilston, V L 1998 Reduction in peripheral inflammation by changes in attention. Physiology and Behaviour 66: 289292CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gentle, M J, Hunter, L N and Corr, S A 1997 Effects of caudolateral neostriatal ablations on pain-related behaviour in the chicken. Physiology and Behaviour 61: 493498CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hardy, J D, Wolff, H G and Goodell, H 1967 Pain Sensations and Reactions. Hafner Publishing Company: New York, USAGoogle Scholar
Hashish, I, Hai, H K, Harvey, W, Feinmann, C and Harris, M 1988 Reduction of postoperative pain and swelling by ultrasound treatment: a placebo effect. Pain 33: 303311CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayes, R L, Dubner, R and Hoffman, D S 1981 Neuronal activity in medullary dorsal horn in awake monkeys trained in a thermal discrimination task. II. Behavioural modulation of response to thermal and mechanical stimuli. Journal of Neurophysiology 46: 428443CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hocking, P M, Gentle, M J, Bernard, R and Dunn, L 1997 Evaluation of a protocol for determining the effectiveness of analgesics for reducing pain in domestic fowl and an assessment of the efficacy of bupivacaine anaesthesia. Research in Veterinary Science 63: 263267CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoffman, H G, Doctor, J N, Patterson, D R, Carrougher, G J and Furness, T A III 2000 Virtual reality as an adjunctive pain control during burn wound care in adolescent patients. Pain 85: 305309CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jaynes, J 1985 Sensory pain and conscious pain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8: 6163CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jessup, B A and Gallegos, X 1994 Relaxation and biofeedback. In: Wall, P D and Melzack, R (eds) Textbook of Pain, 3rd edition pp 13211336. Churchill Livingstone: Edinburgh, UKGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D 1973 Attention and Effort. Prentice Hall: Englewood Cliffs, USAGoogle Scholar
Keefe, F J and Lefebvre, J C 1994 Behaviour therapy. In: Wall, P D and Melzack, R (eds) Textbook of Pain, 3rd edition pp 13671380. Churchill Livingstone: Edinburgh, UKGoogle Scholar
Levine, J and Taiwo, Y 1994 Inflammatory pain. In: Wall, P D and Melzack, R (eds) Textbook of Pain, 3rd edition pp 4556. Churchill Livingstone: Edinburgh, UKGoogle Scholar
Logan, G D 1985 Executive control of thought and action. Acta Psychologia 60: 193210CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCaiil, K D and Haugtvedt, C 1982 Attention, distraction and cold pressor pain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 43: 154162CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCaul, K D and Malott, J M 1984 Distraction and coping with pain. Psychological Bulletin 95: 516533CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGivern, R F and Berntson, G G 1980 Mediation of diurnal fluctuations in pain sensitivity in the rat food intake patterns: reversal by naloxone. Science 210: 210211CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mills, A D 1983 Genetic Analysis of Strain Differences in Pre-laying Behaviour in the Fowl. PhD Thesis, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UKGoogle Scholar
Miron, D, Duncan, G H and Bushnell, M C 1989 Effects of attention on the intensity and unpleasantness of thermal pain. Pain 39: 345352CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mogensen, J and Divac, I 1982 The prefrontal ‘cortex’. Behavioural evidence. Brain Behaviour and Evolution 21: 6066CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mogensen, J and Divac, I 1993 Behavioural effects of ablation of the pigeon-equivalent of the mammalian prefrontal cortex. Behavioural Brain Research 55: 101107CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morton, D B and Griffiths, P H M 1985 Guidelines on the recognition of pain, distress and discomfort in experimental animals and an hypothesis for assessment. Veterinary Record 116: 431436CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Okuda, K, Nakahama, H, Miyakawa, H and Shima, K 1984 Arthritis induced in cat by sodium urate: a possible model for tonic pain. Pain 18: 287297CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petrovic, P, Petersson, K M, Ghatan, P H, Stone-Elander, S and Ingvar, M 2000 Pain-related cerebral activation is altered by a distracting cognitive task. Pain 85: 1930CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Posner, M I 1995 Attention in cognitive neuroscience: an overview. In: Gazzaniga, M S (ed) The Cognitive Neurosciences pp 615624. The MIT Press: Cambridge, USAGoogle Scholar
Rees, H, Sluka, K A, Westlund, K N and Willis, W D 1994 Do dorsal root reflexes augment peripheral inflammation? NeuroReport 5: 821824CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riddell, C 1991 Developmental, metabolic and miscellaneous disorders. In: Calnek B W, Barnes H J, Beard C W, Reid W M and Yoder H W Jr (eds) Diseases of Poultry, 9th edition pp 827862. Iowa State University Press: Ames, USAGoogle Scholar
Sluka, K A, Willis, W D and Westlund, K N 1995 The role of dorsal root reflexes in neurogenic inflammation. Pain Forum 4: 141149CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spanos, N P 1989 Experimental research in hypnotic analgesia. In: Spanos, N P and Chaves, J F (eds) Hypnosis: the Cognitive-behavioural Perspective pp 206240. Prometheus: New York, USAGoogle Scholar
Torebjörk, H E, Schmelz, M and Handwerker, H O 1996 Functional properties of human cutaneous nociceptors and their role in pain and hyperalgesia. In: Belmonte, C and Cevero, F (eds) Neurobiology of Nociceptors pp 349369. Oxford University Press: Oxford, UKGoogle Scholar
Turk, D C, Meichenbaum, D and Genest, M 1983 Pain and Behavioural Medicine: a Cognitive-behavioural Perspective. Guilford Press: New York, USAGoogle Scholar
Waldmann, C and Güntürkün, O 1993 The dopaminergic innervation of the pigeon caudolateral forebrain: immunocytochemical evidence for a ‘prefrontal cortex’ in birds? Brain Research 600: 225234CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wall, P D 1994 The placebo and placebo response. In: Wall, P D and Melzack, R (eds) Textbook of Pain, 3rd edition pp 12971308. Churchill Livingstone: Edinburgh, UKGoogle Scholar
White, J C and Sweet, W H 1955 Pain, its Mechanisms and Neurosurgical Control. Thomas: Springfield, USAGoogle Scholar
White, J C and Sweet, W H 1969 Pain and the Neurosurgeon. Thomas: Springfield, USAGoogle Scholar
Wylie, L M and Gentle, M J 1998 Feeding-induced tonic pain suppression in the chicken: reversal by naloxone. Physiology and Behaviour 64: 2730CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed